Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 2/2016

Open Access 30.12.2015 | Original Paper

Be Careful Where You Smile: Culture Shapes Judgments of Intelligence and Honesty of Smiling Individuals

verfasst von: Kuba Krys, C. -Melanie Vauclair, Colin A. Capaldi, Vivian Miu-Chi Lun, Michael Harris Bond, Alejandra Domínguez-Espinosa, Claudio Torres, Ottmar V. Lipp, L. Sam S. Manickam, Cai Xing, Radka Antalíková, Vassilis Pavlopoulos, Julien Teyssier, Taekyun Hur, Karolina Hansen, Piotr Szarota, Ramadan A. Ahmed, Eleonora Burtceva, Ana Chkhaidze, Enila Cenko, Patrick Denoux, Márta Fülöp, Arif Hassan, David O. Igbokwe, İdil Işık, Gwatirera Javangwe, María Malbran, Fridanna Maricchiolo, Hera Mikarsa, Lynden K. Miles, Martin Nader, Joonha Park, Muhammad Rizwan, Radwa Salem, Beate Schwarz, Irfana Shah, Chien-Ru Sun, Wijnand van Tilburg, Wolfgang Wagner, Ryan Wise, Angela Arriola Yu

Erschienen in: Journal of Nonverbal Behavior | Ausgabe 2/2016

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Smiling individuals are usually perceived more favorably than non-smiling ones—they are judged as happier, more attractive, competent, and friendly. These seemingly clear and obvious consequences of smiling are assumed to be culturally universal, however most of the psychological research is carried out in WEIRD societies (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) and the influence of culture on social perception of nonverbal behavior is still understudied. Here we show that a smiling individual may be judged as less intelligent than the same non-smiling individual in cultures low on the GLOBE’s uncertainty avoidance dimension. Furthermore, we show that corruption at the societal level may undermine the prosocial perception of smiling—in societies with high corruption indicators, trust toward smiling individuals is reduced. This research fosters understanding of the cultural framework surrounding nonverbal communication processes and reveals that in some cultures smiling may lead to negative attributions.

Introduction

It is commonly recognized that it is good to smile—Louis Armstrong sang that when you smile the world smiles with you, and various trainers and guidebooks advise smiling because it improves interpersonal communication. These lay beliefs are supported by numerous studies demonstrating that smiling individuals are perceived as happier (Otta et al. 1994), more attractive, communal, competent (Hess et al. 2002; Matsumoto and Kudoh 1993), likable (Palmer and Simmons 1995), approachable, and friendly, and that a smile from another promises a safe and satisfying interaction (Miles 2009).
Cultures may shape different scripts for social behavior and as a consequence, different logics of nonverbal behavior and its social perception (Matsumoto 2006; Leung and Cohen 2011). In the past few decades, increasingly more psychological research has been carried out in non-WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic; Henrich et al. 2010) societies indicating difficulties to replicate results from psychological experiments across cultures (Smith et al. 2013). Although psychologists broadly recognize the interrelationship between culture and behavior, and the sub-discipline called cross-cultural psychology is flourishing, interactions between culture and social perception of nonverbal behavior still remain understudied. One example of this lack of cross-cultural study is the assessment of cultural variation of traits attributed to smiling individuals that goes beyond East–West cultural comparisons (Hess et al. 2000). Rychlowska and collaborators (2015) were among the first to address this pointing to the importance of heterogeneity versus homogeneity of cultures in predicting the endorsement of smiling.
Although numerous studies confirm that positive perceptions of smiling individuals seem to be universal, anecdotal evidence suggests that in some cultures the opposite may be true. For example, a well-known Russian proverb says ‘Улыбкa, бeз пpичины - пpизнaк дypaчины’ (smiling with no reason is a sign of stupidity). The Norwegian government humorously explains nuances of Norwegian culture by indicating that when a stranger on the street smiles at Norwegians, they may assume that the stranger is insane (EURES 2010). British authors of a popular guidebook about Poland warn tourists that smiling at strangers is perceived by Poles as a sign of stupidity (Bedford et al. 2008). Even Darwin (1872/1998) wrote about “the large class of idiots who are … constantly smiling” (p. 199).
Previous studies have tested this counterintuitive phenomenon in different countries (Krys et al. 2014, 2015). However, these studies included only a small number of cultures (seven) compared to the much broader cross-cultural experiment reported here, which was conducted in 44 cultures. Cross-cultural comparisons involving that many different cultures allow for multilevel and country-level analyses, and are necessary to reliably identify cultural factors that are related to the differential social perception of the most often encountered facial expression, viz., the smile.

Meanings Attributed to Smiles

Smiles are highly diverse in their types and in their possible meanings. They are used to communicate a range of different psychological signals, including positive emotions, social intentions, or a person’s social status (Matsumoto and Willingham 2009). Past research has offered a number of distinctions among smiles. The utility of one of the most popular distinctions, viz. Duchenne versus non-Duchenne smiles (Duchenne 1862), has been recently questioned because there is evidence that the use of the Duchenne marker of a ‘true’ smile is not universal, but rather limited to certain cultures (Abe et al. 2002; Thibault et al. 2012). In their simulation of smiles model, Niedenthal et nl. (2010) focus on the perception of smiles and suggest that the distinction between Duchenne and non-Duchenne smiles may be largely superseded by a distinction based on the functions of smiles, which may be derived from (and mapped onto) identifiable brain systems that represent different meanings of smiling.
Niedenthal et al. (2010) describe three types of smiles that have important and discrete functions, namely, enjoyment, affiliative, and dominance smiles. Humans (and some other primates) smile spontaneously during experiences of pleasure or success (Ekman 2009) and this expression is called the enjoyment smile. Affiliative smiles are those that signal positive social intentions and are essential for the creation and maintenance of social bonds; personal enjoyment does not have to accompany affiliative smiles. The third group of smiles—dominance smiles—reflect social status or control, and may include scheming smiles, critical smiles, and proud smiles which have different physical attributes than affiliative and enjoyment smiles (Niedenthal et al. 2010). Chang and Vermeulen (2010) claim that affiliative and enjoyment smiles cannot be discriminated from each other on the basis of physical markers—their meaning may be derived only from contextual information. Rychlowska and collaborators (2015) delivered evidence that the above distinctions may be cross-culturally identified, and documented that heterogeneity and homogeneity of cultures (i.e., the extent to which a country’s present-day population descended from migration from numerous vs. few source countries over a long period of time) may predict the endorsement of affiliative and dominance smiles, respectively.
The present research focuses on the attributions given to affiliative and enjoyment smiles presented in still photographs in order to uncover the cultural variation of meanings attributed to the most commonly expressed smiles. Limiting the scope of the current research in this way avoids the problems related to differences in cultural scripts that may influence the attributions to dominance smiles. We examined perceptions of honesty and intelligence attributed to smiling individuals because these traits reflect the big two of social perception (Abele and Wojciszke 2013; Bakan 1966). Among academic psychologists, there seems to be a consensus about two fundamental dimensions of social judgments, though these basic dimensions are named differently and have slightly different meanings. For example, Abele and Wojciszke (2014) call them agency and communion, whereas Fiske et al. (2006) use the labels warmth and competence. These dimensions reflect the logic of evolutionary pressure and help us determine whether ‘others’ are friend or foe (communion/warmth/honesty) and whether ‘others’ have the ability to enact their friendly or hostile intentions (agency/competence/intelligence).

Cultural Predictors of Smile Perception

Descriptive accounts of general cultural differences have been available for a long time, but empirical assessments of cross-cultural variability have only emerged relatively recently (Hofstede 2001; House et al. 2004; Leung and Bond 2004). In our research we tested two predictions related to cultural variation. First, we tested the relation between cultural uncertainty avoidance (UA; House et al. 2004) and the social perception of smiling versus non-smiling individuals with regards to intelligence. Societies that rank high on UA socialize their members to alleviate the unpredictability of future events, whereas in societies that rank low on UA, the future is judged to be relatively unpredictable and there are fewer societal guidelines on how to behave (House et al. 2004). As argued elsewhere (Krys et al. 2014), in cultures low on UA, social conditions are regarded as uncertain, so expressing certainty through smiling (Hareli and Hess 2010) can be perceived as inconsistent behavior and people who exhibit inconsistency may be evaluated as unintelligent (Weisbuch et al. 2010).
The second hypothesis tests whether ‘corruption corrupts smiling’. We predicted that the more corrupt a society is, the less trust should be granted to a smile. On the one hand, a smile is the most common signal of positive intentions. In fact, a smile conveys a message that even a newborn baby understands and infants start smiling as early as 3 months old (Wörmann et al. 2014). The smile is perhaps the most commonly observed affiliative signal (Méhu and Dunbar 2008). A smile facilitates the establishment and maintenance of social bonds, and helps to coordinate social interactions (Fridlund 2002). All the above suggest that smiling evolved as a universal signal of honesty and functions as a social glue (Centorrino et al. 2015). On the other hand, this social glue may be counterfeited without difficulty because smiling is a signal that can be easily produced (Méhu 2011). In particular circumstances, some smiles may be expressed to benefit the signaller and may be deceptive (Ekman and Friesen 1982).
Therefore, we predicted that the ease of producing a smile may in some conditions lead to lower trust in this signal and one of the pre-conditions of scepticism about a smile’s honesty is excessive corruption in society. In highly corrupt societies, individuals are exposed to relatively frequent unfair or untruthful behaviors and, thus, scepticism about the positive intentions underlying a smile may be well-grounded and justified. Hence, in our second hypothesis we offer the novel prediction that the higher the corruption index of a country, the more smiling individuals will be perceived as dishonest. In other words, we empirically tested whether ‘corruption corrupts’ the evolutionary social glue of the smile.
Past research has shown that social judgements of smiling and non-smiling individuals may also be affected by gender-related expectations (Hess et al. 2009). Gender stereotypes and beliefs about emotional expressiveness can lead to different standards when men and women evaluate the nonverbal behavior of other men and women (Krumhuber et al. 2007). Women tend to smile more than men (LaFrance et al. 2003; Hall 1984) and there is a greater expectation for them to do so (Brody and Hall 2008). Therefore, the gender of the assessor and poser were included as control variables in all analyses. The contributions of these control variables will be reported, though a detailed discussion of this contribution is beyond the scope of the current paper.

Method

To provide a systematic analysis of the social perception of smiling individuals, we asked participants in 44 cultures to rate photos of smiling and non-smiling individuals on traits assessing honesty and intelligence.

Participants and Selection of Cultures

Data were gathered from a total of 5216 respondents in 44 cultures across six continents. After removing individuals with at least one missing answer on the measures of intelligence or honesty, the final sample that was analysed consisted of 4519 participants. In Table 1, we present demographic characteristics for all national samples. The gender distribution was 56.5 % female and 43.5 % male. The mean age of participants was 22.36 years (SD 5.50). Participants were students from a variety of different disciplines who were recruited at each author’s university. All data were collected from 2011 to 2015.
Table 1
Samples’ characteristics
 
coll.
anlz.
fem.
fem.
age
age
intell.
hones.
intell.
hones.
 
N
N
N
%
M
SD
α
α
d
d
Albania
119
90
50
56
20.81
2.05
.81
.69
.14
.44
Argentina
104
80
51
64
33.77
10.53
.76
.73
.04
−.03
Australia
120
112
86
77
19.74
3.93
.91
.83
.24
.70
Austria
109
95
60
63
25.57
6.35
.92
.88
.49
.56
Brazil
120
103
68
66
23.89
5.70
.77
.71
.31
.47
Canada
117
86
49
57
20.34
4.05
.92
.88
.20
.56
China
120
111
51
46
23.09
4.10
.84
.85
.54
.53
Colombia
120
113
70
62
27.66
13.46
.81
.75
.05
.69
Denmark
112
106
53
50
23.61
2.90
.91
.81
.31
.51
Egypt
93
61
49
80
20.79
4.45
.85
.76
.37
.53
France
120
102
61
60
28.19
8.77
.90
.86
−.16
.23
Georgia
120
115
58
50
25.30
10.06
.70
.69
.22
.54
Germany
81
78
36
46
22.71
6.02
.84
.76
1.01
.43
Greece
125
120
62
52
20.82
1.59
.85
.76
−.04
.62
Hong Kong
120
112
49
44
20.44
1.77
.68
.76
.01
.26
Hungary
118
105
57
54
21.28
3.79
.86
.78
.00
.41
India Karnataka
120
92
49
53
21.15
2.77
.83
.50
−.03
−.08
India Kerala
120
104
54
52
20.32
1.26
.79
.62
−.41
.03
Indonesia
120
120
60
50
19.58
1.37
.75
.69
.09
.00
Iran
48
42
31
74
21.21
4.08
.79
.70
−.40
.09
Ireland
120
104
46
44
19.35
3.08
.88
.80
.14
.51
Israel
99
83
31
37
26.24
5.15
.87
.84
−.12
.26
Italy
160
151
137
91
23.12
6.14
.82
.79
.01
.56
Japan
109
103
52
51
19.24
1.21
.82
.83
−.41
.47
Kuwait
300
298
132
44
21.46
3.22
.71
.62
.12
.31
Malaysia
120
100
73
73
22.81
4.34
.88
.80
.57
.55
Maldives
120
95
44
46
24.09
2.99
.78
.55
.08
−.01
Mexico
136
105
54
51
21.07
2.34
.91
.82
−.09
.35
Nigeria
120
112
59
53
19.16
1.56
.83
.70
.25
.35
Norway
97
85
50
59
22.22
3.76
.87
.70
.05
.41
Pakistan
190
137
68
50
21.52
3.22
.78
.63
.26
.19
Philippines
120
114
83
73
19.22
2.00
.83
.78
.35
.70
Poland
76
68
48
71
22.69
1.81
.90
.79
−.02
.43
Portugal
120
111
62
56
22.03
3.15
.79
.67
.25
.61
Russia
120
113
71
63
22.33
1.87
.87
.79
−.28
.21
So. Afr. n-white
115
41
24
59
20.93
1.82
.83
.76
−.02
.46
So. Afr. white
115
43
21
49
20.93
1.82
.83
.76
.10
.51
South Korea
120
112
62
55
20.81
2.25
.74
.70
−.36
.52
Switzerland
107
99
59
60
25.04
5.62
.92
.90
.96
.91
Taiwan
68
61
37
61
19.51
.94
.81
.78
.15
.48
Turkey
134
127
64
50
22.57
2.71
.76
.65
.22
.36
UK
120
111
59
53
23.82
8.99
.88
.80
.32
.62
USA
84
79
52
66
24.06
9.81
.94
.90
.15
.50
Zimbabwe
120
120
60
50
22.82
3.70
.66
.55
.16
.02
Average
119
103
58
57
22.44
4.15
.83
.75
.13
.40
Total
5216
4519
2552
56
22.36
5.50
.85
.79
.12
.39
coll. N N collected, anlz. N N analyzed (all further data are presented for N analyzed); fem. N N female, fem. % percentage of female in a sample; intell. α Cronbach’s alpha for intelligence measure, hones. α Cronbach’s alpha for honesty measure, So. Afr. n-white/white South Africa non-white/white samples (for South Africa we follow the GLOBE distinction)
We managed to collect data in forty-two out of the sixty-two cultures involved in the GLOBE project (House et al. 2004). We also collected data in Norway (for which practices on the GLOBE dimensions were calculated by Warner-Søderholm 2010) and in Pakistan (ranked high on the corruption dimensions, which relates to our second hypothesis). We aimed to collect data from at least 120 individuals in each analyzed culture (some authors, however, collected more and other authors collected fewer).

Materials

All participants were asked to provide basic demographic information on their gender, age, student status, religion, and father’s highest degree. Individuals were also asked about their ethnicity and nationality in cultures where the team leaders decided that asking about this information was not controversial. The main part of the questionnaire had participants rate eight faces, four smiling and four non-smiling, that were balanced for gender and represented different ethnicities (four European American, two African American, and two Indian, see Fig. 1; the need for ethnic diversity is stressed by Matsumoto and Kudoh 1993) on a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = trait doesn’t fit at all to 7 = trait fits perfectly) measuring intelligence (i.e., intelligent, dumb, smart, and stupid) and honesty (i.e., honest, false, authentic, and unnatural). Questionnaires in cultures that joined the project later also included five items from Rosenberg’s self-esteem scale (1965), three items from Dunton and Fazio’s motivation to control prejudiced reactions scale (1997), and three additional attributes (i.e., attractiveness, friendliness, and familiarity) using the aforementioned 7-point scale. The current report is of the data provided by all participants. Photographs of the same persons posing neutral and smiling expressions were taken from the Center for Vital Longevity Face Database (Minear and Park 2004). The questionnaire, in the form of a small booklet, started with the following instructions: “Research shows that people can quite accurately evaluate others based on their looks. Can you help us and rate some faces?”. Photographs were organised into two sets, with targets who were smiling in one set presented as non-smiling in the other (Fig. 1). Half of the participants received one set, the other half received the other set. Photographs in each set were randomized. In a pre-test carried out among 183 Polish students, smiling faces, were assessed as more joyful, t(173) = 18.43, p < .001, d = 1.40, and affiliative, t(182) = 9.22, p < .001, d = .68, than non-smiling faces, but did not differ in dominance, t(176) = .25, p = .80, d = .02. Materials were originally written in Polish and English and were translated from English into languages of each country where the study was carried out. Following best practices (Brislin 1970), team leaders in each culture were asked to follow the back-translation procedure to establish linguistic equivalence. The original material, including the manual for collaborating researchers, is available from the first author in English.
For each participant, we calculated the average ratings given to smiling and non-smiling target individuals across the traits associated with the intelligence and honesty dimensions. Next, an effect size (i.e., Cohen’s d) for the differences between ratings for smiling and non-smiling individuals was calculated for each dimension in each culture. Thus, we obtained two measures for each culture: a Cohen’s d for intelligence and a Cohen’s d for honesty.

Results

To test our two predictions, we separately examined cross-cultural differences in ratings given to smiling and non-smiling individuals on the intelligence and honesty dimensions. The results are summarised in Figs. 2 and 3.
As predicted, smiling was not consistently perceived as a signal of intelligence across all cultures. Although smiling did lead to attributions of higher intelligence in 18 out of the 44 cultures, we identified six cultures where individuals were perceived as significantly less intelligent when smiling. Moreover, we found no significant difference in the intelligence ratings of smiling versus non-smiling individuals in 20 cultures.
This result supports our prediction that social perception of the intelligence of smiling individuals varies across cultures, and in some cultures, smiling may even lead to attributions of lower levels of intelligence (we observed a significant two-way culture by smile interaction, F(43, 4425) = 5.9, p < .001, η p 2  = .05; for detailed summary of results of ANOVA analyses, including gender of participant and gender of target, see Table 2). As predicted, the key to understanding this variability was not in geography (e.g., neighboring countries like China and Japan or Germany and France are on different ends of the distributions) nor in economic factors, but in cultural dimensions (for detailed correlation and regression analyses, including economic factors, at the cultural level examine Table 3). The predicted correlation between Cohen’s d (i.e., the difference in rated intelligence of smiling and non-smiling individuals) and UA practices was high, r = .63, p < .001, and remained strong after controlling for economic factors (β = .65, p < .001). UA practices remained a significant predictor even when controlling for the heterogeneity-homogeneity of 28 cultures in our study that had heterogeneity-homogeneity data available (Rychlowska et al. 2015; β = .67, p < .001). Consistent with predictions, multilevel analyses revealed a significant cross-level interaction between facial expression and UA in the prediction of intelligence attributions (for details of these analyses see Table 4).
Table 2
Results of two ANOVA analyses for intelligence and honesty perception
 
Intelligence perception
Honesty perception
df
F
η squared
F
η squared
Smile
45.9***
.010
540.6***
.109
1, 4425
Culture
13.3***
.115
20.3***
.165
43, 4425
PG
94.2***
.021
50.2***
.011
1, 4425
TG
24.5***
.006
78.6***
.017
1, 4425
Smile × culture
5.9***
.054
4.5***
.042
43, 4425
Smile × PG
6.7**
.002
11.2**
.003
1, 4425
Smile × TG
1.5
.000
41.9***
.009
1, 4425
Culture × PG
3.9***
.037
2.3***
.022
43, 4425
Culture × TG
4.3***
.040
4.2***
.039
43, 4425
PG × TG
47.4***
.011
1.7
.000
1, 4425
Smile × culture × PG
.8
.008
1.3
.012
43, 4425
Smile × culture × TG
1.3
.013
1.0
.010
43, 4425
Smile × PG × TG
.5
.000
1.0
.000
1, 4425
Culture × PG × TG
1.7**
.016
1.1
.010
43, 4425
Smile × culture × PG × TG
1.1
.011
.6
.006
43, 4425
PG participant’s gender, TG target’s gender. Smile and gender of target as within-subjects factors, and culture and gender of observer as between-subjects factors. Significant values are shown in bold
** p < .01, *** p < .001
Table 3
Analysis at the cultural level: correlations and standardized regression coefficients
 
Intelligence d
Honesty d
Correlations (r)
Cultural practices (GLOBE project)
 Uncertainty avoidance
.63***
.24
 Power distance
−.16
−.28
 Institutional collectivism
−.14
.05
 In-group collectivism
−.29
−.37*
 Gender egalitarianism
−.06
.16
 Assertiveness
.21
.20
 Future orientation
.36*
.28
 Performance orientation
.22
.11
 Humane orientation
−.03
−.17
Cultural values (Schwartz—S; Hofstede—H)
 Harmony—S
.26
.28
 Embeddedness—S
−.22
−.32*
 Hierarchy—S
−.34*
−.35*
 Mastery—S
−.22
−.21
 Affective autonomy—S
.17
.18
 Intellectual autonomy—S
.24
.36*
 Egalitarianism—S
.32*
.35*
 Power distance—H
−.21
−.25
 Individualism—H
.13
.23
 Masculinity—H
.09
.23
 Uncertainty avoidance—H
−.30
.00
 Long term orientation—H
.02
.05
 Indulgence—H
.15
.34*
Social axioms (Bond et al. 2004)
 Dynamic externality
.06
−.36
 Societal cynicism
−.13
−.29
Corruption indexes
 Corruption perception index—ranking
−.23
−.53***
 Global corruption barometer—paying bribe
−.27
−.53**
 Bribe payers index
.41
.59**
 Economic freedom Index—corruption
.28
.51***
Socio-economic indexes
 GDP per capita
.21
.38*
 GDP PPP
−.01
−.02
 GINI index
−.01
−.07
 Historical heterogeneity (vs. homogeneity)
.09
.29
 Life expectation at birth
.09
.38*
 Literacy rate
−.29
.23
 Military expenditures (% GDP)
−.22
−.09
 Population density
−.09
−.14
 Population growth
.05
−.10
 Rural population (% total)
−.02
−.32*
 Unemployment rate
−.13
−.08
Regressions (β)
 
Model 1
 
 Uncertainty avoidance (GLOBE)
.65***
 GDP per capita
.02
 GINI Index
.15
  
Model 2
 Corruption perception index
−.48*
 GDP per capita
−.02
 Life expectation at birth
.03
 Rural population (% total)
−.01
Highly significant (p < .01) values are shown in bold
* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
Table 4
Unstandardized coefficients from multilevel linear regression analyses of perceived intelligence (Models 1 and 2) and perceived honesty (Models 3 and 4)
 
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
DV: perceived intelligence
DV: perceived honesty
IV culture: uncertainty avoidance
IV culture: Corruption Perceptions Index
Intercept
4.7459 (0.2719)***
4.7399 (0.2557)***
4.6347 (0.0688)***
4.62484 (0.0659)***
Culture
0.0105 (0.0651)
0.0055 (0.0612)
−0.0007 (0.0008)
−0.0004 (0.0007)
Smile
−0.8997 (0.1684)***
−0.7745 (0.0907)***
0.3419 (0.0372)***
0.3233 (0.0226)***
PG
−0.8322 (0.2162)***
−0.5479 (0.1466)***
−0.1399 (0.0455)**
−0.1099 (0.0167)***
TG
−0.0153 (0.1684)
−0.0777 (0.0118)***
−0.1409 (0.0372)***
−0.1307 (0.0226)***
Culture × smile
0.2420 (0.0405)***
0.2096 (0.0217)***
−0.0014 (0.0005)**
−0.0017 (0.0003)***
Culture × PG
0.1489 (0.0520)**
0.0891 (0.0355)*
0.0009 (0.0006)
Smile × PG
0.2191 (0.2596)
−0.0255 (0.0560)
Culture × TG
−0.0349 (0.0405)
−0.0013 (0.0005)**
−0.0011 (0.0003)***
Smile × TG
0.1016 (0.2381)
0.1876 (0.0526)***
0.1780 (0.0225)***
PG × TG
0.4127 (0.2596)
0.0388 (0.0560)
Culture × smile × PG
−0.0698 (0.0621)
−0.0009 (0.0007)
Culture × smile × TG
−0.0146 (0.0572)
0.0002 (0.0007)
Culture × PG × TG
−0.0658 (0.0621)
0.0003 (0.0007)
Smile × PG × TG
−0.1263 (0.3671)
−0.0538 (0.0791)
Culture × smile × PG × TG
0.0322 (0.0878)
0.0001 (0.0010)
PG participant’s gender, TG target’s gender. For models 1 and 2, culture means culture-level predictor Uncertainty Avoidance; for models 3 and 4 culture means culture-level predictor Corruption Perceptions Index. Robust standard errors are given in parentheses
p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
We also found support for our second hypothesis. Although smiling individuals were perceived as more honest than non-smiling individuals in almost all analyzed cultures (37 out of 44), there was cultural variability in the size of the effect (we observed a significant two-way culture by smile interaction, F(43, 4425) = 4.5, p < .001, η p 2  = .04; for a summary of ANOVA analyses see Table 2). Moreover, this cultural variability was related to societal corruption levels (see Table 3). The correlation between a smile’s honesty bonus and three different corruption indices: Corruption Perceptions Index (Transparency International 2010a), Global Corruption Barometer—Paying Bribe sub-dimension (Transparency International 2010b), and Index of Economic Freedom – sub-index Freedom from Corruption (The Heritage Foundation 2010), was significant and relatively high (.59 > r > .51, p < .01) and remained significant after controlling for socio-economic factors (β = −.48, p = .04). Corruption remained a significant predictor even when controlling for the heterogeneity-homogeneity of 28 cultures in our study that had heterogeneity-homogeneity data available (Rychlowska et al. 2015; β = −.64, p < .001). Multilevel analyses revealed an interaction between facial expression and corruption index in the prediction of honesty judgments (see Table 4). As predicted, greater corruption levels decreased trust granted toward smiling individuals.
Beyond cultural variability, we found that participant and target gender were important factors that influenced the social perception of smiling versus non-smiling individuals (see Table 2). For intelligence perception, we observed a significant two-way participant gender by smile interaction, indicating that smiling increases ratings of intelligence more among women (t[2551] = 7.80, p < .001, d = .15, M Non-smileFemale = 4.71, SD Non-smileFemale = .75, M SmileFemale = 4.83, SD SmileFemale = .77) than among men (t[1960] = 3.37, p = .001, d = .08, M Non-smileMale = 4.54, SD Non-smileMale = .76, M Smile_Male = 4.61, SD Smile_Male = .78). Two remaining two-way interactions regarded perceptions of honesty. A significant participant gender by smile interaction indicated that smiling increases ratings of honesty more for female assessors (t[2551] = 22.66, p < .001, d = .45, M Non-smileFemale = 4.48, SD Non-smileFemale = .71, M SmileFemale = 4.83, SD SmileFemale = .73) than for male assessors (t[1960] = 13.73, p < .001, d = .31, M Non-smileMale = 4.40, SD Non-smileMale = .67, M SmileMale = 4.64, SD SmileMale = .72). Finally, a significant target gender by smile interaction revealed that non-smiling women were assessed as more honest than non-smiling men (t[4518] = 12.76, p < .001, M Non-smile_Female = 4.55, SD Non-smile_Female = .86, M Non-smile_Male = 4.36, SD Non-smile_Male = .89), but smiling men and women were found to be equally honest (t[4518] = 1.40, p = .16, M Smile_Female = 4.77, SD Smile_Female = .89, M Smile_Male = 4.74, SD Smile_Male = .91).

Discussion

In sum, the data illustrate that the perception of smiling individuals is culturally diversified and that, in some cultures, this generally positive nonverbal signal may have negative associations. In addition, this research indicates that corruption at the societal level may weaken the meaning of an evolutionary important signal such as smiling and undermine its trustworthiness. Our results also show that the gender of the assessor and target have an important influence on the social perception of smiles, which may be related to socialization processes and strong stereotypical expectations for women to be more communal and to smile (LaFrance et al. 2003). Across cultures, smiling increased attributions of intelligence and honesty more for female assessors than for males and target gender affected attributions of honesty in non-smiling targets, but not for smiling targets. These effects of participant and target gender on smile perception did not affect the interactions of culture and smiling, however, which are the focus of the current report.
Although the results are statistically significant, causal inferences need to be drawn with caution because the relationships indicated in our research are mainly correlational. Another limitation of the presented research is that the samples may not be fully representative of the cultures they come from (i.e., predominantly university students were recruited to participate). Furthermore, the situational context, which was not manipulated here, may also play an important role in the perception and judgment of smiles (Niedenthal et al. 2010). Lastly, future studies are needed to examine the potential influence of participant ethnicity as attributions may differ across in-group and out-group faces. The role of smile intensity is also another ripe area for future research (see Kraus and Chen 2013 for instance).
Despite these limitations, this cross-cultural study illuminates surprising nuances of up-to-now seemingly clear and obvious processes of smile perception. Although numerous studies suggest that smiling individuals are perceived favorably, we document that the same person may be judged as less intelligent when smiling than when posing a neutral expression in some cultures. This has important practical implications, for example, in the context of globalization and job applications. In many countries it is still common to submit photographs on one’s CV. Knowing whether a smile is interpreted positively (i.e., as a sign of competence and trustworthiness) or negatively may be crucial knowledge for international applicants (also see Ruben et al. 2014).
Furthermore, this study advances theory about nonverbal behavior in important ways. By recognizing processes underlying the two cultural dimensions used in perceiving smiles, we indicate that those seemingly counter-intuitive findings reflect on highly functional strategies in their own cultural context (also see Matsumoto 2006). Expressing certainty in uncertain social conditions may not be the best way of signalling intelligence (Hypothesis 1), and signalling unconditional trust in untrustworthy settings may be risky (Hypothesis 2). Our research underscores the importance of the cultural framework in understanding nonverbal communication processes and reveals that although positive traits are usually attributed to smiling persons, perception of this common nonverbal signal may have unexpected negative implications in some cultures.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the supporting team-members involved in the translation procedures and data collections. Research was supported by the Polish NCN Grant 2011/03/N/HS6/05112 (K.K.) and Chinese NNSF Grant 31200788 (C.X).
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Abe, J., Beetham, M., & Izard, C. (2002). What do smiles mean? An analysis in terms of differential emotions theory. In A. Millicent (Ed.), The smile: Forms, functions, and consequences (pp. 83–110). New York: The Edwin Mellen Press. Abe, J., Beetham, M., & Izard, C. (2002). What do smiles mean? An analysis in terms of differential emotions theory. In A. Millicent (Ed.), The smile: Forms, functions, and consequences (pp. 83–110). New York: The Edwin Mellen Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Abele, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2013). The Big Two in social judgment and behavior. Social Psychology, 44, 61–62.CrossRef Abele, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2013). The Big Two in social judgment and behavior. Social Psychology, 44, 61–62.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Abele, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition. A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.CrossRef Abele, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition. A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally. Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Zurück zum Zitat Bedford, N., Fallon, S., & McAdam, M. (2008). Lonely planet—Destination Poland. Oakland, CA: LPP. Bedford, N., Fallon, S., & McAdam, M. (2008). Lonely planet—Destination Poland. Oakland, CA: LPP.
Zurück zum Zitat Bond, M., Leung, K., Au, A., Tong, K.-K., De Carrasquel, S., Murakami, F., et al. (2004). Culture-level dimensions of social axioms and their correlates across 41 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 548–570.CrossRef Bond, M., Leung, K., Au, A., Tong, K.-K., De Carrasquel, S., Murakami, F., et al. (2004). Culture-level dimensions of social axioms and their correlates across 41 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 548–570.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brislin, R. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 185–216.CrossRef Brislin, R. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 185–216.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brody, L., & Hall, J. (2008). Gender and emotion in context. In M. Lewis, J. Haviland-Jones, & L. Feldman Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 395–408). New York: Guilford Press. Brody, L., & Hall, J. (2008). Gender and emotion in context. In M. Lewis, J. Haviland-Jones, & L. Feldman Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 395–408). New York: Guilford Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Centorrino, S., Djemai, E., Hopfensitz, A., Milinski, M., & Seabright, P. (2015). Honest signalling in trust interactions: Smiles rated as genuine induce trust and signal higher earnings opportunities. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 8–16.CrossRef Centorrino, S., Djemai, E., Hopfensitz, A., Milinski, M., & Seabright, P. (2015). Honest signalling in trust interactions: Smiles rated as genuine induce trust and signal higher earnings opportunities. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 8–16.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chang, B., & Vermeulen, N. (2010). Re-thinking the causes, processes, and consequences of simulation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 441–442.CrossRef Chang, B., & Vermeulen, N. (2010). Re-thinking the causes, processes, and consequences of simulation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 441–442.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Darwin, C. (1872/1998). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Darwin, C. (1872/1998). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Duchenne, C. (1862). The mechanism of human facial expression. New York: Cambridge University Press. Duchenne, C. (1862). The mechanism of human facial expression. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunton, B., & Fazio, R. (1997). An individual difference measure of motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 316–326.CrossRef Dunton, B., & Fazio, R. (1997). An individual difference measure of motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 316–326.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ekman, P. (2009). Lie catching and microexpressions. In C. Martin (Ed.), The philosophy of deception (pp. 118–133). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Ekman, P. (2009). Lie catching and microexpressions. In C. Martin (Ed.), The philosophy of deception (pp. 118–133). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1982). Felt, false, and miserable smiles. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6, 238–252.CrossRef Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1982). Felt, false, and miserable smiles. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6, 238–252.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fiske, S., Cuddy, A., & Glick, P. (2006). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 77–83.CrossRef Fiske, S., Cuddy, A., & Glick, P. (2006). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 77–83.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fridlund, A. (2002). The behavioral ecology view of smiling and other facial expressions. In A. Millicent (Ed.), The smile: Forms, functions, and consequences (pp. 45–82). New York: The Edwin Mellen Press. Fridlund, A. (2002). The behavioral ecology view of smiling and other facial expressions. In A. Millicent (Ed.), The smile: Forms, functions, and consequences (pp. 45–82). New York: The Edwin Mellen Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Hall, J. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Hall, J. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2010). What emotional reactions can tell us about the nature of others: An appraisal perspective on person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 128–140.CrossRef Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2010). What emotional reactions can tell us about the nature of others: An appraisal perspective on person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 128–140.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Henrich, J., Heine, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83.CrossRefPubMed Henrich, J., Heine, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hess, U., Blairy, S., & Kleck, R. (2000). The influence of expression intensity, gender, and ethnicity on judgments of dominance and affiliation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 265–283.CrossRef Hess, U., Blairy, S., & Kleck, R. (2000). The influence of expression intensity, gender, and ethnicity on judgments of dominance and affiliation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 265–283.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hess, U., Beaupré, M., & Cheung, N. (2002). To whom and why—cultural differences and similarities in the function of smiles. In A. Millicent (Ed.), The smile: Forms, functions, and consequences (pp. 187–216). New York: The Edwin Mellen Press. Hess, U., Beaupré, M., & Cheung, N. (2002). To whom and why—cultural differences and similarities in the function of smiles. In A. Millicent (Ed.), The smile: Forms, functions, and consequences (pp. 187–216). New York: The Edwin Mellen Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Hess, U., Adams, R., & Kleck, R. (2009). The categorical perception of emotions and traits. Social Cognition, 27, 319–325.CrossRef Hess, U., Adams, R., & Kleck, R. (2009). The categorical perception of emotions and traits. Social Cognition, 27, 319–325.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks: Sage. Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat House, R., Hanges, P., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P., & Gupta, V. (2004). Culture, leadership, and organizations. The GLOBE study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks: Sage. House, R., Hanges, P., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P., & Gupta, V. (2004). Culture, leadership, and organizations. The GLOBE study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Kraus, M., & Chen, T. (2013). A winning smile? Smile intensity, physical dominance, and fighter performance, Emotion, 13, 270–279. Kraus, M., & Chen, T. (2013). A winning smile? Smile intensity, physical dominance, and fighter performance, Emotion, 13, 270–279.
Zurück zum Zitat Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A., & Kappas, A. (2007). Temporal aspects of facial displays in person and expression perception: The effects of smile dynamics, head-tilt, and gender. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour, 31, 39–56.CrossRef Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A., & Kappas, A. (2007). Temporal aspects of facial displays in person and expression perception: The effects of smile dynamics, head-tilt, and gender. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour, 31, 39–56.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Krys, K., Hansen, K., Xing, C., Szarota, P., & Yang, M. (2014). Do only fools smile at strangers? Cultural differences in social perception of intelligence of smiling individuals. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 314–321.CrossRef Krys, K., Hansen, K., Xing, C., Szarota, P., & Yang, M. (2014). Do only fools smile at strangers? Cultural differences in social perception of intelligence of smiling individuals. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 314–321.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Krys, K., Hansen, K., Xing, C., Espinoza, A., Szarota, P., & Morales, M. (2015). It is better to smile to women: Gender modifies perception of honesty of smiling individuals across cultures. International Journal of Psychology, 50, 150–154.CrossRefPubMed Krys, K., Hansen, K., Xing, C., Espinoza, A., Szarota, P., & Morales, M. (2015). It is better to smile to women: Gender modifies perception of honesty of smiling individuals across cultures. International Journal of Psychology, 50, 150–154.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat LaFrance, M., Hecht, M., & Paluck, E. (2003). The contingent smile: A meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 305–334.CrossRefPubMed LaFrance, M., Hecht, M., & Paluck, E. (2003). The contingent smile: A meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 305–334.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Leung, K., & Bond, M. H. (2004). Social axioms: A model of social beliefs in multi-cultural perspective. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Leung, K., & Bond, M. H. (2004). Social axioms: A model of social beliefs in multi-cultural perspective. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Leung, A., & Cohen, D. (2011). Within- and between-culture variation: Individual differences and the cultural logics of honor, face, and dignity cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 507–526.CrossRef Leung, A., & Cohen, D. (2011). Within- and between-culture variation: Individual differences and the cultural logics of honor, face, and dignity cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 507–526.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Matsumoto, D. (2006). Culture and nonverbal behavior. In V. Manusov & M. Patterson (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 219–235). Thousand Oaks: Sage.CrossRef Matsumoto, D. (2006). Culture and nonverbal behavior. In V. Manusov & M. Patterson (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 219–235). Thousand Oaks: Sage.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Matsumoto, D., & Kudoh, T. (1993). American–Japanese cultural differences in attributions of personality based on smiles. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17, 231–243.CrossRef Matsumoto, D., & Kudoh, T. (1993). American–Japanese cultural differences in attributions of personality based on smiles. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17, 231–243.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2009). Spontaneous facial expressions of emotion of congenitally and noncongenitally blind individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1–10.CrossRefPubMed Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2009). Spontaneous facial expressions of emotion of congenitally and noncongenitally blind individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1–10.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Méhu, M. (2011). Smiling and laughter in naturally occurring dyadic interactions: Relationship to conversation, body contacts, and displacement activities. Human Ethology Bulletin, 26, 10–28. Méhu, M. (2011). Smiling and laughter in naturally occurring dyadic interactions: Relationship to conversation, body contacts, and displacement activities. Human Ethology Bulletin, 26, 10–28.
Zurück zum Zitat Méhu, M., & Dunbar, R. (2008). Naturalistic observations of smiling and laughter in human group interactions. Behaviour, 145, 1747–1780.CrossRef Méhu, M., & Dunbar, R. (2008). Naturalistic observations of smiling and laughter in human group interactions. Behaviour, 145, 1747–1780.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Miles, L. (2009). Who is approachable? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 262–266.CrossRef Miles, L. (2009). Who is approachable? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 262–266.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Minear, M., & Park, D. (2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 630–633.CrossRef Minear, M., & Park, D. (2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 630–633.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Niedenthal, P., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M., & Hess, U. (2010). The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) Model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 417–433.CrossRefPubMed Niedenthal, P., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M., & Hess, U. (2010). The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) Model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 417–433.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Otta, E., Lira, B., Delevati, N., Cesar, O., & Pires, C. (1994). The effect of smiling and of head tilting on person perception. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 128, 323–331.CrossRef Otta, E., Lira, B., Delevati, N., Cesar, O., & Pires, C. (1994). The effect of smiling and of head tilting on person perception. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 128, 323–331.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Palmer, M., & Simmons, K. (1995). Communicating intentions through nonverbal behaviors. Human Communication Research, 22, 128–160.CrossRef Palmer, M., & Simmons, K. (1995). Communicating intentions through nonverbal behaviors. Human Communication Research, 22, 128–160.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruben, M., Hall, J., & Schmid Mast, M. (2014). Smiling in a job interview: When less is more. Journal of Social Psychology, 155, 107–126.CrossRef Ruben, M., Hall, J., & Schmid Mast, M. (2014). Smiling in a job interview: When less is more. Journal of Social Psychology, 155, 107–126.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rychlowska, M., Miyamoto, Y., Matsumoto, D., Hess, U., Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Kamble, S., et al. (2015). Heterogeneity of long-history migration explains cultural differences in reports of emotional expressivity and the functions of smiles. PNAS, 112, E2429–E2436.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rychlowska, M., Miyamoto, Y., Matsumoto, D., Hess, U., Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Kamble, S., et al. (2015). Heterogeneity of long-history migration explains cultural differences in reports of emotional expressivity and the functions of smiles. PNAS, 112, E2429–E2436.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Smith, P., Fischer, R., Vignoles, V., & Bond, M. (2013). Understanding social psychology across cultures. Engaging with others in a changing world. London: Sage. Smith, P., Fischer, R., Vignoles, V., & Bond, M. (2013). Understanding social psychology across cultures. Engaging with others in a changing world. London: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Thibault, P., Levesque, M., Gosselin, P., & Hess, U. (2012). The Duchenne marker is NOT a universal signal of smile authenticity: But it can be learned! Social Psychology, 43, 215–221.CrossRef Thibault, P., Levesque, M., Gosselin, P., & Hess, U. (2012). The Duchenne marker is NOT a universal signal of smile authenticity: But it can be learned! Social Psychology, 43, 215–221.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Warner-Søderholm, G. (2010). Understanding perceptions of cultural and intracultural societal practices and values of Norwegian managers (Doctoral dissertation). University of Reading: Reading. Warner-Søderholm, G. (2010). Understanding perceptions of cultural and intracultural societal practices and values of Norwegian managers (Doctoral dissertation). University of Reading: Reading.
Zurück zum Zitat Weisbuch, M., Ambady, N., Clarke, A., Achor, S., & Weele, J. (2010). On being consistent: The role of verbal–nonverbal consistency in first impressions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 32, 261–268.CrossRef Weisbuch, M., Ambady, N., Clarke, A., Achor, S., & Weele, J. (2010). On being consistent: The role of verbal–nonverbal consistency in first impressions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 32, 261–268.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wörmann, V., Holodynski, M., Kärtner, J., & Keller, H. (2014). The emergence of social smiling: The interplay of maternal and infant imitation during the first three months in cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 339–361.CrossRef Wörmann, V., Holodynski, M., Kärtner, J., & Keller, H. (2014). The emergence of social smiling: The interplay of maternal and infant imitation during the first three months in cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 339–361.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Be Careful Where You Smile: Culture Shapes Judgments of Intelligence and Honesty of Smiling Individuals
verfasst von
Kuba Krys
C. -Melanie Vauclair
Colin A. Capaldi
Vivian Miu-Chi Lun
Michael Harris Bond
Alejandra Domínguez-Espinosa
Claudio Torres
Ottmar V. Lipp
L. Sam S. Manickam
Cai Xing
Radka Antalíková
Vassilis Pavlopoulos
Julien Teyssier
Taekyun Hur
Karolina Hansen
Piotr Szarota
Ramadan A. Ahmed
Eleonora Burtceva
Ana Chkhaidze
Enila Cenko
Patrick Denoux
Márta Fülöp
Arif Hassan
David O. Igbokwe
İdil Işık
Gwatirera Javangwe
María Malbran
Fridanna Maricchiolo
Hera Mikarsa
Lynden K. Miles
Martin Nader
Joonha Park
Muhammad Rizwan
Radwa Salem
Beate Schwarz
Irfana Shah
Chien-Ru Sun
Wijnand van Tilburg
Wolfgang Wagner
Ryan Wise
Angela Arriola Yu
Publikationsdatum
30.12.2015
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior / Ausgabe 2/2016
Print ISSN: 0191-5886
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-3653
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-015-0226-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2016

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 2/2016 Zur Ausgabe