Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Political Behavior 1/2020

24-08-2018 | Original Paper

Anxious Voters in the 2016 U.S. Election: An Analysis of How They Decided from the ERPC2016

Author: James E. Monogan III

Published in: Political Behavior | Issue 1/2020

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

How did anxiety influence vote choice in the 2016 U.S. presidential election, when both major candidates faced substantial voter disapproval? Drawing from the model of affective intelligence (Marcus et al. 2000), I argue that anxious voters will base their vote choice more strongly on issues and candidate qualities and less on partisanship relative to non-anxious voters. This study uses a registered report as part of the 2016 Election Research Preacceptance Competition, wherein a complete analysis plan was submitted for review prior to the 2016 American National Election Study’s release. In so doing, it serves as a model for other preregistered and potentially preaccepted research. Consistent with past findings, this study shows that anxious voters in 2016 were less likely to rely on past partisanship and more likely to base decisions on personal qualities. However, anxiety did not condition the effect of issues in the election. Further, Donald Trump faced no more anxiety among Republican voters than Mitt Romney did in 2012 due to a rise of in-party enthusiasm. The non-conditional effect of issues and a relatively narrow gap in partisan anxiety served as unique features of the 2016 election.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
Based on ABC News/Washington Post polls over more than 30 years, as reported by USA Today.
 
2
Figure A.5 in the online appendix shows results for the model presented in this paper, repeated for each presidential election from 1984-2016 using the American National Election Study cumulative data file. That figure shows interaction effects of anxiety with partisanship, issue proximity, and candidate personal qualities. As the figure there shows, the interaction term between partisanship and anxiety takes as sharp negative turn in 2012 and 2016 relative to 1984–2008.
 
4
For more information, see: http://​www.​erpc2016.​com/​.
 
5
The originally reported version of the 2012 model actually had a coding error in it due to a mistake in specifying a Boolean statement. Hence, the 2012 results reported now are different from what the reviewers originally saw. This correction was presented to the reviewers as a coding error from the preregistered design for their consideration, though the corrected version exactly fits the substantive specification from the preregistration. This illustrates how human errors can be addressed in registered reports.
 
6
Wallace (2016) cites a report that 92% of presidential campaign ads were either negative or contrast ads during the stretch from November 1–5, 2016 when 69,500 ads were observed on the air.
 
7
An alternative spatial model of politics is directional theory, which maintains that voters prefer candidates who are on the same side of an issue as they are and can be stimulated to support candidates taking strong positions on the issue (Rabinowitz and Macdonald 1989). In fact, Pardos-Prado and Dinas (2010) show that directional theory is particularly apt to explain voter behavior in polarized party systems, which certainly would characterize the United States in 2016. Regardless of whether the proximity or directional model best captures the effect of issues, either model would be more applicable among nervous voters who would be more prone to consider issues as part of their choice than complacent voters would. In fact, Table A.2 in the appendix replicates the main model for 2016, but replaces the proximity model of voter utility with the directional model. The results for the role of issues when conditioning on anxiety are similar.
 
8
For example, Alvarez and Nagler (1995) showed that in 1992 voters who felt the economy had declined under George H.W. Bush were less likely to vote for Bush and more likely to vote for Clinton (with independent Ross Perot seeing no real gain on this consideration).
 
9
Lewis-Beck et al. (2008) observe that candidate characteristics are more likely to influence vote choices among independent partisan leaners and weak partisans than they are for strong partisans. For this reason, the model of Table 4 was replicated on subsamples of independent leaners, weak partisans, and strong partisans. Much like prior findings, weak partisans were the most responsive to candidate qualities.
 
10
As an alternative to absolute distance, I estimated a model using squared Euclidean distance and present the results in appendix Table A.1. Also, when using an individual’s self placement and that same person’s placement of the candidates, a projection bias can emerge in which how a respondent places the candidates can reflect how the voter already feels about the candidates. To address this issue, I estimated a model in which the position of each candidate was fixed for all voters at the overall mean rating of the candidate, and issue distance was measured with the absolute distance between self-placement and the candidate mean. This model is presented in Table A.3. The results are substantively similar in these two alternative models.
 
11
For the 2016 Trump v. Clinton data, Cronbach’s \(\alpha\) on the six issue proximity scores was 0.93, indicating high reliability. Computed with the psych package version 1.7.8 in R 3.4.3.
 
12
Hope and proud were multiplied by -1 so that higher values would correspond to greater anxiety. Anger, afraid, and disgust were left on their original scale.
 
13
The Bayesian measurement model follows the same logic as confirmatory factor analysis (Bollen 1989).
 
14
Factor score estimates are posterior means of each respondent’s score, based on 150,000 MCMC samples after a 150,000 burn-in (3 chains of 50,000 each in both cases). There are 3854 factor scores, so it proved difficult to record all of the scores for as many iterations as the model parameters were recorded.
 
15
In the online appendix, Table A.7 presents an alternate version of the 2016 model that excludes disgust, for the sake of comparability with the 2012 model. The results of Appendix Table A.7 are substantively similar to those presented in Table 4 here in the main text.
 
16
A two-tailed difference-of-means test yielded \(t_{2129.2 df} = 7.7783\) (\(p < 0.0001\)).
 
17
\(t_{1520.8 df} = 3.5889\) (\(p = 0.0003\)).
 
18
\(t_{1703.5 df} = 2.5323\) (\(p = 0.0114\)).
 
19
In fact, the average dropped insignificantly from 0.526 to 0.520. \(t_{1588.2 df} = 0.6348\) (\(p = 0.5257\)).
 
20
Appendix Figs. A.1 and A.2 show the marginal density plot for each interaction term for the 2012 and 2016 models, respectively. The figures show more detail about these coefficients’ distributions and robustness.
 
21
Figures 3 and 4 show predicted probabilities for 2012 and 2016, respectively. The figures feature color but were designed to be grayscale friendly. The online appendix includes alternative versions, called Figs. A.3 and A.4, which rely heavily on color to present 90% credible intervals around the predicted probabilities. Figures A.3 and A.4 also differ in that, instead of using a standard deviation above and below the mean of anxiety, the predicted probabilities are computed for the minimum, mean, and maximum levels of anxiety—which is actually what the preregistered design calls for. The preregistered versions are online because the wider spread in anxiety reduces credible interval overlap, making the color-required figures easier to read.
 
22
Only if anxiety were to exceed a score of 0.988 would partisanship’s mean coefficient be negative on account of the negative interaction effect of anxiety with partisanship. However, 0.988 exceeds the maximum observed value of anxiety, so the effect is always positive. The other two interaction terms have positive mean coefficients, so the effects of issue proximity and candidate qualities certainly are always positive.
 
Literature
go back to reference Abramowitz, A. I. (2013). The polarized public? Why American government is so dysfunctional. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. Abramowitz, A. I. (2013). The polarized public? Why American government is so dysfunctional. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
go back to reference Alvarez, R. M., & Nagler, J. (1995). Economics, issues, & the Perot candidacy: Voter choice in the 1992 election. American Journal of Political Science, 39, 714–744.CrossRef Alvarez, R. M., & Nagler, J. (1995). Economics, issues, & the Perot candidacy: Voter choice in the 1992 election. American Journal of Political Science, 39, 714–744.CrossRef
go back to reference Ansolabehere, S., & Iyengar, S. (1995). Going negative: How political advertisements shrink and polarize the electorate. New York: Free Press. Ansolabehere, S., & Iyengar, S. (1995). Going negative: How political advertisements shrink and polarize the electorate. New York: Free Press.
go back to reference Asendorpf, J. B., Conner, M., de Fruyt, F., de Houwer, J., Denissen, J. J. A., Fiedler, K., et al. (2013). Recommendations for increasing replicability in psychology. European Journal of Personality, 27(2), 108–119.CrossRef Asendorpf, J. B., Conner, M., de Fruyt, F., de Houwer, J., Denissen, J. J. A., Fiedler, K., et al. (2013). Recommendations for increasing replicability in psychology. European Journal of Personality, 27(2), 108–119.CrossRef
go back to reference Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.CrossRef Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.CrossRef
go back to reference Brader, T. (2006). Campaigning for hearts and minds: How emotional appeals in political ads work. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Brader, T. (2006). Campaigning for hearts and minds: How emotional appeals in political ads work. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Brader, T. (2011). The political relevance of emotions: “Reassessing” revisited. Political Psychology, 32(2), 337–346.CrossRef Brader, T. (2011). The political relevance of emotions: “Reassessing” revisited. Political Psychology, 32(2), 337–346.CrossRef
go back to reference Brady, H. E., & Sniderman, P. M. (1985). Attitude attribution: A group basis for political reasoning. American Political Science Review, 79(4), 1061–1078.CrossRef Brady, H. E., & Sniderman, P. M. (1985). Attitude attribution: A group basis for political reasoning. American Political Science Review, 79(4), 1061–1078.CrossRef
go back to reference Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Casey, K., Glennerster, R., & Miguel, E. (2012). Reshaping institutions: Evidence on aid impacts using a preanalysis plan. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 127(4), 1755–1812.CrossRef Casey, K., Glennerster, R., & Miguel, E. (2012). Reshaping institutions: Evidence on aid impacts using a preanalysis plan. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 127(4), 1755–1812.CrossRef
go back to reference Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Chambers, C. D. (2013). Registered reports: A new publishing initiative at Cortex. Cortex, 49(3), 609–610.CrossRef Chambers, C. D. (2013). Registered reports: A new publishing initiative at Cortex. Cortex, 49(3), 609–610.CrossRef
go back to reference Clinton, J., Jackman, S., & Rivers, D. (2004). The statistical analysis of roll call voting: A unified approach. American Political Science Review, 98, 355–370.CrossRef Clinton, J., Jackman, S., & Rivers, D. (2004). The statistical analysis of roll call voting: A unified approach. American Political Science Review, 98, 355–370.CrossRef
go back to reference Converse, P. E. (1966). The concept of the normal vote. In A. Campbell, P. E. Converse, W. E. Miller, & D. E. Stokes (Eds.), Elections and the political order. New York: Wiley. Converse, P. E. (1966). The concept of the normal vote. In A. Campbell, P. E. Converse, W. E. Miller, & D. E. Stokes (Eds.), Elections and the political order. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Darmofal, D. (2005). Elite cues and citizen disagreement with expert opinion. Political Research Quarterly, 58(3), 381–395.CrossRef Darmofal, D. (2005). Elite cues and citizen disagreement with expert opinion. Political Research Quarterly, 58(3), 381–395.CrossRef
go back to reference Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
go back to reference Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: Harper and Row. Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: Harper and Row.
go back to reference Gilens, M. (2001). Political ignorance and collective policy preferences. American Political Science Review, 95(2), 379–396.CrossRef Gilens, M. (2001). Political ignorance and collective policy preferences. American Political Science Review, 95(2), 379–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Gill, J. (2014). Bayesian methods: A social and behavioral sciences approach (3rd ed.). New York: Chapman & Hall/CRC. Gill, J. (2014). Bayesian methods: A social and behavioral sciences approach (3rd ed.). New York: Chapman & Hall/CRC.
go back to reference Grunert, K. G. (1996). Automatic and strategic processes in advertising effects. Journal of Marketing, 60(4), 88–100.CrossRef Grunert, K. G. (1996). Automatic and strategic processes in advertising effects. Journal of Marketing, 60(4), 88–100.CrossRef
go back to reference Hotelling, H. (1929). Stability in competition. Economic Journal, 39(153), 41–57.CrossRef Hotelling, H. (1929). Stability in competition. Economic Journal, 39(153), 41–57.CrossRef
go back to reference Humphreys, M., Sanchez, R., de la Sierra, R. S., & van der Windt, P. (2013). Fishing, commitment, and communication: A proposal for comprehensive nonbinding research registration. Political Analysis, 21(1), 1–20.CrossRef Humphreys, M., Sanchez, R., de la Sierra, R. S., & van der Windt, P. (2013). Fishing, commitment, and communication: A proposal for comprehensive nonbinding research registration. Political Analysis, 21(1), 1–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Janis, I. L. (1982). Groupthink (2nd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Janis, I. L. (1982). Groupthink (2nd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
go back to reference Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision making. New York: Free Press. Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision making. New York: Free Press.
go back to reference Jerit, J., Barabas, J., & Bolsen, T. (2006). Citizens, knowledge, and the information environment. American Journal of Political Science, 50(2), 266–282.CrossRef Jerit, J., Barabas, J., & Bolsen, T. (2006). Citizens, knowledge, and the information environment. American Journal of Political Science, 50(2), 266–282.CrossRef
go back to reference Kelley, S, Jr., & Mirer, T. W. (1974). The simple act of voting. The American Political Science Review, 68, 572–591.CrossRef Kelley, S, Jr., & Mirer, T. W. (1974). The simple act of voting. The American Political Science Review, 68, 572–591.CrossRef
go back to reference Key, V. O, Jr., & Cummings, M. C. (1966). The responsible electorate: Rationality in presidential voting, 1936–1960. New York: Vintage.CrossRef Key, V. O, Jr., & Cummings, M. C. (1966). The responsible electorate: Rationality in presidential voting, 1936–1960. New York: Vintage.CrossRef
go back to reference Kinder, D. R., & Kiewiet, D. R. (1981). Sociotropic politics: The American case. British Journal of Political Science, 11, 129–162.CrossRef Kinder, D. R., & Kiewiet, D. R. (1981). Sociotropic politics: The American case. British Journal of Political Science, 11, 129–162.CrossRef
go back to reference King, G., Gakidou, E., Ravishankar, N., Moore, R. T., Lakin, J., Vargas, M., et al. (2007). A “politically robust” experimental design for public policy evaluation, with application to the Mexican universal health insurance program. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 26(3), 479–506.CrossRef King, G., Gakidou, E., Ravishankar, N., Moore, R. T., Lakin, J., Vargas, M., et al. (2007). A “politically robust” experimental design for public policy evaluation, with application to the Mexican universal health insurance program. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 26(3), 479–506.CrossRef
go back to reference Kuklinski, J. H., & Quirk, P. J. (2001). Conceptual foundations of citizen competence. Political Behavior, 23(3), 285–311.CrossRef Kuklinski, J. H., & Quirk, P. J. (2001). Conceptual foundations of citizen competence. Political Behavior, 23(3), 285–311.CrossRef
go back to reference Ladd, J. M., & Lenz, G. S. (2008). Reassessing the role of anxiety in vote choice. Political Psychology, 29(2), 275–296.CrossRef Ladd, J. M., & Lenz, G. S. (2008). Reassessing the role of anxiety in vote choice. Political Psychology, 29(2), 275–296.CrossRef
go back to reference Ladd, J. M., & Lenz, G. S. (2011). Does anxiety improve voters’ decision making? Political Psychology, 32(2), 347–361.CrossRef Ladd, J. M., & Lenz, G. S. (2011). Does anxiety improve voters’ decision making? Political Psychology, 32(2), 347–361.CrossRef
go back to reference Lewis-Beck, M. S., Jacoby, W. G., Norpoth, H., & Weisberg, H. F. (2008). The American voter revisited. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.CrossRef Lewis-Beck, M. S., Jacoby, W. G., Norpoth, H., & Weisberg, H. F. (2008). The American voter revisited. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Luskin, R. C. (1987). Measuring political sophistication. American Journal of Political Science, 31, 856–899.CrossRef Luskin, R. C. (1987). Measuring political sophistication. American Journal of Political Science, 31, 856–899.CrossRef
go back to reference Luskin, R. C. (1990). Explaining political sophistication. Political Behavior, 12(4), 331–361.CrossRef Luskin, R. C. (1990). Explaining political sophistication. Political Behavior, 12(4), 331–361.CrossRef
go back to reference MacKuen, M., Marcus, G. E., Neuman, W. R., & Keele, L. (2007). The third way: The theory of affective intelligence and American democracy. In W. R. Neuman, G. E. Marcus, A. N. Crigler, & M. MacKuen (Eds.), The affect effect: Dynamics of emotion in political thinking and behavior (pp. 124–151). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef MacKuen, M., Marcus, G. E., Neuman, W. R., & Keele, L. (2007). The third way: The theory of affective intelligence and American democracy. In W. R. Neuman, G. E. Marcus, A. N. Crigler, & M. MacKuen (Eds.), The affect effect: Dynamics of emotion in political thinking and behavior (pp. 124–151). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Marcus, G. E., MacKuen, M. B., & Neuman, W. R. (2011). Parsimony and complexity: Developing and testing theories of affective intelligence. Political Psychology, 32(2), 323–336.CrossRef Marcus, G. E., MacKuen, M. B., & Neuman, W. R. (2011). Parsimony and complexity: Developing and testing theories of affective intelligence. Political Psychology, 32(2), 323–336.CrossRef
go back to reference Marcus, G. E., Neuman, W. R., & MacKuen, M. B. (2000). Affective intelligence & political judgment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Marcus, G. E., Neuman, W. R., & MacKuen, M. B. (2000). Affective intelligence & political judgment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Miller, W. E. (1991). Party identification, realignment, and party voting: Back to the basics. American Political Science Review, 85, 557–568.CrossRef Miller, W. E. (1991). Party identification, realignment, and party voting: Back to the basics. American Political Science Review, 85, 557–568.CrossRef
go back to reference Monogan, J. E, III. (2013). A case for registering studies of political outcomes: An application in the 2010 House elections. Political Analysis, 21(1), 21–37.CrossRef Monogan, J. E, III. (2013). A case for registering studies of political outcomes: An application in the 2010 House elections. Political Analysis, 21(1), 21–37.CrossRef
go back to reference Monogan, J. E, III. (2015). Research preregistration in political science: The case, counterarguments, and a response to critiques. Political Science and Politics, 48(3), 425–429.CrossRef Monogan, J. E, III. (2015). Research preregistration in political science: The case, counterarguments, and a response to critiques. Political Science and Politics, 48(3), 425–429.CrossRef
go back to reference Mutz, D. C. (1998). Impersonal influence: How perceptions of mass collectives affect political attitudes. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Mutz, D. C. (1998). Impersonal influence: How perceptions of mass collectives affect political attitudes. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Neuman, W. R. (1981). Differentiation and integration: Two dimensions of political thinking. American Journal of Sociology, 86, 1236–1268.CrossRef Neuman, W. R. (1981). Differentiation and integration: Two dimensions of political thinking. American Journal of Sociology, 86, 1236–1268.CrossRef
go back to reference Pardos-Prado, S., & Dinas, E. (2010). Systemic polarisation and spatial voting. European Journal of Political Research, 49, 759–786.CrossRef Pardos-Prado, S., & Dinas, E. (2010). Systemic polarisation and spatial voting. European Journal of Political Research, 49, 759–786.CrossRef
go back to reference Peffley, M., Hurwitz, J., & Sniderman, P. (1997). Racial stereotypes and whites’ political views of blacks in the context of welfare and crime. American Journal of Political Science, 41(1), 30–60.CrossRef Peffley, M., Hurwitz, J., & Sniderman, P. (1997). Racial stereotypes and whites’ political views of blacks in the context of welfare and crime. American Journal of Political Science, 41(1), 30–60.CrossRef
go back to reference Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown.
go back to reference Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986a). Communication and persuasion. New York: Springer.CrossRef Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986a). Communication and persuasion. New York: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986b). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 9). San Diego: Academic Press. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986b). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 9). San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Rabinowitz, G., & Macdonald, S. E. (1989). A directional theory of issue voting. American Political Science Review, 83, 93–121.CrossRef Rabinowitz, G., & Macdonald, S. E. (1989). A directional theory of issue voting. American Political Science Review, 83, 93–121.CrossRef
go back to reference Rusting, C. L., & Larsen, R. L. (1995). Moods as sources of stimulation: Relationships between personality and desired mood states. Personality and Individual Differences, 183(3), 321–329.CrossRef Rusting, C. L., & Larsen, R. L. (1995). Moods as sources of stimulation: Relationships between personality and desired mood states. Personality and Individual Differences, 183(3), 321–329.CrossRef
go back to reference Sears, D. O. (1993). Symbolic politics: A socio-psychological theory. In S. Iyengar & W. J. McGuire (Eds.), Explorations in political psychology. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Sears, D. O. (1993). Symbolic politics: A socio-psychological theory. In S. Iyengar & W. J. McGuire (Eds.), Explorations in political psychology. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
go back to reference Zaller, J., & Feldman, S. (1992). A simple theory of the survey response: Answering questions and revealing preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 36, 579–616.CrossRef Zaller, J., & Feldman, S. (1992). A simple theory of the survey response: Answering questions and revealing preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 36, 579–616.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Anxious Voters in the 2016 U.S. Election: An Analysis of How They Decided from the ERPC2016
Author
James E. Monogan III
Publication date
24-08-2018
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Political Behavior / Issue 1/2020
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9491-3

Other articles of this Issue 1/2020

Political Behavior 1/2020 Go to the issue