Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Learning & Behavior 2/2016

05.11.2015

A derived transformation of emotional functions using self-reports, implicit association tests, and frontal alpha asymmetries

Erschienen in: Learning & Behavior | Ausgabe 2/2016

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Research on the derived transformation of stimulus functions (ToF) typically employs single dependent measures for assessing the stimulus functions after derived relations have been established. For the first time, we examined ToF using three dependent measures both prior to and after relational training and testing. Specifically, we employed self-reports, implicit association tests, and frontal alpha asymmetry as pre versus post measures for assessing ToF. First, we trained two abstract shapes as contextual cues for happier-than and unhappier-than relations, respectively. Next, four conditional discriminations (A+/B–, B+/C–, C+/D–, and D+/E–) were trained in the presence of the happier-than cue only, where A, B, C, D, and E were blurred faces. This was followed by tests for contextually controlled transitive inference (TI) in the presence of both the happier-than and unhappier-than cues. For the participants who demonstrated TI, performance across all three measures following relational training and testing indicated that the “happiness” functions of the A/B stimuli were greater than those of the D/E stimuli. This constitutes the first known demonstration of emotional ToF along explicit, implicit, and neurophysiological measures concurrently.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
Research by Kawahara and Shinohara (2012) suggested that “angular” shapes may be more emotionally loaded than “rounded” ones, in that the former appear to be associated with emotional attributes related to arousal rather than to valence per se (e.g., “shocked” rather than “sad”). In the present study, however, even if the more “angular” Cue 2 was emotionally “loaded” a priori, note that the conditional discriminations involving the A–B, B–C, C–D, and D–E stimulus pairs were reinforced in the presence of Cue 1 only.
 
2
Identifying appropriate reference sites in alpha asymmetry research has been a contentious issue (Hagemann, 2004; Hagemann, Naumann, & Thayer, 2001), particularly if FAA is computed as the difference in separately derived power values of individual electrodes referenced to the vertex (Hagemann et al., 2001, p. 848). Although the details of this matter are beyond the scope of the present article, it suffices to say that Cz has been the most commonly used reference in the FAA research (Coan & Allen, 2003) and continues to be a popular reference of choice (cf. Huang et al., 2014). Given that numerous studies have successfully predicted relationships using the Cz reference, it may be the case that published results utilizing Cz may not only reflect an FAA effect in relation to the construct under investigation, but also sources of variance unique to Cz (Allen, Coan, & Nazarian, 2004). Given that the specific topography of the EEG response was not the variable of interest in the present study, the Cz reference was employed so that the present results may be compared with the wider literature on induced FAAs.
 
3
Although individual alpha bands are typically calculated over posterior sites in order to minimize ocular “noise” (which can resemble alpha activity—see Hagemann, 2004, pp. 159–162), ancillary analyses of the aforementioned participants’ data sets revealed no notable differences between frontal and posterior alpha peaks (SDs < 1 Hz).
 
4
According to the expression \( \left(\begin{array}{c}\hfill n\hfill \\ {}\hfill k\hfill \end{array}\right)P \) k q n–k for binomial probability, where k is the number of predictions met, P is the chance probability of meeting a prediction (P = .5), n is the number of predictions made, and q is the probability of failure in one trial (1 – p).
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Allen, J., Coan, J., & Nazarian, M. (2004). Issues and assumptions on the road from raw signals to metrics of frontal EEG asymmetry in emotion. Biological Psychology, 67, 183–218.CrossRefPubMed Allen, J., Coan, J., & Nazarian, M. (2004). Issues and assumptions on the road from raw signals to metrics of frontal EEG asymmetry in emotion. Biological Psychology, 67, 183–218.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Amd, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Ivanoff, J. (2013). A derived transfer of eliciting emotional functions using differences among electroencephalograms as a dependent measure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99, 318–334. doi:10.1002/jeab.19 CrossRefPubMed Amd, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Ivanoff, J. (2013). A derived transfer of eliciting emotional functions using differences among electroencephalograms as a dependent measure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99, 318–334. doi:10.​1002/​jeab.​19 CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Barnes, D., & Roche, B. (1996). Stimulus equivalence and relational frame theory are fundamentally different: A reply to Saunders’ commentary. Psychological Record, 46, 489–508. Barnes, D., & Roche, B. (1996). Stimulus equivalence and relational frame theory are fundamentally different: A reply to Saunders’ commentary. Psychological Record, 46, 489–508.
Zurück zum Zitat Briesemeister, B., Tamm, S., Heine, A., & Jacobs, A. (2013). Approach the good, withdraw from the bad—A review on frontal alpha asymmetry measures in applied psychological research. Psychology, 4, 261–267. doi:10.4236/psych.2013.43A039 CrossRef Briesemeister, B., Tamm, S., Heine, A., & Jacobs, A. (2013). Approach the good, withdraw from the bad—A review on frontal alpha asymmetry measures in applied psychological research. Psychology, 4, 261–267. doi:10.​4236/​psych.​2013.​43A039 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (2003). Frontal EEG asymmetry and behavioral activation and inhibition systems. Psychophysiology, 40, 106–114.CrossRefPubMed Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (2003). Frontal EEG asymmetry and behavioral activation and inhibition systems. Psychophysiology, 40, 106–114.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2000). Understanding complex behavior: The transformation of stimulus functions. The Behavior Analyst/MABA, 23, 239–254.PubMedPubMedCentral Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2000). Understanding complex behavior: The transformation of stimulus functions. The Behavior Analyst/MABA, 23, 239–254.PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.CrossRefPubMed Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hagemann, D., Naumann, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2001). The quest for the EEG reference revisited: A glance from brain asymmetry research. Psychophysiology, 38, 847–857.CrossRefPubMed Hagemann, D., Naumann, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2001). The quest for the EEG reference revisited: A glance from brain asymmetry research. Psychophysiology, 38, 847–857.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press. Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Hinojosa, J. A., Carretié, L., Valcárcel, M. A., Méndez-Bértolo, C., & Pozo, M. A. (2009). Electrophysiological differences in the processing of affective information in words and pictures. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 173–189. doi:10.3758/CABN.9.2.173 CrossRef Hinojosa, J. A., Carretié, L., Valcárcel, M. A., Méndez-Bértolo, C., & Pozo, M. A. (2009). Electrophysiological differences in the processing of affective information in words and pictures. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 173–189. doi:10.​3758/​CABN.​9.​2.​173 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huang, Y., Zhou, R., Cui, H., Wu, M., Wang, Q., Zhao, Y., & Liu, Y. (2014). Variations in resting frontal alpha asymmetry between high‐and low‐neuroticism females across the menstrual cycle. Psychophysiology, 52, 182–191. doi:10.1111/psyp.12301 CrossRefPubMed Huang, Y., Zhou, R., Cui, H., Wu, M., Wang, Q., Zhao, Y., & Liu, Y. (2014). Variations in resting frontal alpha asymmetry between high‐and low‐neuroticism females across the menstrual cycle. Psychophysiology, 52, 182–191. doi:10.​1111/​psyp.​12301 CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kawahara, S., & Shinohara, K. (2012). A tripartite trans-modal relationship among sounds, shapes and emotions: A case of abrupt modulation. In N. Miyake, D. Peebles, & R. P. Cooper (Eds.), Building bridges across cognitive sciences around the world: Proceedings of the 34th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 569–574). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. Kawahara, S., & Shinohara, K. (2012). A tripartite trans-modal relationship among sounds, shapes and emotions: A case of abrupt modulation. In N. Miyake, D. Peebles, & R. P. Cooper (Eds.), Building bridges across cognitive sciences around the world: Proceedings of the 34th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 569–574). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Zurück zum Zitat Langner, O., Dotsch, R., Bijlstra, G., Wigboldus, D. H. J., Hawk, S. T., & van Knippenberg, A. (2010). Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Animal Cognition and Emotion, 24, 1377–1388. doi:10.1080/02699930903485076 CrossRef Langner, O., Dotsch, R., Bijlstra, G., Wigboldus, D. H. J., Hawk, S. T., & van Knippenberg, A. (2010). Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Animal Cognition and Emotion, 24, 1377–1388. doi:10.​1080/​0269993090348507​6 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Light, G. A., Williams, L. E., Minow, F., Sprock, J., Rissling, A., Sharp, R., . . . Braff, D. L. (2010). Electroencephalography (EEG) and event-related potentials (ERPs) with human participants. In J. N. Crawley et al. (Ed.), Current protocols in neuroscience (Unit 6.25, pp. 1–24). New York, NY: Wiley. doi:10.1002/0471142301.ns0625s52 Light, G. A., Williams, L. E., Minow, F., Sprock, J., Rissling, A., Sharp, R., . . . Braff, D. L. (2010). Electroencephalography (EEG) and event-related potentials (ERPs) with human participants. In J. N. Crawley et al. (Ed.), Current protocols in neuroscience (Unit 6.25, pp. 1–24). New York, NY: Wiley. doi:10.​1002/​0471142301.​ns0625s52
Zurück zum Zitat Nedelcu, R., Fields, L., & Arntzen, E. (2015). Arbitrary conditional discriminative functions of meaningful stimuli and enhanced equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 103, 349–360.CrossRefPubMed Nedelcu, R., Fields, L., & Arntzen, E. (2015). Arbitrary conditional discriminative functions of meaningful stimuli and enhanced equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 103, 349–360.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat O’Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Stewart, I. (2014). Antecedent and consequential control of derived instruction following. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 102, 66–85.CrossRefPubMed O’Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Stewart, I. (2014). Antecedent and consequential control of derived instruction following. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 102, 66–85.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat O’Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smyth, S. (2007). A derived transfer of functions and the implicit association test. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88, 263–283.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral O’Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smyth, S. (2007). A derived transfer of functions and the implicit association test. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88, 263–283.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Patel, S. H., & Azzam, P. N. (2005). Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected studies of the Event-Related Potential. International Journal of Medical Sciences, 2, 147–154.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Patel, S. H., & Azzam, P. N. (2005). Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected studies of the Event-Related Potential. International Journal of Medical Sciences, 2, 147–154.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Scherg, M., & Berg, P. (1991). Use of prior knowledge in brain electromagnetic source analysis. Brain Topography, 4, 143–150.CrossRefPubMed Scherg, M., & Berg, P. (1991). Use of prior knowledge in brain electromagnetic source analysis. Brain Topography, 4, 143–150.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2012). E-Prime user’s guide, version 2.0. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2012). E-Prime user’s guide, version 2.0. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.
Zurück zum Zitat Steirn, J. N., Weaver, J. E., & Zentall, T. R. (1995). Transitive inference in pigeons: Simplified procedures and a test of value transfer theory. Animal Learning & Behavior, 23, 76–82. doi:10.3758/BF03198018 CrossRef Steirn, J. N., Weaver, J. E., & Zentall, T. R. (1995). Transitive inference in pigeons: Simplified procedures and a test of value transfer theory. Animal Learning & Behavior, 23, 76–82. doi:10.​3758/​BF03198018 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Von Fersen, L., Wynne, C. D., Delius, J. D., & Staddon, J. E. (1991). Transitive inference formation in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 334–341. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.17.3.334 Von Fersen, L., Wynne, C. D., Delius, J. D., & Staddon, J. E. (1991). Transitive inference formation in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 334–341. doi:10.​1037/​0097-7403.​17.​3.​334
Zurück zum Zitat Weaver, J. E., Steirn, J. N., & Zentall, T. R. (1997). Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 113–117. doi:10.3758/BF03210782 CrossRef Weaver, J. E., Steirn, J. N., & Zentall, T. R. (1997). Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 113–117. doi:10.​3758/​BF03210782 CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
A derived transformation of emotional functions using self-reports, implicit association tests, and frontal alpha asymmetries
Publikationsdatum
05.11.2015
Erschienen in
Learning & Behavior / Ausgabe 2/2016
Print ISSN: 1543-4494
Elektronische ISSN: 1543-4508
DOI
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0198-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2016

Learning & Behavior 2/2016 Zur Ausgabe