Skip to main content
Top
Published in: International Journal of Disaster Risk Science 3/2014

Open Access 01-09-2014 | Article

Collaboration Exercises—The Lack of Collaborative Benefits

Authors: Johan M. Berlin, Eric D. Carlström

Published in: International Journal of Disaster Risk Science | Issue 3/2014

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

The purpose of this article is to analyze what professional emergency personnel learn during collaboration exercises and the benefits of what they have learned. Observations (n = 19) and semistructured interviews (n = 32) were carried out in conjunction with major exercises held in Sweden (2007–2012). The results show that exercises tend to be based on their own logic, which differs from actual events. Exercise participants believe that they mainly learn single-track, parallel, and path-dependent behavior. The exercises do not facilitate the use of cross-boundary activities. This means that learning, as well as benefits from the exercises for actual events, is limited. The exercises would be more appropriate if those participating had the opportunity to identify weaknesses, try alternative ways, and engage in comprehensive organizational analyses at the conclusion of the exercises. Based on the results of the study, alternative models for collaboration exercises are suggested, with elements that would better develop collaboration and contribute to learning.

1 Introduction

Collaboration between professional emergency personnel is crucial in dealing with accidents, crises, and disasters (Kapucu 2008; Powley and Nissen 2012; Christensen et al. 2013; Clarke 2013). This is especially true for larger events when resources are scarce, but also for smaller events when those who first arrive at the scene are not trained to handle the specific situation (Scholtens 2008). In most countries, the police, rescue services, and ambulance are the organizations that deal with a large number of emergency responses on a daily basis and practice collaboration regularly (Borodzicz and van Haperen 2002; Grote et al. 2009; Kapucu et al. 2010; Lateef 2010; Berlin and Carlström 2011; Van Wart and Kapucu 2011; Andersson et al. 2014). Exercises prepare them to be able to handle accidents, crises, and catastrophes in an optimal manner (McConnell and Drennan 2006; Kapucu et al. 2010; Brattberg 2012; Berlin and Carlström 2015).
In Sweden, personnel from police, rescue services, and ambulance gather regularly to practice. The exercises take place on the international, national, regional, and local level. Both management and operational personnel are involved in the planning, implementation, and follow-up of the exercises (Ingemarsdotter and Trané 2013). The exercises are arranged, for example, in the form of desktop exercises with simulations of a particular sequence of events, or as full-scale exercises in a nearby field, where personnel gather for a day and conduct practical sessions based on a simulated event.
Full-scale exercises are conducted regularly in order to strengthen the community’s ability to deal with accidents, crises, and catastrophes. The aim is to allow professional emergency personnel to acquire an increased ability to enlist the help of each other on the operational level, to cross organizational boundaries, and be prepared to act in an integrated manner (Marincioni and Fraboni 2012; Kim 2013). There is a belief that if time and resources are spent on the exercises, the organizations will become better prepared to deal with difficult situations than if no exercises are conducted (Sommer et al. 2013). The effects of the exercises, however, have been described as relatively difficult to identify (Sinclair et al. 2012). One reason for this has been that the scenarios have proven to be either too complex or too simple to contribute to any useful learning (Boin et al. 2004; Smith 2004; Crichton et al. 2009). Although the exercises are considered essential for emergency preparedness, research on full-scale exercises between police, rescue services, and ambulance has been relatively limited (Perry 2004; Rykkja 2010). This also applies to what benefits such exercises may have for the actions at actual events (Quarantelli 1988; Perry and Lindell 2003; GNYHA 2005). The purpose of this article, therefore, is to analyze what professional emergency personnel learn from collaboration exercises and the benefits of what they have learned.

2 Background

Different exercises have different aims. Here, exercise types with three aims are identified (Table 1). “Strategic exercises” (model 1) aim to simulate an event to examine what outcomes different interventions may have. Thus, the primary aim of such exercises is not the teaching of personnel at the operational level in the respective organization. Rather, the idea of strategic exercises is to analyze the outcomes of different strategies under different conditions (Babu et al. 1997). “Drill exercises” (model 2), according to Berlin and Carlström (2011), aim to strengthen individuals’ knowledge in the practice of their profession. Drills repeat key elements such as the police’s use of weapons, rescue services’ fire-fighting techniques, and ambulance personnel’s life-saving efforts. Together with colleagues, professional methods for the quick handling of common and recurring tasks are practiced. “Collaboration exercises” (model 3) aim to bring different organizations together to integrate actions across organizational boundaries. Such exercises challenge the ability to use common global resources in an optimal way (Berlin and Carlström 2011). In Sweden, such exercises are called collaboration exercises because they aim to practice collaboration across organizational boundaries (Berlin and Carlström 2008). A collaboration exercise shows the organizations’ abilities to prioritize, overlap, and complement each other in a resource-intensive effort. It is this last exercise model that is the focus of this study.
Table 1
Exercise types, learning, and benefit
Exercise type
Learning
Benefit
Strategic exercise
Knowledge of the effects
Concepts
Drill
Copying
Routines
Repetitive skills
Collaboration exercise
Flexibility
Cross-organization behavior
Resource optimization
Sources Babu et al. (1997), Berlin and Carlström (2008, 2011)
Previous studies of collaboration exercises have focused on the implementation, significance, and function of the exercises (Mitroff and Anagnos 2001; Fink 2002; Boin et al. 2005; Coombs 2007; Drennan and McConnell 2007). There is a widespread assumption that collaboration exercises prepare individuals, rescue teams, and entire agencies to become better at managing crises (Ingemarsdotter and Trané 2013). However, a serious problem is the difficulty of creating solutions for managing events that involve multiorganizational responsibilities (Donahue and Tuohy 2006; Moats et al. 2008). Organizations tend to work in parallel rather than cooperatively and often have difficulty understanding each others’ concepts, particularly, organizational models, action logics, agendas, legislations, and hierarchical levels (Peterson and Perry 1999; Perry 2004; Berlin and Carlström 2008; Lee et al. 2009).
Getting emergency personnel to take the initiative to pull together across organizational boundaries is one of the key difficulties in a collaboration exercise. This is brought to a head in situations of resource scarcity or resource asymmetry. At events where not all the organizations are represented, the ability to perform the tasks of others is tested (Berlin and Carlström 2008). While collaboration seems to be the goal, in reality, drills have been the focus of exercises (Berlin and Carlström 2013). Each organization tends to focus on its own priorities and specific tasks instead of seeing the big picture (Smith and Elliott 2007). In addition, the participants seem happy to do tasks that they are used to doing but are passive when unknown tasks need to be performed (Berlin and Carlström 2008).
One reason that the focus remains within the individual organization during exercises, which has been identified, is that the scenarios are too large and complex (Berlin and Carlström 2013). Complex scenarios make it difficult to evaluate how things fit together (Babu et al. 1997). With misplaced, overzealous ambition, exercise leaders can expose organizations to challenges far beyond the ability of the participants. In their haste, they run around each other and construct unrealistic chains of events. Such exercises can have completely opposite effects to the ones intended and do not lead to learning, increased self-awareness, or development of the participants’ roles (Loveluck 1994; Borodzicz 1997; Lagadec 1997; Borodzicz and van Haperen 2002; Powley and Nissen 2012).
A disadvantage of exercises, in comparison with actual events, is that they are fictitious and constructed; thus, they can never fully address phenomena that arise at the actual events. Furthermore, the array of accident events that can befall society are endless; this means that all possible scenarios can hardly be practiced (Rosenthal 2003; McLennan et al. 2006; Lalonde 2007; Roux-Dufort 2007; Lalonde and Roux-Dufort 2013).
Exercises have a tendency to exhibit constructed, artificial patterns that would not have occurred during an actual event. Knowing that “it is not real” can lead to behaviors that deviate from what happens at an actual event, which prevents participants from gaining experiences that may be used at an actual event (Berlin and Carlström 2008). There have been reports of so-called “infallibility behavior,” where participants show off their best sides in order to make an impression on the other participants, managers, and external visitors (Boin et al. 2005).
Smith (2004) argues that when there is a learning exercise culture, the participants are keen to reveal their own weaknesses. In such circumstances, there is an assumption that mistakes will happen and that exercises can reveal these shortcomings. According to Gredler (1992), effective exercises are comprehensible and provide space for reflection, which results in new thinking patterns. Gredler believes that the activities that follow exercises are just as important as the exercises themselves. Moynihan (2009) has shown that learning is particularly effective when professionals learn from each other, through open forums where participants from different organizations come together for discussion.
Ineffective exercises repeat the same mistakes exercise after exercise, the same sources of irritation are recorded, similar friction is experienced and even though they are highlighted, the problem is replayed again at the next exercise. Such exercises do not contribute effectively to the learning that is appropriate for the activities at actual events (Boin et al. 2005). Consequently, developing instructive, appropriate, and effective exercises is a difficult art for practice management (Alexander 2013), particularly, if the aim is to enhance preparedness to collectively handle critical events (Lalonde 2004; Lalonde and Roux-Dufort 2013).
Learning is one of the benefits of exercises (Smith and Elliott 2007). Gredler (1992) points out that a beneficial exercise results in new patterns of thought that are considered useful in actual events. Thus, learning stands for development and change. In contrast, police, rescue services, and ambulance are stable, reliable, predictable, rational, and repetitive organizations (Perrow 1984; Frederickson and LaPorte 2002; Boin et al. 2005).
Rutherford (1984) used the concept of path-dependency to illustrate a collectively accepted opinion and conventional stable behavior. Paths can prevail for a long time. This is underscored by classical examples of conservative technologies (Arthur 1988a, b; David 1985, 2001). By reinforcing processes, alternatives are avoided, and by a state of collective conservatism, renewal is resisted (Rutherford 1984). This is the hypothetical core of path narrowing. A path narrows when options for change are turned down. This state of inertia is found in publicly financed organizations that depend on long-established traditions. Such paths have however proven to be too narrow (Carlström 2012). Van Nieuwaal (2006, 2011) defines narrowing paths as having less room for maneuvers. Even though a path can be reinforced, it could still end up narrowing. However, the narrowing process does eventually lead to the creation of new paths (Garud and Karnøe 2001). The new paths are preceded by a deinstitutionalization process of dissolution of ideas and behaviors (Hinings and Malhotra 2008).
In contrast to drills, collaboration exercises require dynamics and the ability to change (Berlin and Carlström 2008). They require the ability to switch strategies depending on the situation that exists and being open to different options (Van Wart and Kapucu 2011; Sommer and Njå 2012). In a crisis context, change is about, for example, finding new ways to handle a situation by customizing one’s efforts in collaboration with others. The challenge is to achieve the quickest and best results (Grote et al. 2009; Nemeth et al. 2011).
Such change is not localized. It affects the majority of employees in an organization (Blumenthal and Haspeslagh 1994). Haydu (1998) defines institutional change as the start of a new behavior, which then spreads through a community. Old models are abandoned when they no longer serve the organization (Bush 1987).
Institutional change processes are the framework for institutional learning. Institutional learning occurs on the collective level as opposed to that in the learning theories dealing with individual learning (Stein 1997). Institutions are formed by reciprocity between individuals who, when it is favorable, change behaviors by creating common understandings and behaviors (Knudsen 2002).
An institution is based on shared values and with institutional learning, individuals adapt to shared values. Institutional learning requires adaptation and an ability to understand the prevailing conventions that exist in a collective (Appelbaum and Wohl 2000). Already established institutions are sluggish; they continue to exist and can withstand influences from the environment for a long time. In such circumstances, longstanding traditions and single-track behaviors dominate (Carlström 2012). Johansson et al. (2013) suggest the importance of combining change-willingness and stability in order to be able to implement something new and make it sustainable. This stresses the need to support flexibility and trust, as well as stability, planning, and repetitive behavior. Alharbi et al. (2014) suggest that organizations that can balance opposing characteristics seem to have the best chance to successfully implement necessary change and sustain it. This is in accordance with Quinn (1988), who asserts that effective organizations present contradictory characteristics.
Popular concepts such as collaboration can be decoupled from actual behavior. In the exercise context, this means that the ambition of learning does not have any effects on the behavior at the actual events, and deep-rooted habits prevent a profound change from taking place (Corbacioglu and Kapucu 2006). In-depth learning, however, can change values to the point that the newly learned behaviors are applied to relevant situations (Stein 1997). According to Klabbers (1999), the deeper types of learning, also called second-order learning, consist not only of abstractions and concepts, but also have elements of intensive collaboration with others (Deverell 2012; Thompson 2012).
Such learning puts the individual in a social context. Within the collective of individuals, new thinking and new methods are developed, which can also be applicable in different situations. Stein (1997) calls the phenomenon an institutional learning where routines, rules, and models are changing and have a great influence on the organization (Stein 1997; Torres and Preskill 2001).

3 Method

The data for this study were collected over 6 years (2007–2012). The study’s primary focus is the overall recurrent features of collaboration exercises. The idea is to follow the general repeated patterns of behavior over time. The study’s data come from Sweden, where the police, rescue services, and ambulance are legally equal. This means that an officer cannot give orders to personnel other than within his/her own organization. This approach challenges organizations when it comes to collaboration.
From a researcher’s perspective, the advantage of studying exercises as they progress is that they can be followed directly as it happens. The researcher can follow and document the participants’ behavior from the arrival of the first rescue unit until the situation is normalized and the exercise has been concluded (Scott 1994; Rolfe et al. 1998; Borodzicz and van Haperen 2002; Lateef 2010; Kim 2013).
Previous studies have largely been based on a retrospective approach to capture relevant data (Palm and Ramsell 2007). To gather data after a real event, however, is difficult. The events can be viewed as vague hindsight reconstructions. Those involved often have difficulty remembering exactly what happened, in what order things were done, and who did what, how, and when (Sikström 2002).
Data were collected through semistructured interviews, accompanying observations, and document studies. During data collection, we had access to the entire exercise area of a given exercise. After an exercise, we also had the opportunity to conduct interviews with the exercise participants and exercise leaders. We were able to capture their understanding of the exercises’ learning effects and how these influenced them in their daily work. Based on the observations, questions were asked about the participants’ experiences and reflections.
The empirical data for this study were collected in western, eastern, and southern Sweden. The exercises were conducted in Västra Götaland, Stockholm, and Skåne. Exercise leaders from each organization were responsible for inviting personnel from their own organizations to participate. The exercises were designed on the basis of an accident’s three phases: initial phase (call and arrival), practical phase (work at the accident site), and a normalization phase (summation, packing up, and completion). Exercise participants were expected to act as they would have in a similar actual situation. They made the same contacts, requested the same resources, and took the same actions. The exercises were full-scale, with 50–300 participants, and lasted from 4 to 48 h. The total dataset consists of 96 h of observation time. The observation time was about 5 h on average for each exercise (Table 2).
Table 2
Collaboration exercises in Sweden observed and analyzed for the study
No.
Year
Scenario
Observation time
1.
2007
Fire—elementary school
310 min
2.
2007
Traffic accident between bus and car
290 min
3.
2007
Fire—car ferry
250 min
4.
2007
Fire—high school
270 min
5.
2008
Fire—prison
330 min
6.
2008
Fire—car tunnel
240 min
7.
2008
Airplane crash
330 min
8.
2008
Fire—car ferry
280 min
9.
2009
Fire—large passenger ferry
290 min
10.
2009
Fire—elementary school
250 min
11.
2009
Threat of suicide bomb
300 min
12.
2009
Fire—defense headquarters
310 min
13.
2010
Train collision
330 min
14.
2010
Car accident pileup
260 min
15.
2011
Fire—nuclear power plant
360 min
16.
2011
Accident between bus and car
320 min
17.
2011
Fire—transport ferry
320 min
18.
2011
Fire—military naval base
360 min
19.
2012
Train collision at Öresund Bridge
360 min
  
Total:
5,760 min (96 h)
Data collection through observations is a form of ethnographic field research (Hammersley and Atkinson 2005; Wolcott 2008; Fetterman 2010). In this project, observation followed the work on collaboration exercises at close range (Jørgensen 1989; Watson 2011). To be present at the area where the exercises were performed, we accompanied one of the practicing organizations (police, rescue services, or ambulance) to the exercise site. As observers, we had an impartial, uninvolved, and passive stance. In several cases, we were both on site and were able to capture various parallel events from several angles. The observations took place where the participants gathered, such as the command center, at the mock accident scene, and at the various assembly points. To document the exercises, we used a notepad, digital camera, and voice recorder.
The observation method meant that the raw data could be collected, even though the event itself was at times relatively fast (Burgess 1991). The analysis was carried out in three steps. First, we selected the relevant images within each exercise. In the second step, we coded the images. Then we sorted and analyzed all the observations as a whole (Miles and Huberman 1984/1994; Burgess 1991; Pope and Mays 2000).
After the exercises, interviews were conducted with the exercise leaders and personnel. A total of 32 semistructured interviews were conducted with police officers, rescue services, and ambulance personnel (Kvale 1996). The total interview time was 23 h. We had the opportunity to ask detailed questions about the behaviors we were able to follow during the observations (Mason 1996; Golden-Biddle and Locke 1997).
The interview questions related to exercise effects, the perception of mistakes during the exercise, difficulty with exercises, learning, and the effects that the exercise had on regular work. They were used to study and verify the various progressions and how the collaboration exercises were understood. The interviews were conducted in person at each interviewee’s workplace. All the interviews were recorded and transcribed. In order to support certain key statements and get an understanding of some critical elements, the interviews and observations were also supplemented with document studies. They were mainly about descriptions of each exercise and subsequent evaluations.

4 Empirical Data

In Sweden, full-scale collaboration exercises are conducted periodically to test society’s catastrophe and crisis preparedness. The exercises are often designed as all-day activities with a comprehensive scenario, mock victims, exercise leaders, vehicles, radio traffic, and practicing personnel. A typical collaboration exercise begins with the organizations gathering together to review the exercise conditions, assembly areas, plan of attack, channels of communication, and safety instructions. Upon completion, the personnel prepare by gathering equipment, moving vehicles to assembly areas, synchronizing exercise channels, and waiting for the alarm. When the call is communicated, each organization goes to the accident site and starts to act. The process from start to finish can last 4–6 h. In some cases, the exercise leaders set a limit for the length of the exercise. After the exercise, the work materials are collected together quickly and participants are treated to simple refreshments. During this period, all participants are gathered, including the mock victims, for a brief review of the exercise. Thereafter, the exercise leaders thank the mock victims and participating organizations for a successful and informative exercise. The exercise is concluded, and the participants express their gratitude and return to their respective stations. Several weeks later, the exercise leaders meet to discuss possible results and what needs to be improved.

4.1 Collaboration

Collaboration is addressed in the following subheadings: complex scenarios, repetitive behaviors, restricted organizations, sequential behavior, and parallel behavior.

4.1.1 Complex Scenarios

Extensive and complex scenarios dominated the exercises. In several cases, the exercise leaders wanted to add an advanced chain of events to a single exercise, challenging a path-dependent behavior. One example of this approach was the exercise “fire at nuclear power plant” (Exercise 15, Table 2). The exercise combined severe weather conditions with technical breakdowns, a traffic accident in the security area, a fire in a building, and finally, a reactor accident involving a radioactive release due to a malfunctioning backup cooling system. Parts of this chain of events were meant to confuse and distract from the events that were most essential. The events were also not related to each other.
Both the exercise leaders and the participants had difficulty practicing collaboration and using the opportunity to practice synchronous operation.
Exercises often go off the tracks and become too big.
(Paramedic)
In order to create structure, some exercise leaders went in and coached the key players in the exercise so that they would not lose track of the event’s development. Exercise leaders became a kind of “exercise prompter” and steered the course of the event so that it was consistent with their planning. Any shortcomings in the exercise were prompted with various ad hoc solutions.
… in such a major exercise in which there are so many participants, the exercise leaders go and give small hints and try to control it, so it doesn’t become completely crazy.
(Chief, Fireman)
The exercise’s complexity, the number of organizations, and the arrangements also made it harder to explain the idea and purpose of the exercise in an easy and comprehensible manner. Exercise leaders had no clear division of responsibilities among them, which affected the logistics, as well as who should do what in connection with the exercise. In the “fire in the prison” exercise (Exercise 5, Table 2), confusion arose because of the ambiguities between the exercise leaders and the prison staff. Police, rescue services, and ambulance had difficulty getting access to the prison area because the prison security staff followed normal procedures for entry and exit. The high safety measures at the prison made it difficult for those practicing to move smoothly inside the prison area. The participating personnel demobilized guns, batons, and pepper spray, and found it difficult to act as they would at an actual event inside the prison area. The ambulance personnel were not admitted inside and instead stood waiting outside the prison’s security gates.
Complex and difficult to manage scenarios and lengthy exercise sessions contributed to the exercise becoming passive. Participants tended to wait for each other in the hope that someone else would take the initiative. They chose not to mingle with the participants from the other organizations. If the aim was to achieve collaboration by increasing the complexity, it had the opposite effect. Participants avoided collaboration and isolated themselves by being with their own colleagues.
The complexity also meant that there could be unnecessary waiting times, for example, to wait for the call, to drive to the accident site, to take action at the accident site, to pack up, to get a snack, and to go home. The exercise got stuck and the development of the event did not go smoothly. The mock victims had to wait a long time before they were taken care of, in some cases getting exposed to the cold as a result. A large part of the personnel never got the opportunity to practice synchronous collaboration. In the “fire in the car tunnel” exercise (Exercise 6, Table 2), there was such a long waiting time that a dozen people never got to participate in the exercise. Both the police and the ambulance personnel stood a distance away from the accident site and listened to the radio traffic. A growing dissatisfaction arose at the assembly area; small groups of personnel stood idly in the biting wind at a parking lot and talked about how useless it felt to not participate actively in the exercise. In the “airplane crash” exercise (Exercise 7, Table 2), a rumor spread about whether there was a bomber on board a crashed plane (Fig. 1).
The plane was on fire and there were a large number of casualties and serious injuries on board. The rumor of a terrorist attack meant that the police cordoned off the area for 40 min to search for the perpetrator. Despite the severity of the injuries, the mock victims were lined up on the runway to be searched and interrogated. In the end, the police intervention was stopped because the mock victims were not dressed for the cold weather and began to show signs of exposure. The complexity of the event and the 10 organizations involved meant that the cross-boundary collaboration moments were difficult to attain. The delay affected the continuation of the exercise; efforts ended up being brief and were conducted dutifully without much enthusiasm.

4.1.2 Repetitive Behaviors and Restricted Organizations

One feature of the exercises was that those practicing had to repeat simple steps that they were already capable of doing and had practiced in their own organization. For example, the rescue services personnel carried out the injured throughout the exercise. In the “fire in the high school” and “fire in the elementary school” exercises (Exercises 4 and 10, Table 2), firemen wearing smoke helmets evacuated smoke-injured young people for most of the exercise, a task that they were well acquainted with from previous experience (Fig. 2). The same applied to ambulance personnel. In 15 of the exercises, the ambulance personnel devoted themselves mainly to tasks that were included in their regular practice, that is, to assess, immobilize, and stabilize patients and then load and transport them to the hospital. In several of the exercises, some 20 individuals from the ambulance personnel worked only on these tasks. Other personnel, especially police officers, were engaged in only a few of these tasks. The repetitive elements were prioritized over collaboration elements. Police officers often found themselves at the barrier tape and watched the event from a distance, recorded data, took witness statements, and maintained order. These elements were repeated over and over again until the conclusion of the exercise. It meant that the exercise time was filled with simple tasks, and the number of synchronous collaboration elements was minimized. Communication between those practicing was flawed. The tasks were performed side-by-side without mutual interaction or coordination.
In the “fire on a car ferry” exercise (Exercise 3, Table 2), resources were needed to transport the injured. Because of an inadequate radio communication, most of the police units and some of the ambulance services had difficulties locating the ferry.
The communication that occurred was mostly within each organization. Several police units had placed themselves at checkpoints in remote areas on the beach, far from the car ferry. Some police officers boarded the ferry using a police boat but did not pay any attention to the injured. They passively looked on as understaffed ambulance personnel worked to attend to, carry, and transport the injured. Subsequent interviews revealed that the police personnel felt unsure of working with the injured individuals. In an interview, a police officer explained the reason for the hesitation to get involved:
We are not used to working with injured individuals. It feels like carrying or handling the injured improperly could worsen the situation.
(Police)
In the “fire in the elementary school” exercise (Exercise 10, Table 2), police were instructed to register injured students. A large number of police officers were allocated throughout the exercise to record the names and addresses of the mock victims on preprinted forms. When the exercise was over, the forms were collected and discarded. A feeling of discontent spread among the police personnel who felt that the exercise was rather pointless. Their collaboration with the other organizations had been limited during the exercise. The task they kept busy with—filling out personal information on claim forms—had nothing to do with collaboration.

4.1.3 Sequential and Parallel Behavior

The collaboration that occurred in all observed exercises was mainly limited to sequential and parallel collaboration, which essentially meant that each organization performed its tasks, one at a time, or worked in parallel side-by-side with the other organizations. Synchronous or overlapping seamless collaboration between organizations could occur for short periods. It seemed spontaneous when working on the extrication of victims from wrecked cars, evacuation of victims from a bus, or evacuation of the wounded from a burning building. This meant that the advanced collaboration elements were practiced relatively sparsely during the exercises.
We try to work as it is stated in our plans. The rescue leader does his thing, the police do theirs, and ambulance personnel do their tasks. Then we try to resolve issues individually.
(Ambulance Nurse)
It proved difficult to break down organizational barriers and collaborate with people the exercise participants did not know and were not used to working with. In the “fire at the defense headquarters” exercise (Exercise 12, Table 2), one police officer had difficulty getting help to carry a person who had been injured due to the smoke from the building. Personnel from the ambulance and rescue services did not see it as their duty to help the police officer even though they were just a few meters away. The police officer, instead, got some unexpected help from a civilian observer who came and helped to carry the smoke-injured victim to the medical care area.
In the “fire on a car ferry” exercise (Exercise 3, Table 2), the engine room on a car ferry had caught fire while out in the middle of the lake. The rescue services, police, and ambulance were supposed to rescue the injured, exhausted, and shocked victims using boats and transporting them to the medical assembly areas located on land. The police boat kept its distance during the rescue work and did not rescue the smoke-injured people from the car ferry. Rescue service and ambulance boats did not coordinate the work among themselves. They did not know what the others were doing or which part of the car ferry was supposed to be taken care of. Organizations worked sequentially and in parallel but without common coordination.
In the “fire at the military naval base” exercise (Exercise 18, Table 2), an arson fire occurred in connection with a school visit at a military naval base. Efforts to rescue the injured were delayed significantly because the ambulance personnel did not go down into the underground rock caverns until the police had surrounded, blocked off, and secured the area. The area in the rock caverns was quite large, which made it impossible for the police to secure the area within a short time. The fact that the organizations misunderstood each other meant that the exercise leaders finally had to order the ambulance personnel to begin the crucial life-saving medical work down in the rock cavern, even though the area was not cordoned off and secured. The police conducted the subsequent evaluation internally. The other participants did not get any insight into how the other organizations had arrived at different decisions during the exercise.
In the “fire on car ferry” exercise (Exercise 8, Table 2), some 20 injured persons were evacuated (Fig. 3). Already at the beginning of the exercise, the organizations split up and began repetitively to perform their own tasks, without evaluating the overall needs. This meant that the exercise was out of step and the quality of the efforts varied because some periods were understaffed. The reason was that those practicing did not keep track of the overall picture at critical moments.
In the studied exercises, the exercise leaders generally considered collaboration as something that was “given” and “well-proven,” while in reality it was not “given” how organizations should act together during the exercise. To handle the situation, the participants, for the most part, worked sequentially and in parallel. The synchronous, cross-boundary operations the exercises were intended to rehearse, encourage, and reinforce were rarely in evidence.

4.2 Lessons Learned

The section Lessons Learned refers to the data collected and asks the question “What did you learn at the exercise?”

4.2.1 What Did You Learn at the Exercise?

During subsequent interviews, the participants found it difficult to articulate clearly what they learned during the exercise. Some focused on a variety of practical features, others highlighted personal reflections, while a large group did not perceive that they had learned anything at all. One interviewee described it this way:
What did you learn about collaboration?
Well, actually nothing, you can say. I sound very negative, but I did nothing. I stood there and sent people to an assembly area, I mean, that’s nothing. It taught me nothing.
(Ambulance Nurse)
Some chose to view the fact that the exercises did not focus on collaboration as an experience. They perceived that they could still learn something from the exercise that could be applied and used in a real event. Therefore, many of the participants thought it was important to make mistakes, try things on their own during the exercise, and test alternative strategies.
… it’s really through mistakes that one learns (…)… if you end up in an exercise scenario where things do not work well, you can use that at an actual event, where you can change the things that did not work.
(Ambulance Nurse)
… regardless of how it goes, you always learn something in the exercises. (…) It is also an experience of collaboration exercises that it does not really matter how it goes, you always learn something. That’s worth something in itself.
(Chief, Fireman)
I think it is important that the exercises are designed in such a way that you can really try different things that could fail and then compare different strategies.
(Ambulance Nurse)
One difficulty was that very little time was allocated following the exercises to discuss experiences and reflections in connection with the exercises. When the exercises were concluded, the material had to be packed up and a meal had to be served. In most cases, there was not much time left for collective reflections.
There wasn’t much time for collaboration in this exercise. A longer discussion might have been preferable. Then we would also have learned more about how the others work and how to collaborate better.
(Fireman)
Long, time-consuming exercises made it difficult to have enough time for monitoring and evaluating.
I think that what happens afterward is almost more important than the exercise itself, to tie things up, and it was short, I think, and stressful. It’s always the same when you have exercises, there isn’t enough time. But it must be planned so that you have plenty of time to assess and learn from it.
(Police)
There was a need to plan the exercises so that those practicing did not run out of time at the end. Respondents felt that more emphasis needed to be placed on a concluding evaluation with discussion, feedback, and reflection.

4.3 Benefit

The section Benefit is based on questions relating to the perceived value or usefulness of the collaboration exercises in actual situations.

4.3.1 Value of Collaboration Exercises in Actual Situations

There were different views on the extent to which those practicing had gained some benefit from the collaboration exercise, which could be used in actual situations. The responses can be grouped into two categories. There was a small group who felt that they did not gain any benefits from the exercise and a larger group who believed that they had gained limited benefits from the exercise, which could be useful in actual accident work. Those who found it difficult to relate the exercise to their daily work felt that the situations that were practiced were too broadly constructed and cumbersome to be recognized as being related to the regular work.
Do you use what you learn in the exercises in actual situations?
No, I cannot think of anything that I’ve used actually.
(Police)
It’s so seldom, or almost never, that one or that I’ve been through this kind of complex event with so many injured. Then, the incident seemed imaginary. It makes it hard to bring this to your daily work.
(Fireman)
Unfortunately, it’s a little bad with the lessons learned, I would say. It seems that you forget what you’ve learned when it happens for real. (…) In principle, we almost never do this with markings and logbooks in reality. We never have this claims experience. I have worked since 1994, and have never been part of that.
(Exercise Leader, Police)
I felt like I have learned nothing new about this here. So there’s not much I can take with me.
(Ambulance Nurse)
The reason that the exercises did not have a stronger impact on the actual accident work was due to the extent of the exercises, lack of realism, and the participants’ difficulty to empathize with and relate to the exercise scenario. A large proportion of respondents felt that they could relate some parts of the exercises to real situations. In particular, when new strategies were tried, they turned out to work in actual situations. One interviewee expressed it like this:
Finally… at an exercise, then I did this and this and it actually worked well. I am testing it and also using it now in a critical situation.
(Ambulance Paramedic)
Through the exercises, those practicing could test their stress tolerance. Even if the scenarios were so confusing that things were difficult to evaluate, the participants could test their ability to stay calm and think clearly. Many of them saw this as beneficial for actual events.
At the same time, I want to state that these exercises, they help in that in real-life situations, they keep you a little bit, a little calmer, and you start thinking in a different way already on the way.
(Ambulance Paramedic)
Although it was difficult to specify exactly what from an exercise could be beneficial in an actual event, several respondents felt that the exercise contributed to an unconscious experience, which was activated during actual events. The experience of the exercise remained with them and affected the practical work in the field. This was a good reason for having these exercises regularly and for the participants who met at actual events to participate in the collaboration exercises.
Yes, absolutely. Everything we practice, you put in a backpack. You put it somewhere in the back of your head. If you end up in a similar situation again, you remember what you did in the exercise. All exercises give you experience.
(Fireman)
A limitation in terms of the benefits was that it was difficult to transfer the experiences of those practicing to other colleagues in the same organization who did not participate in the exercise. The respondents expressed that there was a transmission problem within the organization that was difficult to come to terms with.
It’s very difficult to take the experiences from one exercise and try to share them with those who were not there.
(Chief, Fireman)
Often, the exercise evaluations came long after the exercises had been conducted and the colleagues did not read them. A few people talked about the lessons learned from these exercises. When those practicing had returned home, it was as if the exercises had never occurred. This meant that the exercises could be beneficial to those practicing, but not for those colleagues who did not participate.
Another manifestation of the difficulty with the exercises was to get the experienced officers of each organization to sign up for the collaboration exercises. In many cases, they did not want to participate. They switched their shift or took sick leave to avoid the exercise.
The regular officers do not want to go. They are afraid to appear weak. (…) In the past, I checked them off on a list. Then they switched their shift at the last minute with someone else.
(Exercise Leader, Police)
This tended to result in a certain contagious effect when even younger employees were reluctant to participate in the exercises.

5 Analysis

The exercises studied were intended to strengthen the ability to prioritize, overlap, and complement each other at events that required efforts from multiple organizations at the same time. The focus of the exercises was thus collaboration, and the learning and the benefits that were expected by those practicing were primarily the ability to work across boundaries and use common resources as optimally as possible.
However, the results from the exercises showed that the collaboration elements were relatively limited. Participation mainly consisted of sequential and parallel behavior, that is, each organization performed its tasks, one at a time, or side-by-side. Synchronous or seamless collaboration occurred only briefly and was hindered because the exercises were unstructured, had messy scenarios, and complicated chains of events. In addition, collaboration was limited by involuntary inactivity, long waiting periods, and repetition of already known elements.
Learning was dependent on the possibilities to collaborate during the exercises. It benefited from opportunities to test different strategies and making room for discussion. It was hampered by the police, ambulance, and rescue services prioritizing the focus within their own organization and by the lack of exercises that allowed for common reflection (Alexander 2013).
The benefits from the exercises were that innovative approaches were tested, stress tolerance was tested, and routines were carried out. However, the benefits were limited by unrealistic scenarios, constructed layout, cumbersome organization, and difficulties in transferring one’s experiences to colleagues who did not participate in the exercises (Table 3).
Table 3
The most important results divided into collaboration, learning, and benefit
A. Collaboration
B. Learning
C. Benefit
Appeared as
Appeared as
Appeared when
 Sequential and parallel
 Opportunities to make mistakes
 Innovative approaches were tested
 Synchronous for short periods
 Ability to test different strategies
 Realistic scenarios were used
 Comparisons
 Application of systematic approaches and guidelines
 Discussions
Hindered by
Hindered by
Hindered by
 Internal organizational focus
 Lack of structure
 Unrealistic scenarios
 Advanced scenarios
 No common reflection
 Constructed scenarios
 Passivity
 Few high-level organizational discussions
 Complex organization
 Long waits
 Exercise leaders acting like teleprompters
 Difficult to transfer experience to colleagues
 Focus on simple elements (drill)
When it comes to learning, Stein (1997) asserts that there is a difference between the immediate learning or imitation of different behaviors and a deeper learning that fundamentally changes the behavior of the person practicing. This argument highlights the difficulty that arises when exercise leaders are too prescriptive during an exercise. The effect can be that those practicing do not see the consequence of a particular strategy, which prevents learning. Such an approach can also contribute to a procedural approach in which the exercise leaders and the participants strive to get to a “flawless” exemplary exercise (Boin et al. 2005). The respondents questioned the purpose of the exercises and thought it was meaningless and time-consuming to act in a sequential and parallel way during a collaboration exercise. They requested joint discussions at the conclusion of the exercises. They also wanted more opportunities to gain insights into the collaborating organizations, to openly discuss the strengths and weaknesses, and to refine and develop the approaches in dialogue with each other (Senge 1990). Some respondents even believed that they did not gain any direct benefits from the collaboration exercise they participated in.
Nonetheless, the respondents emphasized the value of being able to make mistakes and then also having the opportunity to repeat an element to correct any errors. This meant that different approaches could be tested and compared, something that could result in new patterns of thought, that is, development and change (Gredler 1992). The exercises, however, mainly consisted of repetitive actions. The emergency personnel avoided mingling with the participants from the other organizations. When the scenario became complex, the personnel avoided collaboration and isolated themselves by being with their own colleagues, and repeated simple steps that they were already familiar with. The actions were path-dependent and conventional (Rutherford 1984).
The respondents indicated, however, an emerging path narrowing. The options for change, as expressed by the participants, were rejected, indicating limited room for maneuvers (Van Nieuwaal 2006). Such narrowing processes often end up being frustrating and eventually change behaviors (Van den Ven and Poole 1995; Garud and Karnøe 2001). Consequently, critique from the participants can contribute to change, introducing new and more organic ways to perform collaboration exercises (Carlström 2012). The concept of deinstitutionalization preceding something new, presented by Hinings and Malhotra (2008), can be useful in order to identify the change process of conservative organizations characterized by long traditions and stability.
During the interviews, none of the respondents focused on the exercise that was practiced. This suggests that the scenario is secondary to collaboration skills. Even if the techniques and approaches were different at different events, it seemed that the ability to collaborate was central to those participating in the exercise. It was about practicing collaboration techniques, structures, and communication that could easily be applied to all kinds of events. This suggests that collaboration exercises, regardless of the specific scenario, can be designed so that they are beneficial for all types of crises, provided that they focus on overlap and the ability to use common resources optimally.
One purpose of complex scenarios is to achieve collaboration by forcing participants to seek help from each other. However, the complexity of the exercises had the opposite effect and helped to create uncertainty. The staff isolated themselves and solved tasks together with the colleagues from their own organization. Since institutional learning is cultural and based on common experiences, norms, and beliefs, isolation can prevent learning (Fiol and Lyles 1985). The complexity tended to reduce the cross-boundary collaboration between the participants.
The interviews show that the exercises need to be structured and that efforts should mainly be based on known routines, that is, applying the repetitive knowledge that every profession exercises regularly. Bergström et al. (2010) have shown that a lack of structure and the absence of routines, in favor of an exaggerated belief in prestigeless collaboration, lead to uncertainty. It was confirmed by interviews that participants in certain exercises became passive because of overambitious, unstructured, and messy exercises (Powley and Nissen 2012). Although the exercises need to have elements of improvisation and unexpected situations (Mendonça and Fiedrich 2006; Grote et al. 2009), they also need to allow for well-established structures. One way to achieve this is to make sure that the exercise has a clear purpose and a structure, as well as clearly defined roles for participants (Petrenj et al. 2011). This approach calls for the integration of change-willingness and stability. If the participating organizations can balance contrasting characteristics such as flexibility and firmness, then learning can be more than an individualistic and repetitive behavior (drill). New integrative strategies can be developed and implemented (collaboration exercises) (Johansson et al. 2013; Alharbi et al. 2014).
Thus, an exercise can ideally be designed as a collaboration exercise when it is applicable to different situations and provides the individual with relevant challenges. It should also allow some space for participants to synchronize their work with those from the other organizations. The essence of a collaboration exercise, as opposed to strategic exercises and drills, is to help strengthen an organization’s cross-boundary behavior (Moynihan 2009). Allowing those who are practicing to test different approaches to deal with an event, and then to evaluate the effects can, in the next stage, allow for a new approach to be tested, which supports successive method development. With such a model, institutional learning can be developed as an operational approach. It may result in new ways of thinking. Under such circumstances, the exercise would be a way to identify weaknesses and try alternative ways to address actual events.
The data in t his study comes only from Sweden. To confirm the results of the study and its application in other countries, similar studies need to be done. The study is also limited in that only collaboration exercises were studied. Experiences from other forms of exercises with other aims can only partly be applied to this study. A further limitation is that learning as a benefit of collaboration exercises is based on the respondents’ expressed perceptions. Further research that measures learning and benefits of collaboration exercises are needed to confirm the study’s results.

6 Conclusion

By focusing on the collaboration elements during exercises, that is: (1) encourage synchronous collaboration, (2) test alternative strategies, (3) be able to make mistakes, (4) encourage comparisons, and (5) have joint evaluations, vulnerability can be reduced and preparedness to handle events that require advanced collaboration can be strengthened. This applies regardless of the scenario’s characteristics, as the ability to collaborate is also applicable to other situations.
The study’s findings can inspire exercise leaders to create cross-boundary exercises that have more focus on the common task. However, there is need to examine the effects of such exercises in future studies. This could be accomplished by creating shorter exercises with more synchronous collaboration elements than were observed in these exercises

Acknowledgments

This research was made possible thanks to funding from the Swedish Civil Contingencies Agency. The authors would also like to thank police superintendents Lars Lindros, Anders Bjärgård and Anders Björneberg from the Stockholm County Police Department for their practical assistance with transportation and allowing us to follow the exercises. We especially want to thank the research project’s reference group, the journal’s Editor-in-Chief, Professor Peijun Shi, the three anonymous reviewers, and Professor Douglas L. Johnson at Clark University, for their constructive comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd.Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.
Literature
go back to reference Alexander, D.E. 2013. Approaches to emergency management teaching at the Master’s level. Journal of Emergency Management 13(1): 59–72.CrossRef Alexander, D.E. 2013. Approaches to emergency management teaching at the Master’s level. Journal of Emergency Management 13(1): 59–72.CrossRef
go back to reference Alharbi, T.S., E.D. Carlström, I. Ekman, and L.-E. Olsson. 2014. Implementation of person-centred care: Management perspective. Journal of Hospital Administration 3(3): 107–120.CrossRef Alharbi, T.S., E.D. Carlström, I. Ekman, and L.-E. Olsson. 2014. Implementation of person-centred care: Management perspective. Journal of Hospital Administration 3(3): 107–120.CrossRef
go back to reference Andersson, A., E.D. Carlström, B. Åhgren, and J.M. Berlin. 2014. Managing boundaries at the accident scene—A qualitative study of collaboration exercises. International Journal of Emergency Services 3(1): 77–94.CrossRef Andersson, A., E.D. Carlström, B. Åhgren, and J.M. Berlin. 2014. Managing boundaries at the accident scene—A qualitative study of collaboration exercises. International Journal of Emergency Services 3(1): 77–94.CrossRef
go back to reference Appelbaum, S.H., and L. Wohl. 2000. Transformation or change: Some prescriptions for health care organizations. Managing Service Quality 10(5): 279–298.CrossRef Appelbaum, S.H., and L. Wohl. 2000. Transformation or change: Some prescriptions for health care organizations. Managing Service Quality 10(5): 279–298.CrossRef
go back to reference Arthur, W.B. 1988a. Self-reinforcing mechanisms in economics. In The economy as an evolving complex system, the proceedings of the evolutionary paths of the global economy, ed. P.W. Anderson, K.J. Arrow, and D. Pines, 9–31. Massachusetts: Addisson-Wesley. Arthur, W.B. 1988a. Self-reinforcing mechanisms in economics. In The economy as an evolving complex system, the proceedings of the evolutionary paths of the global economy, ed. P.W. Anderson, K.J. Arrow, and D. Pines, 9–31. Massachusetts: Addisson-Wesley.
go back to reference Arthur, W.B. 1988b. Competing technologies: An overview. In Technical change and economic theory, ed. G. Dosi, C. Freeman, R. Nelson, G. Silverberg, and L. Soetes, 38–66. London and New York: Printer Publishers. Arthur, W.B. 1988b. Competing technologies: An overview. In Technical change and economic theory, ed. G. Dosi, C. Freeman, R. Nelson, G. Silverberg, and L. Soetes, 38–66. London and New York: Printer Publishers.
go back to reference Babu, S., K. Hodges, and E. Kjonnerod. 1997. Simulations and institutional change: Training US government professionals for improved management of complex emergencies abroad. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 5(4): 231–240.CrossRef Babu, S., K. Hodges, and E. Kjonnerod. 1997. Simulations and institutional change: Training US government professionals for improved management of complex emergencies abroad. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 5(4): 231–240.CrossRef
go back to reference Bergström, J., N. Dahlström, E. Henriqson, and S. Dekker. 2010. Team coordination in escalating situations: An empirical study using Mid-Fidelity simulation. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 18(4): 220–230.CrossRef Bergström, J., N. Dahlström, E. Henriqson, and S. Dekker. 2010. Team coordination in escalating situations: An empirical study using Mid-Fidelity simulation. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 18(4): 220–230.CrossRef
go back to reference Berlin, J.M., and E.D. Carlström. 2008. The 90-second collaboration: A critical study of collaboration exercises at extensive accident sites. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 16(4): 173–181.CrossRef Berlin, J.M., and E.D. Carlström. 2008. The 90-second collaboration: A critical study of collaboration exercises at extensive accident sites. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 16(4): 173–181.CrossRef
go back to reference Berlin, J.M., and E.D. Carlström. 2011. Why is collaboration minimised at the accident scene? A critical study of a hidden phenomenon. Disaster Prevention and Management 20(2): 159–171.CrossRef Berlin, J.M., and E.D. Carlström. 2011. Why is collaboration minimised at the accident scene? A critical study of a hidden phenomenon. Disaster Prevention and Management 20(2): 159–171.CrossRef
go back to reference Berlin, J.M., and E.D. Carlström. 2013. The dominance of mechanistic behaviour: A critical study of emergency exercises. International Journal of Emergency Management 9(4): 327–350.CrossRef Berlin, J.M., and E.D. Carlström. 2013. The dominance of mechanistic behaviour: A critical study of emergency exercises. International Journal of Emergency Management 9(4): 327–350.CrossRef
go back to reference Berlin, J.M., and E.D. Carlström. 2015. Collaboration exercises: What do they contribute?—A study of learning and usefulness. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management. doi:10.1111/1468-5973.12064. Berlin, J.M., and E.D. Carlström. 2015. Collaboration exercises: What do they contribute?—A study of learning and usefulness. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management. doi:10.​1111/​1468-5973.​12064.
go back to reference Blumenthal, B., and P. Haspeslagh. 1994. Toward a definition of corporate transformation. Sloan Management Review 35(3): 101–106. Blumenthal, B., and P. Haspeslagh. 1994. Toward a definition of corporate transformation. Sloan Management Review 35(3): 101–106.
go back to reference Boin, A., P. ’t HartB, E. Stern, and B. Sundelius. 2005. The politics of crisis management: Public leadership under pressure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Boin, A., P. ’t HartB, E. Stern, and B. Sundelius. 2005. The politics of crisis management: Public leadership under pressure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Boin, A., C. Kofman-Bos, and W. Overdijk. 2004. Crisis simulation: Exploring tomorrow’s vulnerabilities and threats. Simulation & Gaming 34(3): 378–393.CrossRef Boin, A., C. Kofman-Bos, and W. Overdijk. 2004. Crisis simulation: Exploring tomorrow’s vulnerabilities and threats. Simulation & Gaming 34(3): 378–393.CrossRef
go back to reference Borodzicz, E.P. 1997. Risky business: Crisis simulations examined in the context of the safety people. London: University of London. Borodzicz, E.P. 1997. Risky business: Crisis simulations examined in the context of the safety people. London: University of London.
go back to reference Borodzicz, E.P., and K. van Haperen. 2002. Individual and group learning in crisis simulations. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 10(3): 139–147.CrossRef Borodzicz, E.P., and K. van Haperen. 2002. Individual and group learning in crisis simulations. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 10(3): 139–147.CrossRef
go back to reference Brattberg, E. 2012. Coordination for contingencies: Taking stock of post-9/11 homeland security reforms. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 20(2): 77–89.CrossRef Brattberg, E. 2012. Coordination for contingencies: Taking stock of post-9/11 homeland security reforms. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 20(2): 77–89.CrossRef
go back to reference Burgess, R.G. 1991. In the field—An introduction to field research. London: Routledge. Burgess, R.G. 1991. In the field—An introduction to field research. London: Routledge.
go back to reference Bush, P.D. 1987. The theory of institutional change. Journal of Economic Issues 21(3): 1075–1116.CrossRef Bush, P.D. 1987. The theory of institutional change. Journal of Economic Issues 21(3): 1075–1116.CrossRef
go back to reference Carlström, E.D. 2012. Strategies for change: Adaptation to new accounting conditions. Journal of Accounting & Organizational Change 8(1): 41–61.CrossRef Carlström, E.D. 2012. Strategies for change: Adaptation to new accounting conditions. Journal of Accounting & Organizational Change 8(1): 41–61.CrossRef
go back to reference Christensen, T., P. Lægreid, and L.H. Rykkja. 2013. After a terrorist attack: Challenges for political and administrative leadership in Norway. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 21(3): 167–177.CrossRef Christensen, T., P. Lægreid, and L.H. Rykkja. 2013. After a terrorist attack: Challenges for political and administrative leadership in Norway. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 21(3): 167–177.CrossRef
go back to reference Clarke, J.N. 2013. Transitional coordination in Sudan (2006–08): Lessons from the United Nations Resident Coordinator’s Office. Disasters 37(3): 420–441.CrossRef Clarke, J.N. 2013. Transitional coordination in Sudan (2006–08): Lessons from the United Nations Resident Coordinator’s Office. Disasters 37(3): 420–441.CrossRef
go back to reference Coombs, W.T. 2007. Ongoing crisis communication. Planning, managing, and responding. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. Coombs, W.T. 2007. Ongoing crisis communication. Planning, managing, and responding. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
go back to reference Corbacioglu, S., and N. Kapucu. 2006. Organisational learning and self-adaptation in dynamic disaster environment. Disasters 30(2): 212–233.CrossRef Corbacioglu, S., and N. Kapucu. 2006. Organisational learning and self-adaptation in dynamic disaster environment. Disasters 30(2): 212–233.CrossRef
go back to reference Crichton, M.T., C.G. Ramsay, and T. Kelly. 2009. Enhancing organizational resilience through emergency planning: Learning from cross-sectoral lessons. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 17(1): 24–37.CrossRef Crichton, M.T., C.G. Ramsay, and T. Kelly. 2009. Enhancing organizational resilience through emergency planning: Learning from cross-sectoral lessons. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 17(1): 24–37.CrossRef
go back to reference David, P.A. 1985. Clio and the economics of QWERTY. American Economic Review 75(2): 332–337. David, P.A. 1985. Clio and the economics of QWERTY. American Economic Review 75(2): 332–337.
go back to reference David, P.A. 2001. Path dependence, its critics, and the quest for historical economics. In Evolution and path dependence in economic ideas. Past and present, ed. P. Garrouste, and S. Ioannides, 15–40. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar. David, P.A. 2001. Path dependence, its critics, and the quest for historical economics. In Evolution and path dependence in economic ideas. Past and present, ed. P. Garrouste, and S. Ioannides, 15–40. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.
go back to reference Deverell, E. 2012. Crisis management and learning: Factors that influence authorities’ ability to learn (Krishantering och lärande: Faktorer som påverkar myndigheters förmåga att lära). Kungl Krigsvetenskapsakademiens Handlingar och Tidskrift 216(1): 117–130 (in Swedish). Deverell, E. 2012. Crisis management and learning: Factors that influence authorities’ ability to learn (Krishantering och lärande: Faktorer som påverkar myndigheters förmåga att lära). Kungl Krigsvetenskapsakademiens Handlingar och Tidskrift 216(1): 117–130 (in Swedish).
go back to reference Drennan, L., and A. McConnell. 2007. Risk and crisis management in the public sector. New York: Routledge. Drennan, L., and A. McConnell. 2007. Risk and crisis management in the public sector. New York: Routledge.
go back to reference Fetterman, D.M. 2010. Ethnography—Step by step. London: Sage Publications. Fetterman, D.M. 2010. Ethnography—Step by step. London: Sage Publications.
go back to reference Fink, S. 2002. Crisis management: Planning for the inevitable. Lincoln: Universe. Fink, S. 2002. Crisis management: Planning for the inevitable. Lincoln: Universe.
go back to reference Fiol, C.M., and M.A. Lyles. 1985. Organizational learning. Academy of Management Review 10(4): 803–813. Fiol, C.M., and M.A. Lyles. 1985. Organizational learning. Academy of Management Review 10(4): 803–813.
go back to reference Frederickson, H.G., and T.R. LaPorte. 2002. Airport security, high reliability, and the problem of rationality. Public Administration Review 62(S1): 33–43.CrossRef Frederickson, H.G., and T.R. LaPorte. 2002. Airport security, high reliability, and the problem of rationality. Public Administration Review 62(S1): 33–43.CrossRef
go back to reference Garud, R., and P. Karnøe. 2001. Path dependency and creation. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Garud, R., and P. Karnøe. 2001. Path dependency and creation. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference GNYHA (Greater New York Hospital Association). 2005. Effective emergency management drills and exercises. Greater New York Hospital Association. http://www.gnyha.org. Accessed 12 Aug 2014. GNYHA (Greater New York Hospital Association). 2005. Effective emergency management drills and exercises. Greater New York Hospital Association. http://​www.​gnyha.​org. Accessed 12 Aug 2014.
go back to reference Golden-Biddle, K., and K. Locke. 1997. Composing qualitative research. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. Golden-Biddle, K., and K. Locke. 1997. Composing qualitative research. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
go back to reference Gredler, M. 1992. Evaluating games and simulations, a process approach. London: Kogan Page. Gredler, M. 1992. Evaluating games and simulations, a process approach. London: Kogan Page.
go back to reference Grote, G., J.C. Weichbrodt, H. Günter, E. Zala-Mezö, and B. Künzle. 2009. Coordination in high-risk organizations: The need for flexible routines. Cognition, Technology & Work 11(1): 17–27.CrossRef Grote, G., J.C. Weichbrodt, H. Günter, E. Zala-Mezö, and B. Künzle. 2009. Coordination in high-risk organizations: The need for flexible routines. Cognition, Technology & Work 11(1): 17–27.CrossRef
go back to reference Hammersley, M., and P. Atkinson. 2005. Ethnography Principles in practice. Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge. Hammersley, M., and P. Atkinson. 2005. Ethnography Principles in practice. Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge.
go back to reference Haydu, J. 1998. Making use of the past: Time periods as cases to compare and as sequences of problem solving. The American Journal of Sociology 104(2): 339–371.CrossRef Haydu, J. 1998. Making use of the past: Time periods as cases to compare and as sequences of problem solving. The American Journal of Sociology 104(2): 339–371.CrossRef
go back to reference Hinings, C.R., and N. Malhotra. 2008. Change in institutional fields. In The institutions of the market: Organizations, social systems, and governance, ed. A. Ebner, and N. Beck, 106–131. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Hinings, C.R., and N. Malhotra. 2008. Change in institutional fields. In The institutions of the market: Organizations, social systems, and governance, ed. A. Ebner, and N. Beck, 106–131. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Ingemarsdotter, J., and C. Trané. 2013. Why exercise together?—Toward a common theme and coordination of cross-sectorial exercises (Varför öva tillsammans?—mot en gemensam inriktning och samordning av tvärsektoriella övningar). FOI Totalförsvarets forskningsinstitut. Rapport nr: FOI-R-3679-SE. Stockholm: FOI Totalförsvarets forskningsinstitut (in Swedish). Ingemarsdotter, J., and C. Trané. 2013. Why exercise together?—Toward a common theme and coordination of cross-sectorial exercises (Varför öva tillsammans?mot en gemensam inriktning och samordning av tvärsektoriella övningar). FOI Totalförsvarets forskningsinstitut. Rapport nr: FOI-R-3679-SE. Stockholm: FOI Totalförsvarets forskningsinstitut (in Swedish).
go back to reference Johansson, C., Å. Åstrom, A. Kauffeldt, L. Helldin, and E.D. Carlström. 2013. Culture as a predictor of resistance to change: A study of competing values in a psychiatric nursing context. Health Policy 114(2–3): 156–162. Johansson, C., Å. Åstrom, A. Kauffeldt, L. Helldin, and E.D. Carlström. 2013. Culture as a predictor of resistance to change: A study of competing values in a psychiatric nursing context. Health Policy 114(2–3): 156–162.
go back to reference Jørgensen, D. 1989. Participant observation. A methodology for human studies. London: Sage Publications. Jørgensen, D. 1989. Participant observation. A methodology for human studies. London: Sage Publications.
go back to reference Kapucu, N. 2008. Collaborative emergency management: Better community organising, better public preparedness and response. Disasters 32(2): 239–262.CrossRef Kapucu, N. 2008. Collaborative emergency management: Better community organising, better public preparedness and response. Disasters 32(2): 239–262.CrossRef
go back to reference Kapucu, N., T. Arslan, and F. Demiroz. 2010. Collaborative emergency management and national emergency management network. Disaster Prevention and Management 19(4): 452–468.CrossRef Kapucu, N., T. Arslan, and F. Demiroz. 2010. Collaborative emergency management and national emergency management network. Disaster Prevention and Management 19(4): 452–468.CrossRef
go back to reference Kim, H. 2013. Improving simulation exercises in Korea for disaster preparedness. Disaster Prevention and Management 22(1): 38–47.CrossRef Kim, H. 2013. Improving simulation exercises in Korea for disaster preparedness. Disaster Prevention and Management 22(1): 38–47.CrossRef
go back to reference Klabbers, J.H.G. 1999. The exercises planners guide. London: HMSO. Klabbers, J.H.G. 1999. The exercises planners guide. London: HMSO.
go back to reference Knudsen, T. 2002. Economy selection theory. Journal of Evolutionary Economics 12: 443–470.CrossRef Knudsen, T. 2002. Economy selection theory. Journal of Evolutionary Economics 12: 443–470.CrossRef
go back to reference Kvale, S. 1996. Interviews: An introduction to qualitative research interviewing. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. Kvale, S. 1996. Interviews: An introduction to qualitative research interviewing. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
go back to reference Lagadec, P. 1997. Learning processes for crisis management in complex organizations. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 5(1): 24–31.CrossRef Lagadec, P. 1997. Learning processes for crisis management in complex organizations. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 5(1): 24–31.CrossRef
go back to reference Lalonde, C. 2004. In search of archetypes in crisis management. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 12(2): 76–88.CrossRef Lalonde, C. 2004. In search of archetypes in crisis management. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 12(2): 76–88.CrossRef
go back to reference Lalonde, C. 2007. The potential contribution of the field of organizational development to crisis management. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 15(2): 95–104.CrossRef Lalonde, C. 2007. The potential contribution of the field of organizational development to crisis management. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 15(2): 95–104.CrossRef
go back to reference Lalonde, C., and C. Roux-Dufort. 2013. Challenges in teaching crisis management: Connecting theories, skills, and reflexivity. Journal of Management Education 37(1): 21–50.CrossRef Lalonde, C., and C. Roux-Dufort. 2013. Challenges in teaching crisis management: Connecting theories, skills, and reflexivity. Journal of Management Education 37(1): 21–50.CrossRef
go back to reference Lateef, F. 2010. Simulation-based learning: Just like the real thing. Journal of Emergencies, Trauma and Shock 3(4): 348–352.CrossRef Lateef, F. 2010. Simulation-based learning: Just like the real thing. Journal of Emergencies, Trauma and Shock 3(4): 348–352.CrossRef
go back to reference Lee, Y.-I., P. Trim, J. Upton, and D. Upton. 2009. Large emergency-response exercises: Qualitative characteristics—A survey. Simulation & Gaming 40(6): 726–751.CrossRef Lee, Y.-I., P. Trim, J. Upton, and D. Upton. 2009. Large emergency-response exercises: Qualitative characteristics—A survey. Simulation & Gaming 40(6): 726–751.CrossRef
go back to reference Loveluck, C. 1994. Eight propositions and one proposal, edited version of the keynote address given at the 1994 ABSEL/SAGSET Conference in Warwick, England. Loveluck, C. 1994. Eight propositions and one proposal, edited version of the keynote address given at the 1994 ABSEL/SAGSET Conference in Warwick, England.
go back to reference Marincioni, F., and R. Fraboni. 2012. A baseline assessment of emergency planning and preparedness in Italian universities. Disasters 36(2): 291–315.CrossRef Marincioni, F., and R. Fraboni. 2012. A baseline assessment of emergency planning and preparedness in Italian universities. Disasters 36(2): 291–315.CrossRef
go back to reference Mason, J. 1996. Qualitative researching. London: Sage Publications. Mason, J. 1996. Qualitative researching. London: Sage Publications.
go back to reference McConnell, A., and L. Drennan. 2006. Mission impossible? Planning and preparing for crisis. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 14(2): 59–70.CrossRef McConnell, A., and L. Drennan. 2006. Mission impossible? Planning and preparing for crisis. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 14(2): 59–70.CrossRef
go back to reference McLennan, J., A.M. Holgate, M.M. Omodei, and A.J. Wearing. 2006. Decision making effectiveness in wildfire teams. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 14(1): 27–37.CrossRef McLennan, J., A.M. Holgate, M.M. Omodei, and A.J. Wearing. 2006. Decision making effectiveness in wildfire teams. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 14(1): 27–37.CrossRef
go back to reference Mendonça, D., and F. Fiedrich. 2006. Training for improvisation in emergency management: Opportunities and limits for information technology. International Journal of Emergency Management 3(4): 348–363.CrossRef Mendonça, D., and F. Fiedrich. 2006. Training for improvisation in emergency management: Opportunities and limits for information technology. International Journal of Emergency Management 3(4): 348–363.CrossRef
go back to reference Miles, M., and M. Huberman. 1984/1994. Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. Miles, M., and M. Huberman. 1984/1994. Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
go back to reference Mitroff, I.I., and G. Anagnos. 2001. Managing crisis before they happen: What every executive needs to know about crisis management. New York: Amacom. Mitroff, I.I., and G. Anagnos. 2001. Managing crisis before they happen: What every executive needs to know about crisis management. New York: Amacom.
go back to reference Moats, J.B., T.J. Chermack, and L.M. Dooley. 2008. Using scenarios to develop crisis managers: Applications of scenario planning and scenario-based training. Advances in Developing Human Resources 10(3): 397–424.CrossRef Moats, J.B., T.J. Chermack, and L.M. Dooley. 2008. Using scenarios to develop crisis managers: Applications of scenario planning and scenario-based training. Advances in Developing Human Resources 10(3): 397–424.CrossRef
go back to reference Moynihan, D.P. 2009. From intercrisis to intracrisis learning. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 17(3): 189–198.CrossRef Moynihan, D.P. 2009. From intercrisis to intracrisis learning. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 17(3): 189–198.CrossRef
go back to reference Nemeth, C., R.L. Wears, S. Patel, G. Rosen, and R. Cook. 2011. Resilience is not control: Healthcare, crisis management, and ICT. Cognition, Technology & Work 13(3): 189–202.CrossRef Nemeth, C., R.L. Wears, S. Patel, G. Rosen, and R. Cook. 2011. Resilience is not control: Healthcare, crisis management, and ICT. Cognition, Technology & Work 13(3): 189–202.CrossRef
go back to reference Palm, J., and E. Ramsell. 2007. Developing local emergency management by co-ordination between municipalities in policy networks: Experiences from Sweden. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 15(4): 173–182.CrossRef Palm, J., and E. Ramsell. 2007. Developing local emergency management by co-ordination between municipalities in policy networks: Experiences from Sweden. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 15(4): 173–182.CrossRef
go back to reference Perrow, C. 1984. Normal accidents. Living with high-risk technologies. New Jersey: Princeton University Press. Perrow, C. 1984. Normal accidents. Living with high-risk technologies. New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
go back to reference Perry, R.W. 2004. Disaster exercise outcomes for professional emergency personnel and citizen volunteers. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 12(2): 64–75.CrossRef Perry, R.W. 2004. Disaster exercise outcomes for professional emergency personnel and citizen volunteers. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 12(2): 64–75.CrossRef
go back to reference Perry, R.W., and M.K. Lindell. 2003. Preparedness for emergency response: Guidelines for the emergency planning process. Disasters 27(4): 336–350.CrossRef Perry, R.W., and M.K. Lindell. 2003. Preparedness for emergency response: Guidelines for the emergency planning process. Disasters 27(4): 336–350.CrossRef
go back to reference Peterson, D.M., and R.W. Perry. 1999. The impacts of disaster exercises upon participants. Disaster Prevention and Management 8(4): 241–254.CrossRef Peterson, D.M., and R.W. Perry. 1999. The impacts of disaster exercises upon participants. Disaster Prevention and Management 8(4): 241–254.CrossRef
go back to reference Petrenj, B., E. Letterie, and P. Trucco. 2011. Towards enhanced collaboration and information sharing towards critical infrastructure resilience: Current barriers and emerging capabilities. The 4th Annual International Conference on Next Generation Infrastructures, Virginia, November 2011. Petrenj, B., E. Letterie, and P. Trucco. 2011. Towards enhanced collaboration and information sharing towards critical infrastructure resilience: Current barriers and emerging capabilities. The 4th Annual International Conference on Next Generation Infrastructures, Virginia, November 2011.
go back to reference Pope, C., and N. Mays (eds.). 2000. Qualitative research in health care. London: BMJ. Pope, C., and N. Mays (eds.). 2000. Qualitative research in health care. London: BMJ.
go back to reference Powley, E.H., and M.E. Nissen. 2012. If you can’t trust, stick to hierarchy: Structure and trust as contingency factors in threat assessment contexts. Journal of Homeland Security and Emergency Management 9(1). Article 14. doi:10.1515/1547-7355.1986. Powley, E.H., and M.E. Nissen. 2012. If you can’t trust, stick to hierarchy: Structure and trust as contingency factors in threat assessment contexts. Journal of Homeland Security and Emergency Management 9(1). Article 14. doi:10.​1515/​1547-7355.​1986.
go back to reference Quarantelli, E.L. 1988. Disaster crisis management: A summary of research findings. Journal of Management Studies 25(4): 373–385.CrossRef Quarantelli, E.L. 1988. Disaster crisis management: A summary of research findings. Journal of Management Studies 25(4): 373–385.CrossRef
go back to reference Quinn, R.E. 1988. Beyond rational management. Mastering the paradoxes and competing demands of high performance. San Francisco: Josey-Bass. Quinn, R.E. 1988. Beyond rational management. Mastering the paradoxes and competing demands of high performance. San Francisco: Josey-Bass.
go back to reference Rolfe, I., D. Saunders, and T. Powell. 1998. Simulations and games for emergency and crisis management. London: Kogan Press. Rolfe, I., D. Saunders, and T. Powell. 1998. Simulations and games for emergency and crisis management. London: Kogan Press.
go back to reference Rosenthal, U. 2003. September 11: Public administration and the study of crises and crisis management. Administration & Society 35(2): 129–143.CrossRef Rosenthal, U. 2003. September 11: Public administration and the study of crises and crisis management. Administration & Society 35(2): 129–143.CrossRef
go back to reference Roux-Dufort, C. 2007. Is crisis management (only) a management of exceptions? Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 15(2): 105–114.CrossRef Roux-Dufort, C. 2007. Is crisis management (only) a management of exceptions? Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 15(2): 105–114.CrossRef
go back to reference Rutherford, M. 1984. Thorstein Veblen and the processes of institutional change. History of Political Economy 16(3): 341–348.CrossRef Rutherford, M. 1984. Thorstein Veblen and the processes of institutional change. History of Political Economy 16(3): 341–348.CrossRef
go back to reference Rykkja, L.H. 2010. Crisis simulations as tools for government control and coordination (Ovelser i Samfunnssikkehet som Styrnings- og samordningsverktoy). Nordiske Organisasjonsstudier 12(2): 3–26. (in Norwegian). Rykkja, L.H. 2010. Crisis simulations as tools for government control and coordination (Ovelser i Samfunnssikkehet som Styrnings- og samordningsverktoy). Nordiske Organisasjonsstudier 12(2): 3–26. (in Norwegian).
go back to reference Scholtens, A. 2008. Controlled collaboration in disaster and crisis management in the Netherlands, history and practice of an overestimated and underestimated concept. Journal of Contingency and Management 16(4): 195–207.CrossRef Scholtens, A. 2008. Controlled collaboration in disaster and crisis management in the Netherlands, history and practice of an overestimated and underestimated concept. Journal of Contingency and Management 16(4): 195–207.CrossRef
go back to reference Scott, W. 1994. Open peer commentaries on accidents in high-risks systems. Technology Studies 1(1): 23–25. Scott, W. 1994. Open peer commentaries on accidents in high-risks systems. Technology Studies 1(1): 23–25.
go back to reference Senge, P. 1990. The fifth discipline. New York: Doubleday. Senge, P. 1990. The fifth discipline. New York: Doubleday.
go back to reference Sikström, S. 2002. Forgetting curves: Implications for connectionist models. Cognitive Psychology 45(1): 95–152.CrossRef Sikström, S. 2002. Forgetting curves: Implications for connectionist models. Cognitive Psychology 45(1): 95–152.CrossRef
go back to reference Sinclair, H., E.E. Doyle, D.M. Johnston, and D. Patton. 2012. Assessing emergency management training and exercises. Disaster Prevention and Management 21(4): 507–521.CrossRef Sinclair, H., E.E. Doyle, D.M. Johnston, and D. Patton. 2012. Assessing emergency management training and exercises. Disaster Prevention and Management 21(4): 507–521.CrossRef
go back to reference Smith, D. 2004. For whom the bell tolls: Imagining accidents and the development of crisis simulation in organizations. Simulation & Gaming 35(3): 347–362.CrossRef Smith, D. 2004. For whom the bell tolls: Imagining accidents and the development of crisis simulation in organizations. Simulation & Gaming 35(3): 347–362.CrossRef
go back to reference Smith, D., and D. Elliott. 2007. Exploring the barriers to learning from crisis: Organizational learning and crisis. Management Learning 38(5): 519–538.CrossRef Smith, D., and D. Elliott. 2007. Exploring the barriers to learning from crisis: Organizational learning and crisis. Management Learning 38(5): 519–538.CrossRef
go back to reference Sommer, M., and O. Njå. 2012. Dominant learning processes in emergency response organizations: A case study of a joint rescue coordination centre. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 20(4): 219–230.CrossRef Sommer, M., and O. Njå. 2012. Dominant learning processes in emergency response organizations: A case study of a joint rescue coordination centre. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management 20(4): 219–230.CrossRef
go back to reference Sommer, M., G.S. Braut, and O. Njå. 2013. A model for learning in emergency response work. International Journal of Emergency Management 9(2): 151–169.CrossRef Sommer, M., G.S. Braut, and O. Njå. 2013. A model for learning in emergency response work. International Journal of Emergency Management 9(2): 151–169.CrossRef
go back to reference Stein, J. 1997. How institutions learn: A socio-cognitive perspective. Journal of Economic Issues 31(3): 729–740.CrossRef Stein, J. 1997. How institutions learn: A socio-cognitive perspective. Journal of Economic Issues 31(3): 729–740.CrossRef
go back to reference Thompson, D. 2012. Leveraging learning to improve disaster management outcomes. International Journal of Disaster Risk Science 3(4): 195–206.CrossRef Thompson, D. 2012. Leveraging learning to improve disaster management outcomes. International Journal of Disaster Risk Science 3(4): 195–206.CrossRef
go back to reference Torres, R.T., and H. Preskill. 2001. Evaluation and organizational learning: Past, present, and future. American Journal of Evaluation 22(3): 387–395.CrossRef Torres, R.T., and H. Preskill. 2001. Evaluation and organizational learning: Past, present, and future. American Journal of Evaluation 22(3): 387–395.CrossRef
go back to reference Van den Ven, A.H., and M.S. Poole. 1995. Explaining development and change in organizations. Academy of Management Review 20(3): 510–540. Van den Ven, A.H., and M.S. Poole. 1995. Explaining development and change in organizations. Academy of Management Review 20(3): 510–540.
go back to reference Van Nieuwaal, K. 2011. The institutional survival path—A case study on mechanical cockle fishery and gas extraction in the Dutch Wadden Sea. Enschede: Ipskamp Drukkers. Van Nieuwaal, K. 2011. The institutional survival path—A case study on mechanical cockle fishery and gas extraction in the Dutch Wadden Sea. Enschede: Ipskamp Drukkers.
go back to reference Van Wart, M., and N. Kapucu. 2011. Crisis management competencies. The case of emergency managers in the USA. Public Management Review 13(4): 489–511.CrossRef Van Wart, M., and N. Kapucu. 2011. Crisis management competencies. The case of emergency managers in the USA. Public Management Review 13(4): 489–511.CrossRef
go back to reference Watson, T.J. 2011. Ethnography, reality, and truth: The vital need for studies of ‘how things work’ in organizations and management. Journal of Management Studies 48(1): 202–217.CrossRef Watson, T.J. 2011. Ethnography, reality, and truth: The vital need for studies of ‘how things work’ in organizations and management. Journal of Management Studies 48(1): 202–217.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolcott, H.F. 2008. Ethnography. A way of seeing. Lanham: Altamira Press. Wolcott, H.F. 2008. Ethnography. A way of seeing. Lanham: Altamira Press.
Metadata
Title
Collaboration Exercises—The Lack of Collaborative Benefits
Authors
Johan M. Berlin
Eric D. Carlström
Publication date
01-09-2014
Publisher
Beijing Normal University Press
Published in
International Journal of Disaster Risk Science / Issue 3/2014
Print ISSN: 2095-0055
Electronic ISSN: 2192-6395
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s13753-014-0025-2

Other articles of this Issue 3/2014

International Journal of Disaster Risk Science 3/2014 Go to the issue