Sexual and gender minority (SGM) is an umbrella term capturing populations
who identify as gay, lesbian, bisexual, transgender, queer, intersex, and other
nonnormative identities that are highlighted by the use of the “ + ” sign (LGBTQI+). SGM
individuals often experience sexual- and gender-based prejudices, or rather negative
attitudes, due to their nonadherence to societal norms in terms of sexual orientation
and/or gender identity (e.g., Herek, 2004;
Hill, 2003). These negative attitudes
toward SGM individuals rely on a sociocultural system defined as heterosexism (Herek,
1990), which is described as an
implicit set of beliefs that considers heterosexuality and cisgender identity as natural
and normal, entailing negative feelings and attitudes toward sexual and gender
minorities.
Notwithstanding, the ways in which heterosexism can manifest itself are
diverse and can range from more violent and hostile expressions to more benevolent and
less aggressive attitudes, and this difference can depend on various social conditions.
Walls (2008) developed the Multidimensional
Heterosexism Inventory (MHI) to assess the different manifestations and dimensions of
heterosexism, and to our knowledge, the MHI is currently the only scale that can
comprehensively assess the different nuances of heterosexist attitudes, including the
more subtle and stereotypically benevolent ones.
Advertisement
The current study is aimed at validating an Italian version of the MHI
(Walls, 2008) in a sample of Italian
cisgender heterosexual adults. In the following sections, we provide a comprehensive
definition of the main psychosocial construct of the study (heterosexism), discussing
its relations with other crucial related constructs, i.e., sexism, authoritarianism, and
social dominance orientation (SDO). Then, we provide a brief presentation of the
construct of benevolent heterosexism—as outlined in the work of Walls (2008)—moving from the construct of benevolent sexism
and showing its theoretical and empirical significance for psychosocial research with
SGM.
Sexism, Authoritarianism, Social Dominance Orientation
The characteristics and manifestations of homonegativity in Western
societies have undergone considerable change in recent decades, even though the
ideological system that feeds them, namely, heterosexism, appears relatively stable
in its basic assumptions. Heterosexism has been originally defined by Herek
(1990) as “an ideological system
that denies, denigrates, and stigmatizes any nonheterosexual form of behavior,
identity, relationship, or community” (p. 316). As an attitude or prejudice that can
serve different psychological functions for different people, heterosexism is
present in various individuals’ attitudes and actions, is influenced by various
psychological (i.e., cognitive, motivational, behavioral) and situational factors,
and can result in violence against SGM (Herek, 1992, 1995).
Accordingly, like other ideologies of oppression such as racism and sexism,
heterosexism can be regarded through a cultural lens, insofar as it is manifested in
social norms and institutions (e.g., religion and the legal system), or through a
psychological lens, as it is exhibited in individuals’ attitudes and behavior. In
this sense, heterosexism is a measure of aversive or negative attitudes toward SGM,
and as such it has been mainly employed in the scales measuring homo-/trans-negative
prejudice and stigma.
Numerous empirical and theoretical studies have emphasized the strong
association between sexism and heterosexism (Bochicchio et al., 2020; Brett et al., 2023; Kilianski, 2003; Scandurra et al., 2019a). Both sexism and heterosexism refer to negative attitudes,
beliefs, and types of behavior that are aimed at devaluing, denigrating,
stigmatizing, or restricting women and female-related characteristics on the one
hand and LGBTQI+ persons or nonheterosexual forms of behavior on the other
(Szymanski & Moffitt, 2012).
Furthermore, both can be expressed on an individual, familial, institutional, and
sociocultural level. Whereas sexism includes thinking that women are inferior to
men, endorsing behaviors such as workplace sexual harassment, gender inequity in
salaries, and media depictions of women that emphasize their body parts and sexual
readiness, heterosexism includes believing that LGBTQI+ persons are disgusting,
deserving rejection and condemnation, and denying rights, protections, and benefits
associated with marriage (Szymanski & Carr, 2008) or civil unions (Scandurra et al., 2019b) to same-sex couples. Both sexism and
heterosexism are often associated with right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) and SDO in
research scholarship (e.g., Christopher & Wojda, 2008; Rollero et al., 2021; Sibley et al., 2007). Indeed, findings from the literature show that RWA is
associated with prejudice toward gay individuals (Crawford et al., 2016; Stefurak et al., 2010) and that SDO is similarly related to both
old-fashioned and modern heterosexism (Eldridge & Johnson, 2011). Even though originally conceptualized as
an intra-group and an intergroup phenomenon, respectively (Altemeyer, 1998; Pratto et al., 1994), RWA and SDO both refer to attitudes
supporting group-based hierarchy and inequality (Lehmiller & Schmitt,
2007). Accordingly, individuals
embracing higher levels of SDO tend to feel threatened by progresses toward group
equality and are thus more prone to exhibit high levels of RWA. Finally, both
hostile and benevolent sexism are associated with hierarchical beliefs like those
supporting RWA- and SDO-related attitudes (Lee, 2013).
Altemeyer (1998) originally
regarded RWA as a fixed personality construct comprising three general
characteristics, namely, authoritarian submission, conventionalism, and
authoritarian aggression. More recently, Jugert and Duckitt (2009) viewed these three dimensions as
expressing specific motivational goals or values. RWA has been variously
conceptualized as a latent, context-specific disposition (Feldman, 2003), as a general attitude comprising
dogmatism, conformist behaviors, punitiveness toward real or perceived adversaries,
and a strong desire for social hierarchy (Manson, 2020), and as a group phenomenon that tends to be associated with
negative opinions toward marginalized groups (Stellmacher & Petzel, 2005). Therefore, RWA-related norms are formed
on the basis of both the individual’s personality and socially informed worldview
beliefs. Furthermore, as RWA-related behaviors tend to increase along with societal
threats (Bochicchio et al., 2021;
Mezzalira et al., 2023b) and since
threat hinders perceived control of events, RWA seems to increase with lower
perceived control, leading individuals to support social ingroups against the
outgroups (Fritsche et al., 2011). As
another type of ideological orientation, SDO reflects the endorsement of a
hierarchical order of social groups within society (Pratto et al., 1994). Individuals high in SDO tend to be
reluctant to distribute resources and give power to subordinate groups (Halkjelsvik
& Rise, 2014). Accordingly, SDO
reflects the individuals’ belief that social groups are not equal, resulting in the
belief in one group’s superiority over other groups. SDO has been found to be a
predictor of hostile sexism toward women and tolerance of sexual harassment as well
(Feather & McKee, 2012; Russell
& Trigg, 2004; Sibley et al.,
2007).
Advertisement
At the end of the twentieth century, the concept of
sexism—traditionally encompassing hostility toward women and their confinement to
roles that accorded them a lower status and power than men—has been disputed within
the existing scholarship. Glick and Fiske (1996, 1997) proposed
that the concept of sexism is intrinsically ambivalent, insofar as it subjectively involves both benevolent and
hostile feelings toward women. Hostile and benevolent sexism are two sexist
ideologies based on male structural power in society and female “dyadic power,”
namely, a power stemming from dependency in interpersonal relationships (Guttentag
& Secord, 1983). Whereas hostile
sexism is intended to justify male power, traditional gender roles, and men’s
exploitation of women as sexual objects, benevolent sexism is based on gentler
justifications of male power and dominance and prescribed rigid gender roles (Glick
& Fiske, 1996, 1997). Accordingly, benevolent sexism is an
attitude perceived as positive for the sexist, in that it recognizes male dependence
on women (women’s dyadic power) and is based on a romanticized conceptualization of
sexual relationships with women, thus involving feelings of protectiveness and
affection toward them. The two forms of sexism (i.e., hostile and benevolent) share
three elements, namely, male power over women, gender differentiation (along with
the endorsement of traditional gender roles), and sexuality (viewed within a
patriarchal social structure) (Glick & Fiske, 1996, 1997).
In Italy, which is the context of this study, research confirms the
data reported in the international scientific literature regarding the associations
between sexism, heterosexism, RWA, and SDO (e.g., Callahan & Loscocco,
2023; Lingiardi et al.,
2016; Rollè et al., 2022; Scandurra et al., 2017, 2020; Trappolin, 2022). Indeed, various studies have confirmed that Italy is still
a highly heteronormative country, where sexual and gender minorities are strongly
discriminated due to their nonheterosexual orientation or gender nonconformity
(Baiocco et al., 2013; Bochicchio et
al., 2019; Lingiardi et al.,
2016; Mezza et al., 2023; Scandurra et al., 2020). Furthermore, Italy lacks a specific
legislation protecting gender and sexual minorities from homo- or trans-phobic hate
crimes, thus exposing these individuals to negative outcomes in terms of mental
health and well-being (Hatzenbuehler, 2010, 2014), also
due to the gender pressure they are subject to in their everyday life (Egan &
Perry, 2001; Mezzalira et al.,
2023a).
Benevolent Heterosexism and the Tool for Assessing It: The Multidimensional Heterosexism Inventory (MHI)
The strong association between sexism and heterosexism has been
theoretically well explained by Kilianski (2003), who proposed the theory of exclusively masculine identity.
The author argued that the assumption of a stereotypical male hetero-cisgender
identity can entail bias and hostility toward all individuals that are not
hetero/cisgender males, namely, cisgender women and lesbian/gay individuals, as well
as transgender persons. This means that sexism and heterosexism stem from a common
root, namely, the exclusively masculine identity, and can share common forms of
expression and manifestation. Just as sexism expresses an ambivalent dimensionality
ranging from overt hostility toward women to benevolent and paternalistic—and,
therefore, devaluing—attitudes (Glick & Fiske, 1996), so heterosexism can be described as a multidimensional
construct as well.
Assuming that heterosexism can also express a benevolent and
paternalistic dimension, Walls (2008)
theorized heterosexism as a multidimensional construct and proposed to capture its
dimensions through the Multidimensional Heterosexism Inventory (MHI). According to
the multidimensional conceptualization of heterosexism, the latter can be divided
into four subdomains: (1) paternalistic
heterosexism, defined as “subjectively neutral or positive attitudes,
myths and beliefs that express concern for the physical, emotional or cognitive
well-being of nonheterosexual persons while concurrently denying, denigrating,
stigmatizing and/or segregating any nonheterosexual form of behavior, identity,
relationship, or community” (Walls, 2008, pp. 27–28); (2) positive
stereotypic heterosexism, including “subjectively positive attitudes,
myths and beliefs that express appreciation of stereotypic characteristics often
attributed to lesbians and gay men which function by denying, denigrating,
stigmatizing and/or segregating any nonheterosexual form of behavior, identity,
relationship, or community” (Walls, 2008, p. 28); (3) aversive
heterosexism, defined as “attitudes, myths, and beliefs that dismiss,
belittle, or disregard the impact of sexual orientation on life chances by denying,
denigrating, stigmatizing and/or segregating any nonheterosexual form of behavior,
identity, relationship, or community” (Walls, 2008, p. 46); and (4) amnestic
heterosexism, that is “attitudes, myths and beliefs that deny the
impact of sexual orientation on life chances by denying, denigrating, stigmatizing
and/or segregating any nonheterosexual form of behavior, identity, relationship, or
community” (Walls, 2008, pp.
46–47).
The MHI, therefore, is proposed as a tool to assess the different forms
of heterosexism in those contexts where the openly hostile homonegativity is
discouraged, contrasted, or even punished, like Italy and some other Western
countries. The presence of heterosexist beliefs and attitudes, in those contexts,
can indeed take a benevolent form, like the paternalistic and the positive
stereotypic heterosexism, that can still represent a bias fostering discrimination,
inequalities, and prejudice toward lesbians, gays, and bisexuals.
In the last decades, indeed, a vast array of research literature has
been based on or mentioned the MHI for research purposes. For instance, Katz et al.
(2019) found that endorsing
amnestic heterosexist attitudes reduces feelings of personal responsibility to
address anti-gay bullying. Van der Toorn et al. (2020) showed that threat reactions to nonheteronormative behavior
reinforced the individual’s heteronormative beliefs. In addition to being included
as a measure of attitudes toward gay men (Grey et al., 2013) and more generally toward sexual
orientation (Ryan & Blascovich, 2015), the MHI (Walls, 2008) has been also quoted among the measures of affirmation and
discrimination in LGBTQI+ individuals (e.g., Peterson et al., 2017). Seelman and Walls (2010) found that higher levels of perceived
incongruence with social work values in a graduate program were associated with
significantly higher levels of hostile, aversive, and paternalistic heterosexism, as
well as with higher levels of RWA and SDO. Henry et al. (2022) demonstrated that religious individuals
endorsing fundamentalist belief systems exhibit higher levels of heterosexism.
Heterosexist attitudes using Walls’s (2008) multidimensional construct have been widely used in
educational and academic settings (Clarke, 2019; Gredig & Bartelsen-Raemy, 2021; Katz & Federici, 2018; López-Sáez et al., 2020). Lastly, the multidimensional nature of
heterosexism according to Stones and Glazzard (2019) has been addressed along with the minority stress theory
proposed by Ilan Meyer (2003) and, more
generally, in its relationship with anti-LGB microaggressions (Nadal et al.,
2016; Platt & Lenzen,
2013; Spengler et al., 2016; Vishwanathan, 2022).
The literature thus points to the significance of the MHI in the
research of prejudice and discrimination against lesbians, gays, and bisexual people
based on the various facets of heterosexism. Therefore, our study is aimed at
validating an Italian version of the MHI as proposed by Walls (2008). This is particularly important since
Italy is not exempt from heterosexist attitudes that can foster serious harm to SGM,
and our research can provide a useful instrument to assess the levels of
heterosexism, and particularly the benevolent heterosexism, in the Italian
population.
The Current Study
The current study is aimed at assessing the psychometric
characteristics of the Italian version of the MHI in a national sample of Italian
people by evaluating the model fit and different validities (i.e., predictive,
convergent, and discriminant). Specifically, we hypothesized that (1) four subscales
of the MHI have good fit indices in the Italian sample (i.e., model fit; Hypothesis
1); (2) social dominance orientation (SDO) and right-wing authoritarianism (RWA)
will correlate positively with each of the subscales of the MHI (i.e., predictive
validity; Hypothesis 2); (3) each of the subscales of the MHI will correlate
positively with attitudes toward lesbians and gay men and ambivalent sexism (i.e.,
convergent validity; Hypothesis 3); (4) following Kazdin’s (2003) recommendations on the conceptual
distinction between constructs, correlations between MHI and all other scales will
be below 0.60 (i.e., discriminant validity; Hypothesis 4).
The literature has historically shown that men exhibit more negative
attitudes than women toward homosexuality (Kite & Whitley, 1996). In a more recent study, Montgomery and
Stewart (2012) found that women scored
higher than men on both heterosexual privilege awareness and resistance to
heteronormativity, and both these variables were associated with engagement in gay
rights activism. Other studies confirmed that greater biases exist against sexual
minorities among men compared to women (e.g., Andersen & Fetner, 2008; Mange & Lepastourel, 2013). Furthermore, Walls (2008) found that having any LGBTQI+ friend (as
opposed to the number of such friends) was associated with higher levels of
paternalistic heterosexism and that seculars were lower in apathetic heterosexism
than conservative Protestants. Finally, Habarth et al. (2020) found that higher education was associated
with lower heteronormativity among women.
Based on this literature, in addition to the hypotheses concerning the
psychometric characteristics of the MHI, we also hypothesized that men, less
educated participants, religious people, and those not having LGBTQI+ relatives and
friends are generally higher in all subscales of MHI than their counterparts.
Method
Procedures
Translation of the MHI
The MHI was translated into Italian through the
back-translation method (Behling & Law, 2000). Thus, 5 steps were implemented, as follows: (1)
items were independently translated from English into Italian by three
experts in the fields of gender studies and psychology by obtaining 3
Italian versions of the scale; (2) these versions were then compared to
reach an agreement on a final unique Italian version; (3) this last Italian
version was then translated into English by a native speaker with an
excellent proficiency in the Italian language; (4) this new English version
of the MHI was compared with the original English version and no substantial
differences were found; and (5) three Italian researchers expert in gender
studies and psychology participated in an online survey to evaluate
independently the contents and comprehensibility of each item of the Italian
version of the MHI, by answering the question “How clear are the contents of
the following items?” Raters had to answer on a five-point Likert scale,
from 1 (“not at all clear”) to 5 (“completely clear”). The average of all
items was 4.81. The Italian version of the MHI is reported in the
Appendix.
Survey Procedures
The data were collected through a web-based survey inserted in
Google. Participants were recruited via internet in Italy between May 2022
and October 2022. To ensure wide and diverse participation, we strategically
utilized popular social networks such as Facebook and Instagram as
dissemination platforms. This approach enabled us to reach a good community
sample comprising individuals from various backgrounds and demographics,
comparable in size to the sample used in Walls’ (2008) first study. In recruiting
participants, our goal was to engage individuals with diverse
characteristics, aiming for a balanced representation of both progressive
and conservative groups. To achieve this, we reached out to administrators
of specific online groups, requesting them to circulate the survey among
their members. Additionally, we implemented a snowball sampling procedure,
encouraging all potentially interested participants to share the survey
within their personal networks.
In the advertisements, it was specified that we were looking for
people who (1) were aged ≥ 18 years, the Italian age of consent; (2) spoke
Italian; (3) have been living in Italy at least in the last 10 years; and (4)
were heterosexual and cisgender (i.e., not LGBTQI+). Participants were reached
through a snowball sampling procedure, asking them to share the survey with
their contacts.
By clicking on the link provided, participants could read the
informed consent form, the objectives, information about the researchers, and
benefits and risks of the study. They were informed about the anonymity of the
survey, as well as about their right to withdraw from it if needed. All
questions were mandatory to avoid missing data.
The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the
University of Calabria (date of approval: March 3, 2022), designed in respect of
the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki, and conducted in accordance with
the EU General Data Protection Regulation.
Participants
A total of 201 individuals (129 women and 72 men) participated in
the survey. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 81 years (M = 36.42, SD = 12.56) and most of them had an educational level ≥ college
(n = 145; 72.1%). Most participants
declared to not have LGBTQI+ relatives (n = 152; 75.6%), but to have LGBTQI+ friends (n = 161; 80.1%), and were not religious (n = 124; 61.7%).
Measures
Sociodemographic Characteristics
The sociodemographic characteristics assessed in the current
study included gender (women, men, and other), age, level of education
(1 ≤ high school; 2 ≥ college), having LGBTQI+ relatives (yes vs. no) or
friends (yes vs. no), and being religious (yes vs. no).
Multidimensional Heterosexism
The MHI (Walls, 2008) is a 23-item questionnaire assessing 4 dimensions
of heterosexism on a 7-point Likert scale, from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 7
(“strongly agree”). For the first 7 items only, the Likert scale provides in
addition the option 0, indicating “I am OK with a gay son/lesbian daughter.”
The measure consists of 4 subscales, as follows: (1) aversive heterosexism, containing items such as “Gay men
should stop shoving their lifestyle down everyone’s throat”; (2) amnestic heterosexism, containing items such as
“Discrimination against lesbians is virtually nonexistent in today’s
society”; (3) paternalistic heterosexism,
containing items such as “I would prefer my daughter NOT be a homosexual
because she would face unfair discrimination”; (4) positive stereotypic heterosexism, containing items such as
“Lesbians are better than heterosexual women at physically defending
themselves.” Scoring and statistical information of the scale are reported
in the Appendix, where the Italian
version is also included.
Social Dominance Orientation
Social dominance attitudes were assessed using the Social Dominance Orientation Scale (SDO; Pratto
et al., 1994) in its short
version proposed by Ho et al. (2015). The short version of the SDO is an 8-item scale
intended to measure “the extent to which one desires that one’s in-group
dominate and be superior to outgroups” (Pratto et al., 1994, p. 742). Response options ranged
from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 7 (“strongly agree”). The SDO is subdivided
into four domains: (1) pro-trait
dominance (example item: “Some groups of people are simply
inferior to other groups”); (2) con-trait
dominance (example item: “No one group should dominate in
society”); (3) pro-trait
antiegalitarianism (example item: “It is unjust to try to
make groups equal”); (4) con-trait
antiegalitarianism (example item: “We should do what we can
to equalize conditions for different groups”) (Ho et al., 2015). The alpha coefficient in the
current sample was 0.72.
Right Wing Authoritarianism (RWA)
RWA was assessed through the 15-item version of the Right-Wing Authoritarianism Scale (Altemeyer,
1998; Giampaglia &
Roccato, 2002), a scale
assessing the degree to which people adhere to established authorities,
exhibit aggression toward outgroups based on authorities sanctioning that
aggression, and support traditional values endorsed by authorities (Saunders
& Ngo, 2017). Response
options ranged from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 4 (“strongly agree”), with
higher scores indicating greater RWA. An example item is “The only way our
country can get through the crisis ahead is to get back to our traditional
values, put some tough leaders in power, and silence the troublemakers
spreading bad ideas.” The alpha coefficient in the current sample was
0.82.
Sexism
Sexism was assessed through the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (ASI; Glick & Fiske,
1996; Rattazzi et al.,
2008), a 22-item scale
measuring sexist attitudes and feelings on a 6-point Likert scale, from 0
(“disagree strongly”) to 5 (“agree strongly”). The ASI consists of two
subscales: (1) hostile sexism, which
assesses the extent to which people endorse negative stereotypes of women
rejecting traditional female roles and behaviors (example item: “Women
exaggerate problems they have at work”), and (2) benevolent sexism, which assesses positive feelings about
stereotypes associated with women who embrace traditional female roles
(example item: “A good woman should be set on a pedestal by her man”).
Higher scores on both subscales indicate greater sexism. The alpha
coefficient in the current sample was 0.94 for hostile sexism and 0.90 for
benevolent sexism.
Attitudes Toward Lesbian and Gay People
Negative attitudes toward lesbian and gay people (i.e.,
heterosexism intended as a unifactorial construct) were assessed through the
short version of the Attitudes toward Lesbians and
Gay Men (ATLG) scale (Herek, 1988), a 10-item scale measuring individuals’ attitudes
toward SGM people. Response options ranged from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 9
(“strongly agree”), with higher scores indicating greater heterosexism. An
example item is “Male homosexuality is a perversion”. The alpha coefficient
was 0.71.
Statistical Analyses
To test Hypothesis 1 (i.e., model fit of the MHI), a confirmatory
factor analysis (CFA) with Version 2.3.0. of Jamovi using robust weighted least
square estimation was performed. Specifically, we evaluated model fit by
extracting the estimated loadings and evaluating the following indices: chi
square/degrees of freedom (χ2/df), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA),
standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), comparative fit index (CFI), and
Tucker-Lewis index (TLI). We followed the recommendations by Kline (1998), according to whom values of χ2/df < 2, RMSEA and SRMR < 0.08, TLI and
CFI > 0.95 can be considered indicative values of a good fit with the data.
Furthermore, the internal consistency reliability of the four subscales of the
MHI was assessed through Cronbach’s alpha.
To test the hypotheses from 2 to 4 (i.e., predictive, convergent,
and discriminant validity), a series of correlations between MHI subscales,
social dominance orientation, RWA, attitudes toward lesbians and gay men, and
ambivalent sexism was conducted using the Pearson’s correlation
coefficient.
Lastly, to test potential MHI differences concerning
sociodemographic factors (i.e., gender, level of education, having
LGBTQI+ relatives and friends, and religiousness), independent samples t-tests were performed.
Results
Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA)
The original 4-factor model proposed by Walls (2008) demonstrated an adequate fit with the
data obtained from the Italian sample, confirming our first hypothesis.
Specifically, the following indices were found: χ2/df = 1.38, RMSEA = 0.039 (confidence intervals [CI] = 0.030,
0.048), SRMR = 0.068, CFI = 0.961, and TLI = 0.942. Internal consistency
reliability was adequate for each subscale as Cronbach’s alpha ranged from 0.78
to 0.96. Full model statistics (i.e., Cronbach’s alphas, range, mean, standard
deviations, standardized factor loadings, and standard error) are reported in
Table 1.
Table 1
Confirmatory factor analysis of Multidimensional
Heterosexism Inventory
Scale | Alpha | Range | Total score: M (SD) |
---|---|---|---|
Paternalistic heterosexism | 0.94 | 0–7 | 1.25 (1.79) |
Item | Factor loading (SE) | ||
MHI 1 | 0.82 (0.12) | ||
MHI 2 | 0.78 (0.14) | ||
MHI 3 | 0.83 (0.10) | ||
MHI 4 | 0.80 (0.10) | ||
MHI 5 | 0.87 (0.11) | ||
MHI 6 | 0.91 (0.11) | ||
MHI 7 | 0.81 (0.14) | ||
Aversive heterosexism | 0.96 | 0–7 | 1.99 (1.62) |
Item | Factor loading (SE) | ||
MHI 8 | 0.88 (0.11) | ||
MHI 9 | 0.95 (0.08) | ||
MHI 10 | 0.90 (0.08) | ||
MHI 11 | 0.90 (0.10) | ||
MHI 12 | 0.94 (0.09) | ||
MHI 13 | 0.81 (0.10) | ||
Amnestic heterosexism | 0.78 | 0–7 | 1.95 (1.09) |
Item | Factor loading (SE) | ||
MHI 14 | 0.61 (0.09) | ||
MHI 15 | 0.83 (0.10) | ||
MHI 16 | 0.75 (0.09) | ||
MHI 17 | 0.62 (0.08) | ||
Positive stereotypic heterosexism | 0.88 | 0–7 | 1.99 (1.23) |
Item | Factor loading (SE) | ||
MHI 18 | 0.92 (0.06) | ||
MHI 19 | 0.95 (0.06) | ||
MHI 20 | 0.89 (0.07) | ||
MHI 21 | 0.57 (0.12) | ||
MHI 22 | 0.52 (0.14) | ||
MHI 23 | 0.73 (0.09) |
Predictive, Convergent, and Discriminant Validity of the MHI
Correlational analyses for hypotheses 2 to 4 are reported in
Table 2. Regarding the predictive
validity of the MHI, both SDO and RWA correlated positively with each of the
subscales of the MHI, confirming Hypothesis 2. Similarly, regarding the
convergent validity of the MHI, each of the subscales of the MHI correlated
positively with negative attitudes toward lesbians and gay men, hostile sexism,
and benevolent sexism, confirming Hypothesis 3.
Table 2
Correlations between Multidimensional Heterosexism
Inventory, Social Dominance Orientation Scale, Ambivalent Sexism
Inventory, right-wing authoritarianism, and attitudes toward
lesbians and gay men
Scales | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. MHI—paternalistic | − | |||||||
2. MHI—aversive | 0.47*** | − | ||||||
3. MHI—amnestic | 0.15* | 48*** | − | |||||
4. MHI—positive stereotypic | 0.44*** | 56*** | 41*** | − | ||||
5. Social dominance orientation | 0.28*** | 44*** | 30*** | 39*** | − | |||
6. Ambivalent hostile sexism | 32*** | 69*** | 43*** | 53*** | 32*** | − | ||
7. Ambivalent benevolent sexism | 33*** | 68*** | 45*** | 58*** | 47*** | 78*** | − | |
8. RWA | 33*** | 62*** | 34*** | 38*** | 60*** | 54*** | 70*** | − |
9. ATLG | 21** | 53*** | 30*** | 36*** | 56*** | 36*** | 46*** | 61*** |
Finally, regarding the discriminant validity of the MHI, Hypothesis
4 was partially confirmed, as not all correlations related to hypotheses 2 and 3
were below 0.60. Indeed, the subscale of the MHI concerning the aversive
heterosexism showed correlation coefficients > 0.60 with hostile sexism,
benevolent sexism, and RWA, thus showing a relatively high overlap between
constructs.
Sociodemographic Differences in MHI Dimensions
The Student t-test was performed
to compare potential differences between MHI dimensions based on gender identity
and demonstrated that men presented higher levels of aversive and amnestic
heterosexism, as well as general multidimensional heterosexism, than women. On
the contrary, no gender differences were found with respect to either
paternalistic and positive stereotypic heterosexism.
Results concerning levels of education, having LGBTQI+ friends, and
religiousness were more robust. Indeed, with the exception of the absence of
difference in means on paternalistic heterosexism regarding educational level,
all other differences resulted statistically significant. Specifically, less
educated participants, those with no LGBTQI+friends, and religious people were
higher in all MHI subscales than their counterparts. On the contrary, no
differences between participants with LGBTQI+ relatives and participants without
LGBTQI+ relatives were found on any of the subscales. All results are reported
in Table 3.
Table 3
Independent sample t-test concerning gender identity, level of
education, LGBTQI+ relatives, LGBTQI+ friends, and religiousness
on MHI dimensions
M (SD) | t | p | 95%
CI | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | |||||
Men (n = 72) | Women (n = 129) | ||||
MHI—paternalistic | 1.45 (2.04) | 1.14 (1.63) | −1.16 | 0.247 | −0.82, 0.21 |
MHI—aversive | 2.50 (1.91) | 1.72 (1.36) | −3.35 | 0.001 | −1.24, −0.32 |
MHI—amnestic | 2.24 (1.18) | 1.79 (.99) | −2.86 | 0.005 | −0.76, −0.14 |
MHI—positive stereotypic | 2.09 (1.46) | 1.93 (1.09) | −0.89 | 0.376 | −0.52, 0.20 |
MHI—tot | 2.07 (1.33) | 1.65 (0.89) | −2.69 | 0.008 | −0.73, −0.11 |
Level of
education | |||||
≤ High school (n = 56) | ≥ College (n = 145) | ||||
MHI—paternalistic | 1.46 (2.01) | 1.17 (1.70) | 1.03 | 0.303 | −0.26, 0.84 |
MHI—aversive | 2.84 (1.97) | 1.67 (1.33) | 4.86 | < 0.001 | 0.69, 1.65 |
MHI—amnestic | 2.25 (1.27) | 1.84 (0.99) | 2.46 | 0.015 | 0.08, 0.75 |
MHI—positive stereotypic | 2.36 (1.50) | 1.84 (1.8) | 2.73 | 0.007 | 0.14, 0.89 |
MHI—tot | 2.23 (1.35) | 1.63 (0.92) | 3.62 | < 0.001 | 0.27, 0.93 |
LGBTQI+ relatives | |||||
Yes (n = 49) | No (n = 152) | ||||
MHI—paternalistic | 1.05 (1.73) | 1.32 (1.81) | −0.93 | 0.352 | −0.85, 0.30 |
MHI—aversive | 2.03 (1.54) | 1.99 (1.65) | 0.18 | 0.860 | −0.48, 0.57 |
MHI—amnestic | 1.90 (1.25) | 1.97 (1.03) | −0.39 | 0.700 | −0.42, 0.28 |
MHI—positive stereotypic | 1.97 (1.17) | 1.99 (1.26) | −0.13 | 0.893 | −0.43, 0.37 |
MHI—tot | 1.73 (0.94) | 1.82 (1.13) | −0.45 | 0.651 | −0.43, 0.27 |
LGBTQI+ friends | |||||
Yes (n = 161) | No (n = 40) | ||||
MHI—paternalistic | 1.12 (1.75) | 1.79 (1.88) | −2.14 | 0.033 | −1.29, −0.05 |
MHI—aversive | 1.69 (1.35) | 3.22 (2.01) | −5.77 | < 0.001 | −2.05, −1.01 |
MHI—amnestic | 1.85 (1.01) | 2.39 (1.28) | −2.91 | 0.004 | −0.92, −0.17 |
MHI—positive stereotypic | 1.83 (1.11) | 2.62 (1.49) | −3.77 | < 0.001 | −1.21, −0.38 |
MHI—tot | 1.62 (0.95) | 2.51 (1.31) | −4.88 | < 0.001 | −1.24, −0.53 |
Religiousness | |||||
Yes (n = 77) | No (n = 24) | ||||
MHI—paternalistic | 1.59 (1.90) | 1.04 (1.69) | 2.15 | 0.033 | 0.04, 1.06 |
MHI—aversive | 2.64 (1.82) | 1.59 (1.34) | 4.69 | < 0.001 | 0.61, 1.49 |
MHI—amnestic | 2.21 (0.97) | 1.79 (1.13) | 2.64 | 0.009 | 0.01, 0.71 |
MHI—positive stereotypic | 2.35 (1.42) | 1.76 (1.04) | 3.42 | 0.001 | 0.25, 0.94 |
MHI—tot | 2.21 (1.24) | 1.54 (0.90) | 4.32 | < 0.001 | 0.35, 0.95 |
Discussion
The current study is aimed at assessing the psychometric properties of
the MHI in a sample of Italian cisgender and heterosexual individuals. The results
obtained by CFA showed a good fit to the data, confirming the original 4-factor
model of the scale. Moreover, the results showed that the MHI has adequate
predictive and convergent validity in the recruited Italian sample, but that
discriminant validity is not fully achieved due to overlaps of multidimensional
heterosexism with other theoretical constructs (i.e., hostile sexism, benevolent
sexism, and RWA). Finally, results showed some significant gender differences with
respect to aversive and amnestic heterosexism, but not with respect to paternalistic
and positive stereotypic heterosexism. To our knowledge, this is the first available
measure to comprehensively assess the different dimensions and manifestations of
heterosexist attitudes and beliefs in an Italian context, and in particular, the MHI
is currently the only measure that can assess the extent of benevolent sexism in the
Italian population.
Regarding predictive validity, we found significant associations of all
MHI subscales with SDO and RWA. These results seem to confirm previous literature
that found that both RWA and political conservatism are associated with certain
negative attitudes toward same-sex sexuality (e.g., Whitley & Lee, 2000). Specifically, RWA has been shown to have
a consistently positive association with prejudice toward gay individuals (Stefurak
et al., 2010). As to the specific forms
of multidimensional heterosexism, Walls (2008) had already found that higher levels of conservative
political orientation predicted higher levels of apathetic heterosexism in an
earlier version of the MHI. Eldridge and Johnson (2011), in turn, found strong associations between SDO and
heterosexism (both old-fashioned and modern), likely because RWA and SDO refer to
beliefs that support group-based domination and inequality (Lehmiller & Schmitt,
2007) and group-based hierarchy
(Eldridge & Johnson, 2011). Indeed,
individuals with high SDO tend to feel threatened by advances toward group equality
and are therefore more predisposed to RWA. In short, our results are consistent with
the literature in showing that all four domains of heterosexism, as theorized by
Walls (2008), are associated with both
RWA and SDO.
Regarding convergent validity, we found significant associations of all
MHI subscales with negative attitudes toward lesbians and gay men, hostile sexism,
and benevolent sexism. Glick and Fiske (1996) conceptualized benevolent sexism as consisting of three
subcomponents: heterosexual intimacy (i.e., intimate relationships between men and
women), protective paternalism (i.e., an individual’s unelicited interference or
assistance in another person’s behavior or decision-making processes deemed
beneficial to that person), and gender differentiation (i.e., conventional norms and
beliefs that distinguish men and women according to their status differences).
Walls’s (2008) conceptualization of
paternalistic heterosexism and positive stereotypic heterosexism partially traces
back and mirrors these aspects of sexism; the author also found a “significant,
positive relationship between hostile sexism and amnestic heterosexism” (p. 52).
Although hostile and benevolent sexism are interdependent, complementary, and
cross-culturally prevalent ideologies (Glick & Fiske, 1996), they have also been considered as
distinct constructs (Lee, 2013). On the
one hand, hostile sexism is associated with behaviors such as sexual harassment
(Begany & Milburn, 2002). On the
other hand, benevolent sexism predicts paternalistic behaviors such as men’s
chivalry toward women (Viki et al., 2003). The two forms of sexism (hostile and benevolent) are not
only associated with higher levels of gender inequality (Glick et al., 2000), but are also positively associated with
general hierarchical beliefs as held by individuals who embrace SDO and RWA (Lee,
2013). Specifically, Lee
(2013) found that endorsement of
SDO more strongly predicted the endorsement of hostile sexism than that of
benevolent sexism, whereas the endorsement of RWA more strongly predicted
endorsement of benevolent sexism than that of hostile sexism. The rationale
underlying such difference lies in the different sexist ideologies supported by
different worldviews, as posited by Duckitt’s dual-process model (Duckitt,
2001; Duckitt & Sibley,
2017). Accordingly, individuals
high in SDO tend to endorse hostile sexism because they hold a “competitive”
worldview and thus support male dominance over women. Conversely, individuals high
in RWA tend to endorse benevolent sexism because they hold a “dangerous” worldview
and adhere to social traditions that value women who conform to gender norms.
On the other hand, discriminant validity was partially confirmed as the
aversive heterosexism subscale of the MHI was found to be overly associated with
hostile sexism and RWA, indicating some degree of overlap between these theoretical
constructs. In our view, the overlap between hostile sexism and aversive
heterosexism is unavoidable to some degree, as they have a common origin that has
been well explained theoretically by Kilianski (2003) through the theory of “exclusively masculine identity.”
According to this theory, all identities that deviate from the hetero-cisgender male
identity tend to generate aversion and hostility, which can be expressed at
individual, familial, social, and institutional levels (Szymanski & Moffitt,
2012). Indeed, defining aversive
heterosexism as a set of attitudes that dismiss or disregard the impact of sexual
orientation on life chances by denying, stigmatizing, or marginalizing any
nonheterosexual form of existence seems to incorporate the core definition of
ambivalent sexism, as it includes the endorsement of traditional gender roles (Glick
& Fiske, 1997).
We have also found that aversive heterosexism is strongly associated
with RWA, perhaps because those who fall more on the “authoritarian specter”
(Altemeyer, 1996) tend to perceive SGM
as an affront and threat to the social order and are therefore treated with
hostility and contempt. Indeed, recent research on the psychosocial impact of the
COVID-19 pandemic has shown that societal threats tend to increase the levels of RWA
(Bochicchio et al., 2021), which is
strongly associated with higher levels of aversion and hostility toward SGM (Pacilli
et al., 2022).
Lastly, accordingly to the previous literature, we found that MHI
subscales were generally higher in religious people (Cragun & Sumerau,
2015; Jäckle & Wenzelburger,
2015; Roggemans et al.,
2015), in less educated people
(e.g., Bickmore, 2002), and in those who
had no LGBTQI+ friends (e.g., Bochicchio et al., 2019). Interestingly, with respect to gender difference, we found
that only the subscales of aversive and amnestic heterosexism were higher in men if
compared to women, but not the subscales of paternalistic and positive stereotypic
heterosexism. Herek (1988) had already
shown that heterosexual men tend to have more negative attitudes toward SGM than
heterosexual women, and recent research on this topic tends to confirm that gender
difference affects attitudes toward homosexuality, with men being less accepting of
and more aversive toward same-sex sexuality than women (Hildebrandt & Jäckle,
2023). Nevertheless, our study
seems to refine the previous literature with regard to the different domains of
multidimensional heterosexism by showing that only the two forms of aversive
heterosexism and amnestic heterosexism are higher in men than in women. This finding
is particularly interesting because it represents a further element of convergence
between benevolent sexism and benevolent heterosexism. As Becker (2010) has shown, women tend to be more likely to
hold benevolent sexist attitudes than men because of the internalization of sexist
beliefs. Thus, it appears that benevolent expressions of sexual and gender
prejudice—both sexism and heterosexism—are more acceptable to women than hostile
ones. This may explain why the level of aversive heterosexism is lower for women,
but the level of benevolent heterosexism is not affected by gender
difference.
Limitations
Our study has significant limitations that should be considered.
The cross-sectional nature of the study did not allow a complete investigation
of the predictive and convergent validity of the Italian version of the MHI. In
addition, the nonprobabilistic nature of the sample limited the external
validity of our findings. Furthermore, the sample was relatively small, not
representative of the Italian population, and unbalanced in terms of gender (129
vs. 72 men). Future studies should consider examining multidimensional
heterosexism in a larger and gender-balanced sample and conduct a longitudinal
study design to reevaluate our conclusions in more diverse samples and assess
predictive validity through causal statistical analyses. In addition, it may be
extremely interesting to include SGM individuals in future samples, as in the
current study the sample consists exclusively of cisgender and heterosexual
individuals, because it would be useful to understand whether SGM individuals
hold benevolent heterosexist attitudes toward their own identity group as do
women with benevolent sexist beliefs. Indeed, while our study captured
heterosexist attitudes endorsed by heterosexual individuals, it is clear that
lesbian and gay people themselves can have heterosexist beliefs and behaviors
toward their own social minority group. In fact, lesbian and gay people are
embedded in the same society where heterosexual individuals live, and they
likely go through similar socialization processes. For this reason, it might be
of great value to test the MHI among nonheterosexual individuals, also to assess
group and individual differences among these partially different populations.
This might also help highlight the levels of internalized heterosexism among
nonheterosexual individuals (Amadio, 2006).
Social Policy Implications
Despite these limitations, the MHI could be considered an important
resource for Italian researchers who need to evaluate heterosexism taking into
account its multidimensional nuances. As we reported in the introduction of this
paper, heterosexism is rooted in social ideologies produced and maintained in
social institutions. Therefore, using the MHI can help researchers to assess not
only explicit and aversive forms of heterosexism that are often sanctioned by
legal systems (e.g., explicit workplace discrimination, hate speech, etc.), but
also benevolent and subtle forms of heterosexism that tend to insidiously and
implicitly perpetuate the heterosexist system, with significant consequences in
terms of health, disparities, and inequality among SGM people. The various
dimensions of heterosexism, maintained in the Italian validation of the MHI, can
help assess the various forms in which heterosexism can manifest itself in the
Italian population, thus aiding in shaping effective social policies that can
contrast the discrimination and marginalization of nonheterosexual individuals.
We agree with Hatzenbuehler (2010)
in considering social policies as fundamental in fostering social contexts that
can substantially compromise the mental health and well-being of the
nonheterosexual population. Accordingly, social policies can be regarded as
actual “health” policies, in that they can negatively impact and perpetuate
mental health problems among nonheterosexual individuals by exposing them to
specific minority stressors and rendering it difficult for them to access
health-enhancing resources. As a result, monitoring the levels of heterosexism
in the general population can be of great value to evaluate the social and
health policies that can be effectively put into effect to reduce the
stigmatization and discrimination of lesbian and gay people. Since the current
study is aimed at providing an essential tool to assess the levels of
heterosexism in the Italian population, it represents a resource that we deem as
valuable in order to achieve a deeper understanding of the phenomenon and its
social implications, thus helping foster actual policies aimed at reducing the
marginalization of nonheterosexual individuals.
Conclusions
Our study is aimed at validating the Italian version of the MHI and
also at assessing the associations between the different dimensions of heterosexism
and some related constructs, i.e. sexism, RWA, and SDO. The results suggest that
assessing the different nuances of heterosexism is crucial in contemporary society,
as the decline of old-fashioned aversive and hostile heterosexism may give way to
more subtle and implicit forms of prejudice, such as benevolent heterosexism in its
positive stereotypic and paternalistic dimensions. And as the case of benevolent
sexism has shown, this sociocultural process can lead to new forms of discrimination
and inequality against SMG individuals. Future research should better assess the
impact of benevolent heterosexism on health, well-being, and quality of life of SGM
people, also examining how sociocultural and political dynamics may predict the
development of heterosexist beliefs and attitudes.
Acknowledgements
The authors acknowledge the support of the COST Action “LGBTI+ Social and
Economic (in)equalities” (CA19103), funded by the European Union, as this research
falls within the scope of its activities.
Declarations
Ethics Approval
The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the University
of Calabria (date of approval: March 3, 2022), designed in respect of the
principles of the Declaration of Helsinki, and conducted in accordance with the
EU General Data Protection Regulation.
Consent to Participate
Participants were informed about the objectives of the survey,
completion times, benefits, and risks, as well as about the anonymity of the
responses and the right to stop the survey in any point and for any reasons.
Furthermore, participants were informed that the data collected would have been
published in scientific journals in aggregate form. After reading all
information, participants had to give their consent to participate in the online
survey by clicking on the bottom “I accept to take part in the survey.”
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare no competing interests.
Open Access This article is licensed
under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits
use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format,
as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were
made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in
the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit
line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative
Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation
or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affiliations.