Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Human Studies 3/2019

11.12.2018 | Theoretical / Philosophical Paper

Cogitor Ergo Sum: The Origin of Self-awareness in Dyadic Interaction

verfasst von: Stephen Langfur

Erschienen in: Human Studies | Ausgabe 3/2019

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

When I see a mountain to be far away, there is non-reflective awareness of myself as that from which distance is measured. Likewise, there is self-awareness when I see a tree as offering shade or a hiding place. In such cases, how can the self I am aware of be the same as I who am aware of it? Can the perceived be its perceiver? Mobilizing infancy research, I offer the following thesis as to how one can be aware of oneself, at a single stroke, as perceiver and as embodied entity. During face-to-face interaction at 2 or 3 months, the infant has a sensuous perception of the caregiver as well as a non-sensuous impression of something she is eyeing and vocalizing toward. This implicit target is the self as it first becomes present to the child. It is shown how the target of her attending is experienced by him as embodied, active, affective, and continuous. After acquiring language, however, the child becomes capable of playing the caregiver toward himself: He can speak in her manner while listening as the one addressed. Thus the relation is internalized. The outcome is the independent and secure self-awareness that typifies post-infancy life. Independence bears a price, which is assessed.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
I have argued the main points of the thesis, called the You-I account, in previous papers to which I'll be referring. Langfur (2013) focused on the first 3 months of life. A paper of 2014 followed the carer-infant relation through the first 3 years, pitting the account against Heidegger's analysis of Dasein. Another of 2016 explained time-consciousness in terms of the account, comparing the latter with Husserl's view. What then justifies a new treatment? There has been further thought, stimulated by readings in the work of Carey (2009), Csibra (2010), and Tomasello (2014). The result has been to strengthen the main pillar of the argument, namely, the claim that the human infant is born with the capacity to recognize a person as attending. Secondly, the decision to present the thesis directly, without comparisons, has provided me the scope to work out a number of topics more fully: the maintenance of self-awareness during the carer's absence; the effect of the carer's affect-attunements on the learning of psychological concepts in their twofold application (to self and others); and the power of the You-I account in explaining the developmental sequence of crawling, joint attention, and language acquisition. The article does not presuppose acquaintance with the earlier ones.
 
2
Because much of the material I'll be citing is based on studies of mother-infant behavior, to avoid confusion I'll use masculine pronouns for the infant and feminine for the caregiver.
 
3
The Murray-Trevarthen experiment is replicable only when the experimenter waits for lively interaction before switching on the replay condition. See Nadel et al. (1999); Reddy (2008:76). Also, Gergeley (2002) has claimed that the Murray-Trevarthen data can be explained on the basis of sheer contingency (like that between a baby and a mobile that is set in motion by a string tied to his foot) without supposing that the infant perceives a person interacting with him. However, the baby smiles only half as much when contingency is not accompanied by eye contact (Hains and Muir 1996b: 1949; also Markova and Legerstee 2006; Reddy 2008: 77–82).
 
4
Merleau-Ponty (2005: 106): "[T]he two hands are never simultaneously in the relationship of touched and touching to each other".
 
5
Familiarity comes quickly to newborns (and probably to third-trimester fetuses): They "form 'prototypes' in less than 1 minute" (Walton and Bower 1993). Quick familiarity can account for another finding that might also mislead us into positing innate self-awareness: When newborns hear a tape of their own crying, they do not cry or show distress, but they do in response to tapes of other newborns crying. According to Dondi et al. (1999), the ability to resist contagion from familiar ("own") crying is probably an evolutionary inheritance; without it there would be an ever-intensifying spiral of wails, which would damage the baby's health (Malatesta 1985), not to mention that of the parents.
 
6
Newborns adjust head positions according to optic flow (Jouen and Gapenne 1995). Could self-awareness arise on the basis of head adjustments alone? This seems unlikely. Referring to upper and lower pathways in the brain, Bertenthal and Rose write that the information from the optic flow in this case "is restricted to its visuomotor function and not accessible to the infant in the form of self-knowledge" (1995: 309). Nor could head movements create a form of self-awareness at 2–3 months that would include the body as a whole.
 
7
In a thought he left undeveloped (1989: 101, n. 1), Husserl suggests that, even without independent self-awareness, imitative vocalizations between carer and infant can serve as a bridge both joining and differentiating them. Such vocalizations can indeed explain the perception of the carer as animated, but not as attending. See Langfur (2016).
 
8
When the experimenter protrudes her tongue, the newborn is being attended to. Such a moment could create a fleeting self-awareness, although the newborn is not in sufficient control of his gaze to sustain perception of the adult's attending.
 
9
A fuller account appears in Langfur (2013).
 
10
There are more patterns of response to the Strange Situation than the two I have named. See Karen 1998: 147–155.
 
11
About the transcendental ego: writing of "the understanding and its original power of combining the manifold of intuition," Kant says that "its synthesis…is nothing but the unity of the act, of which, as an act, it is conscious to itself" (1970: B153). But the newborn first opens his eyes on a manifold that is largely combined already (Carey 2009: 10–12, 40–46, 61–63), and further syntheses depend on interaction with carers (Langfur 2014).
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E. S., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in 5-month-old infants. Cognition, 20(3), 191–208.CrossRef Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E. S., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in 5-month-old infants. Cognition, 20(3), 191–208.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Baldwin, M. W., & Holmes, J. G. (1987). Salient private audiences and awareness of the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1087–1098.CrossRef Baldwin, M. W., & Holmes, J. G. (1987). Salient private audiences and awareness of the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1087–1098.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. In S. Baron-Cohen (Ed.), The maladapted mind: Classic readings in evolutionary psychopathology (pp. 207–239). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. In S. Baron-Cohen (Ed.), The maladapted mind: Classic readings in evolutionary psychopathology (pp. 207–239). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Bermúdez, J. L. (1998). The paradox of self-consciousness. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRef Bermúdez, J. L. (1998). The paradox of self-consciousness. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bertenthal, B. I. (1992). Implicit versus explicit origins of the self. Psychological Inquiry, 3(2), 112–133.CrossRef Bertenthal, B. I. (1992). Implicit versus explicit origins of the self. Psychological Inquiry, 3(2), 112–133.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bertenthal, B. I., & Rose, J. L. (1995). Two modes of perceiving the self. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research (pp. 303–325). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.CrossRef Bertenthal, B. I., & Rose, J. L. (1995). Two modes of perceiving the self. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research (pp. 303–325). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bigelow, A. E., & Rochat, P. (2006). Two-month-old infants’ sensitivity to social contingency in mother–infant and stranger–infant interaction. Infancy, 9(3), 313–325.CrossRef Bigelow, A. E., & Rochat, P. (2006). Two-month-old infants’ sensitivity to social contingency in mother–infant and stranger–infant interaction. Infancy, 9(3), 313–325.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Buber, M. (1958). I and thou (R. G. Smith, Trans.). New York: Scribner’s. Buber, M. (1958). I and thou (R. G. Smith, Trans.). New York: Scribner’s.
Zurück zum Zitat Buber, M. (2002). Between Man and Man (R. G. Smith, Trans.). New York: Routledge. Buber, M. (2002). Between Man and Man (R. G. Smith, Trans.). New York: Routledge.
Zurück zum Zitat Campanella, J., & Rovee-Collier, C. (2005). Latent learning and deferred imitation at 3 months. Infancy, 7(3), 243–262.CrossRef Campanella, J., & Rovee-Collier, C. (2005). Latent learning and deferred imitation at 3 months. Infancy, 7(3), 243–262.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1(2), 149–219.CrossRef Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1(2), 149–219.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Kindle Edition.CrossRef Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Kindle Edition.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Castañeda, H.-N. (2001). ‘He’: A study in the logic of self-consciousness. In A. Brook & R. C. DeVidi (Eds.), Self-reference and self-awareness (pp. 51–79). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef Castañeda, H.-N. (2001). ‘He’: A study in the logic of self-consciousness. In A. Brook & R. C. DeVidi (Eds.), Self-reference and self-awareness (pp. 51–79). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. S. (1974). Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science, 183(4120), 99–101.CrossRef Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. S. (1974). Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science, 183(4120), 99–101.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61(5), 1584–1595.CrossRef Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61(5), 1584–1595.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind and Language, 25(2), 141–168.CrossRef Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind and Language, 25(2), 141–168.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dondi, M., Simion, F., & Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant? Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 418–426.CrossRef Dondi, M., Simion, F., & Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant? Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 418–426.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dore, J. (1989). Monologue as reenvoicement of dialogue. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 231–260). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Dore, J. (1989). Monologue as reenvoicement of dialogue. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 231–260). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans, G. (2001). Self-identification. In A. Brook & R. C. DeVidi (Eds.), Self-reference and self-awareness (pp. 95–141). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef Evans, G. (2001). Self-identification. In A. Brook & R. C. DeVidi (Eds.), Self-reference and self-awareness (pp. 95–141). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99(14), 9602–9605.CrossRef Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99(14), 9602–9605.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fernyhough, C. (2016). The voices within: The history and science of how we talk to ourselves. London: Profile Books. Kindle edition. Fernyhough, C. (2016). The voices within: The history and science of how we talk to ourselves. London: Profile Books. Kindle edition.
Zurück zum Zitat Field, J. (1976). Relation of young infants’ reaching behavior to stimulus distance and solidity. Developmental Psychology, 12(5), 444–448.CrossRef Field, J. (1976). Relation of young infants’ reaching behavior to stimulus distance and solidity. Developmental Psychology, 12(5), 444–448.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Field, T. M. (1985). Neonatal perception of people: Maturational and individual differences. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 31–52). Ablex, NJ: Norwood. Field, T. M. (1985). Neonatal perception of people: Maturational and individual differences. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 31–52). Ablex, NJ: Norwood.
Zurück zum Zitat Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2004). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. London: Karnac. Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2004). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. London: Karnac.
Zurück zum Zitat Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., & Target, M. (2007). The parent-infant dyad and the construction of the subjective self. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(3/4), 288–328.CrossRef Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., & Target, M. (2007). The parent-infant dyad and the construction of the subjective self. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(3/4), 288–328.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Freud, S. (1960). The ego and the id (J. E. Strachey, Trans.). New York: Norton. Freud, S. (1960). The ego and the id (J. E. Strachey, Trans.). New York: Norton.
Zurück zum Zitat Gallagher, S. (1996). The moral significance of primitive self-consciousness: A response to Bermúdez. Ethics, 107(1), 129–140.CrossRef Gallagher, S. (1996). The moral significance of primitive self-consciousness: A response to Bermúdez. Ethics, 107(1), 129–140.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gergely, G. (2002). The development of understanding of self and agency. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 26–46). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRef Gergely, G. (2002). The development of understanding of self and agency. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 26–46). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J. J. (2015). The ecological approach to visual perception. New York: Psychology Press. Gibson, J. J. (2015). The ecological approach to visual perception. New York: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Hains, S. M., & Muir, D. W. (1996a). Infant sensitivity to adult eye direction. Child Development, 67(5), 1940–1951.CrossRef Hains, S. M., & Muir, D. W. (1996a). Infant sensitivity to adult eye direction. Child Development, 67(5), 1940–1951.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hains, S. M., & Muir, D. W. (1996b). Effects of stimulus contingency in infant-adult interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 19(1), 49–61.CrossRef Hains, S. M., & Muir, D. W. (1996b). Effects of stimulus contingency in infant-adult interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 19(1), 49–61.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hermans, H. J. M. (2004). The dialogical self: Between exchange and power. In H. J. M. Hermans & G. Dimaggio (Eds.), The dialogical self in psychotherapy: An introduction. New York: Brunner-Routedge.CrossRef Hermans, H. J. M. (2004). The dialogical self: Between exchange and power. In H. J. M. Hermans & G. Dimaggio (Eds.), The dialogical self in psychotherapy: An introduction. New York: Brunner-Routedge.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–134.CrossRef Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–134.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hurlburt, R. T., Heavey, C. L., & Kelsey, J. M. (2013). Toward a phenomenology of inner speaking. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1477–1494.CrossRef Hurlburt, R. T., Heavey, C. L., & Kelsey, J. M. (2013). Toward a phenomenology of inner speaking. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1477–1494.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Husserl, E. (1989). Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy, second book: Studies in the phenomenology of constitution (R. Rojcewicz & A. Schuwer, Trans.). Dordrecht: Kluwer. Husserl, E. (1989). Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy, second book: Studies in the phenomenology of constitution (R. Rojcewicz & A. Schuwer, Trans.). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Zurück zum Zitat Husserl, E. (1991). On the phenomenology of the consciousness of internal time (1893–1917) (J. B. Brough, Trans.). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic. Husserl, E. (1991). On the phenomenology of the consciousness of internal time (18931917) (J. B. Brough, Trans.). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.
Zurück zum Zitat James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience. New York: The Modern Library.CrossRef James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience. New York: The Modern Library.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jonsson, C., & Clinton, D. (2006). What do mothers attune to during interactions with their infants? Infant and Child Development, 15(4), 387–402.CrossRef Jonsson, C., & Clinton, D. (2006). What do mothers attune to during interactions with their infants? Infant and Child Development, 15(4), 387–402.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jonsson, C., Clinton, D. N., Fahrman, M., Mazzaglia, G., Novak, S., & Sörhus, K. (2001). How do mothers signal shared feeling-states to their infants? An investigation of affect attunement and imitation during the first year of life. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42(4), 377–381.CrossRef Jonsson, C., Clinton, D. N., Fahrman, M., Mazzaglia, G., Novak, S., & Sörhus, K. (2001). How do mothers signal shared feeling-states to their infants? An investigation of affect attunement and imitation during the first year of life. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42(4), 377–381.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jouen, F., & Gapenne, O. (1995). Interactions between the vestibular and visual systems in the neonate. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research (pp. 277–301). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.CrossRef Jouen, F., & Gapenne, O. (1995). Interactions between the vestibular and visual systems in the neonate. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research (pp. 277–301). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kant, I. (1970). Immanuel Kant’s critique of pure reason (N. K. Smith, Trans.). London: Macmillan. Kant, I. (1970). Immanuel Kant’s critique of pure reason (N. K. Smith, Trans.). London: Macmillan.
Zurück zum Zitat Karen, R. (1998). Becoming attached: First relationships and how they shape our capacity to love. New York: Oxford University Press. Karen, R. (1998). Becoming attached: First relationships and how they shape our capacity to love. New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaye, K. (1977). Toward the origin of dialogue. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 89–117). London: Academic Press. Kaye, K. (1977). Toward the origin of dialogue. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 89–117). London: Academic Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Keller, H. (2003). The story of my life: With supplementary accounts by Anne Sullivan, her teacher, and John Albert Macy. New York: W. W. Norton & Co. Keller, H. (2003). The story of my life: With supplementary accounts by Anne Sullivan, her teacher, and John Albert Macy. New York: W. W. Norton & Co.
Zurück zum Zitat Kleeman, J. A. (2017). The Peek-A-Boo game. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 22(1), 239–273.CrossRef Kleeman, J. A. (2017). The Peek-A-Boo game. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 22(1), 239–273.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kindle Edition. Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kindle Edition.
Zurück zum Zitat Langfur, S. (2013). The You-I event: On the genesis of self-awareness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 769–790.CrossRef Langfur, S. (2013). The You-I event: On the genesis of self-awareness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 769–790.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Langfur, S. (2014). Heidegger and the infant: A second-person alternative to the Dasein-analysis. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 34(4), 257–274.CrossRef Langfur, S. (2014). Heidegger and the infant: A second-person alternative to the Dasein-analysis. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 34(4), 257–274.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Langfur, S. (2016). The interactive now: A second-person approach to time-consciousness. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 47(2), 156–182.CrossRef Langfur, S. (2016). The interactive now: A second-person approach to time-consciousness. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 47(2), 156–182.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lecercle, J.-J., & Riley, D. (2004). The force of language. New York: Palgrave.CrossRef Lecercle, J.-J., & Riley, D. (2004). The force of language. New York: Palgrave.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Locke, J. L. (1993). The child’s path to spoken language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Locke, J. L. (1993). The child’s path to spoken language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Malatesta, C. Z. (1985). Developmental course of emotion expression in the human infant. In G. Zivin (Ed.), The development of expressive behavior: Biology, environment, interaction (pp. 183–219). New York: Academic Press.CrossRef Malatesta, C. Z. (1985). Developmental course of emotion expression in the human infant. In G. Zivin (Ed.), The development of expressive behavior: Biology, environment, interaction (pp. 183–219). New York: Academic Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Markova, G., & Legerstee, M. (2006). Contingency, imitation and affect sharing: Foundations of infants’ social awareness. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 132–141.CrossRef Markova, G., & Legerstee, M. (2006). Contingency, imitation and affect sharing: Foundations of infants’ social awareness. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 132–141.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Masataka, N. (2003). The onset of language. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Masataka, N. (2003). The onset of language. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McGinn, M. (1997). Wittgenstein and the Philosophical Investigations. London: Routledge. McGinn, M. (1997). Wittgenstein and the Philosophical Investigations. London: Routledge.
Zurück zum Zitat Mead, G. H. (1967). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Mead, G. H. (1967). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.CrossRef Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17(1), 83–99.CrossRef Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17(1), 83–99.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Merleau-Ponty, M. (2005). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans.). London: Taylor & Francis e-Library. Merleau-Ponty, M. (2005). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans.). London: Taylor & Francis e-Library.
Zurück zum Zitat Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In T. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177–197). New York: Ablex. Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In T. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177–197). New York: Ablex.
Zurück zum Zitat Musholt, K. (2015). Thinking about oneself: From nonconceptual content to the concept of a self. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRef Musholt, K. (2015). Thinking about oneself: From nonconceptual content to the concept of a self. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., & Reserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2(2), 164–173.CrossRef Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., & Reserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2(2), 164–173.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nadel, J., & Tremblay-Leveau, H. (1999). Early perception of social contingencies and interpersonal intentionality: Dyadic and triadic paradigms. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 189–212). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Nadel, J., & Tremblay-Leveau, H. (1999). Early perception of social contingencies and interpersonal intentionality: Dyadic and triadic paradigms. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 189–212). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Nagy, E. (2008). Innate intersubjectivity: Newborns’ sensitivity to communication disturbance. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1779–1784.CrossRef Nagy, E. (2008). Innate intersubjectivity: Newborns’ sensitivity to communication disturbance. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1779–1784.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1(1), 35–59.CrossRef Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1(1), 35–59.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson, K. (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Nelson, K. (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Peacocke, C. (2001). First-person reference, representational independence, and self-knowledge. In A. Brook & R. C. DeVidi (Eds.), Self-reference and self-awareness (pp. 215–245). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef Peacocke, C. (2001). First-person reference, representational independence, and self-knowledge. In A. Brook & R. C. DeVidi (Eds.), Self-reference and self-awareness (pp. 215–245). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic.CrossRef Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Preyer, W. (1890). The mind of the child. New York: D. Appleton. Preyer, W. (1890). The mind of the child. New York: D. Appleton.
Zurück zum Zitat Reddy, V. (2000). Coyness in early infancy. Developmental Science, 3(2), 186–192.CrossRef Reddy, V. (2000). Coyness in early infancy. Developmental Science, 3(2), 186–192.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Reddy, V. (2009). Before the “third element”: Understanding attention to self. In N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, & J. Roessler (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 85–109). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Reddy, V. (2009). Before the “third element”: Understanding attention to self. In N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, & J. Roessler (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 85–109). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Reissland, N., Francis, B., Aydin, E., Mason, J., & Schaal, B. (2014). The development of anticipation in the fetus: A longitudinal account of human fetal mouth movements in reaction to and anticipation of touch. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(5), 955–963.CrossRef Reissland, N., Francis, B., Aydin, E., Mason, J., & Schaal, B. (2014). The development of anticipation in the fetus: A longitudinal account of human fetal mouth movements in reaction to and anticipation of touch. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(5), 955–963.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rochat, P. (2001). The infant’s world. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Rochat, P. (2001). The infant’s world. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Rochat, P., & Hespos, S. J. (1997). Differential rooting response by neonates: Evidence for an early sense of self. Early Development and Parenting, 6(3–4), 105–112.CrossRef Rochat, P., & Hespos, S. J. (1997). Differential rooting response by neonates: Evidence for an early sense of self. Early Development and Parenting, 6(3–4), 105–112.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. B. (2009). Understanding the role of communicative intentions in word learning. In N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, & J. Roessler (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 165–184). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. B. (2009). Understanding the role of communicative intentions in word learning. In N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, & J. Roessler (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 165–184). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Sartre, J.-P. (1966). Being and nothingness (H. E. Barnes, Trans.). New York: Washington Square Press. Sartre, J.-P. (1966). Being and nothingness (H. E. Barnes, Trans.). New York: Washington Square Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Simpson, E. A., Murray, L., Paukner, A., & Ferrari, P. F. (2014). The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: Presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369(1644), 20130289.CrossRef Simpson, E. A., Murray, L., Paukner, A., & Ferrari, P. F. (2014). The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: Presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369(1644), 20130289.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sloman, A., & Chrisley, R. (2003). Virtual machines and consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(4–5), 133–172. Sloman, A., & Chrisley, R. (2003). Virtual machines and consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(4–5), 133–172.
Zurück zum Zitat Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books. Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.
Zurück zum Zitat Strawson, P. F. (1959). Individuals: An essay in descriptive metaphysics. London: Methuen. Strawson, P. F. (1959). Individuals: An essay in descriptive metaphysics. London: Methuen.
Zurück zum Zitat Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based approach to child language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based approach to child language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRef Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44(2), 112–119.CrossRef Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44(2), 112–119.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Walton, G. E., & Bower, T. G. R. (1993). Newborns form “prototypes” in less than 1 minute. Psychological Science, 4(3), 203–205.CrossRef Walton, G. E., & Bower, T. G. R. (1993). Newborns form “prototypes” in less than 1 minute. Psychological Science, 4(3), 203–205.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wilson, A., & Weinstein, L. (1990). Language, thought, and interiorization. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 26(1), 24.CrossRef Wilson, A., & Weinstein, L. (1990). Language, thought, and interiorization. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 26(1), 24.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Winnicott, D. W. (1965). The maturational processes and the facilitating environment: Studies in the theory of emotional development. London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psycho-Analysis. Winnicott, D. W. (1965). The maturational processes and the facilitating environment: Studies in the theory of emotional development. London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psycho-Analysis.
Zurück zum Zitat Zahavi, D. (1999). Self-awareness and alterity. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. Zahavi, D. (1999). Self-awareness and alterity. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Zahavi, D. (2014). Self and other: Exploring subjectivity, empathy, and shame. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Zahavi, D. (2014). Self and other: Exploring subjectivity, empathy, and shame. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Cogitor Ergo Sum: The Origin of Self-awareness in Dyadic Interaction
verfasst von
Stephen Langfur
Publikationsdatum
11.12.2018
Verlag
Springer Netherlands
Erschienen in
Human Studies / Ausgabe 3/2019
Print ISSN: 0163-8548
Elektronische ISSN: 1572-851X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10746-018-09487-y

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2019

Human Studies 3/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Theoretical / Philosophical Paper

Self-Stigma, Bad Faith and the Experiential Self