Skip to main content

Abstract

In this chapter some special features of mathematical knowledge are considered in order to better understand the nature of conceptual change in this domain. In learning mathematics, every extension to the number concept demands, not only accepting new concepts, but new logic as well. This new logic more or less contradicts the prior fundamental logic of natural numbers. Therefore, misconceptions and learning difficulties are possible at every enlargement. To understand the problems students have in the conceptual change pertaining to the enlargement of the number concept a test was administered to 564 students (mean age 17.3) from randomly selected Finnish upper secondary schools. The test included identification, classification and construction problems in the domain of rational and real numbers. We found that changes of number conceptions, which was measured through questions in the domain of rational and real numbers, was not adequately carried out by the majority of the students who had just finished their first calculus class. While working on the tasks on the more advanced numbers they spontaneously used the logic and general presumptions of natural numbers or based their answers on their everyday intuition. The number concept of the majority of these students seemed to be based on the spontaneous logic of natural numbers but had also fragmented pieces of more advanced numbers. The students tended to overestimate the certainty of their answers when they used the logic of natural numbers even if it was erroneous.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Boyer, C. B. (1959). The history of calculus and its conceptual development (2nd ed.). New York: Dover Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind. Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 169–200). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. (1992). Conceptual Change within and across Ontological Categories: Examples from Learning and Discovery in Science. In R. Giere (Ed.) Cognitive models of science. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science (pp. 129–186). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M., Slotta, J., & de Leeuw, N. (1994). From things to process: A theory of conceptual change for learning science concepts. Learning and Instruction, 26, 27–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornu, B. (1991). Limits, In Tall, D. (Ed.) Advanced mathematical thinking (pp. 153–166). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dantzig, T. (1954). Number, the language of science (4th ed.) New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, T. (1991). Advanced mathematical thinking processes. In D. Tall, D. (Ed.) Advanced mathematical thinking (pp. 25–41). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubinsky, E. (1991). Reflective abstraction in advanced mathematical thinking. In D. Tall (Ed.) Advanced mathematical thinking (pp. 95–123). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E., Jehiam, R., & Cohen, D. (1995). The concept of irrational numbers in high-school students and prospective teachers. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 29, 29–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics. An educational approach. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: D. Reidel Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, G. R,. & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44, 43–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R., & Brenneman, K. (1994). First principles can support both universal and culture-specific learning about number and music. In L. A. Hirschfeld, & S. A. Gelman (Eds.) Mapping the mind. Domain specificy in cognition and culture (pp 369–390). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodson-Espy, T. (1998). The roles of reification and reflective abstraction in the development of abstract thought: transition from arithmetic to algebra. Educational Studies in mathematics, 36, 219–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cifarelli, V. (1988). The role of abstraction as a learning process in mathematical problem solving. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Purdue University, IN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartnett, P., & Gelman, R. (1998). Early understanding of numbers: paths or barriers to the construction of new understandings? Learning and Instruction, 8, 341–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harel, G., & Kaput, J. (1991). The role of conceptual entities and their symbols in building advanced mathematical concepts. In D. Tall (Ed.) Advanced mathematical thinking (pp. 82–94). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G. (1996). A conception of knowledge acquisition and its implications for mathematics education. In P. Steffe, P. Nesher, P. Cobb, G. Goldin, & B. Greer (Eds.) Theories of mathematical learning (pp. 197–217). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaput, J. (1994). Democratizing access to calculus: New routes to old roots. In A. J. Schoenfeld (Ed.) Mathematical thinking and problem solving (pp. 77–155). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1995. Beyond modularity. A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kieren, T. (1992). Rational and fractional numbers as mathematical and personal knowledge: implications for curriculum and instruction. In G. Leinhardt, R. Putnam, & R. Hattrup (Eds.) Analysis of arithmetic for mathematics teaching (pp. 323–371). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline, M. (1980). Mathematics. The loss ofcertainty. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, E. (1960). Foundations of analysis. The arithmetic of whole, rational, irrational and complex numbers (2nd ed.). New York: Chelsea Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehtinen, E. (1998, November). Conceptual change in mathematics. Paper presented at the Second European Symposium on Conceptual Change. Madrid, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehtinen, E., Merenluoto, K., & Kasanen, E (1997). Conceptual change in mathematics: From rational to (un)real numbers. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 12, 131–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehtinen, E., & Ohlsson, S. (1999, August). The function of abstraction in deep learning. Paper presented at the 8th European Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction, Göteborg, Sweden.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merenluoto K., & Lehtinen E. (2000) The “conflicting” concepts of continuity and limit, a conceptual change perspective: Proceedings of the 24th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, T. Nakahara & M. Koyama (Eds), Hiroshima University, 3, 303–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, R. (1998). Schülervorstellungen bezüglich der Dichtheit von Bruchzahlen. Mathematica didadtica. Zeitschrift für Didaktik der Mathematik. 21, 109–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohlsson, S., & Lehtinen, E. (1997). Abstraction and the acquisition of complex ideas. International Journal of Educational Research, 27, 37–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. (1993). Introduction to mathematical philosophy. New York: Dover Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. (1996). The principles of mathematics (2nd ed.) London: Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudin, W. (1953). Principles of mathematical analysis. London: McGraw-Hill

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 105–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (1991). On the dual nature of mathematical conceptions: Reflections on processes and objects as different sides of the same coin. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 22, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A, & Linchevski, L. (1994). The gains and pitfalls of reification — the case of algebra. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 26, 191–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sierpinska, A. (1987). Humanities students and epistemological obstacles related to limits. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 18, 371–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, S. E. 1991. Physical knowledge in infancy: Reflections on Piaget’s theory. In S. Carey & R. Gelman (Eds.) The epigenesis of mind. Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 133–170). Hillsdale: NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stafilidou, M., & Vosniadou, S. (1999, August). Children’s believes about the mathematical concept of fraction. Abstracts. Proceedings of the European Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction, Göteborg, Sweden.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starkey, P., Spelke, E,. & Gelman, R. (1990). Numerical abstraction by human infants. Cognition, 36, 97–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Szydlik, J. E. (2000). Mathematical beliefs and conceptual understanding of the limit of a function. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 31, 258–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tall, D. & Schwarzenberger, R. (1978). Conflict in the learning of real numbers and limits. Mathematics Teaching 82, 44–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tall, D. (1991). The Psychology of Advanced Mathematical Thinking. In Tall, D, (Ed.) Advanced mathematical thinking (pp. 3–24). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tall, D., & Vinner, S. (1981). Concept image and concept definition in mathematics with particular reference to limits and continuity. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 12, 151–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S. (1994). Capturing and modelling the process of conceptual change. Learning and Instruction, 4, 45–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S. (1996). Towards a revised cognitive psychology for new advances in learning and instruction. Learning and Instruction, 6, 95–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Ioannides, C. (1998). From conceptual development to science Education: a psychological point of view. International Journal of Science Education, 20, 1213–1230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, S. R. (1991). Models of limit held by college students. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 22, 219–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wistedt, I., & Martinsson, M. (1996). Orchestrating a mathematical theme: eleven year olds discuss the problem of infinity. Learning and Instruction, 6, 173–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2002 Kluwer Academic Publishers

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Merenluoto, K., Lehtinen, E. (2002). Conceptual Change in Mathematics: Understanding the Real Numbers. In: Limón, M., Mason, L. (eds) Reconsidering Conceptual Change: Issues in Theory and Practice. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-47637-1_13

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-47637-1_13

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4020-0494-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-0-306-47637-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics