Skip to main content
Top
Published in: ROBOMECH Journal 1/2017

Open Access 01-12-2017 | Development Report

Development of an implicit method for directing weight shifting to the affected side in patients with stroke: a proof of concept study

Authors: Kazuhiro Yasuda, Kenta Saichi, Yu Kitaji, Hiroaki Harashima, Hiroyasu Iwata

Published in: ROBOMECH Journal | Issue 1/2017

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Weight-shift training during stroke rehabilitation requires patient effort, potentially causing leg spasticity and anxiety, which disturb motor learning. The purpose of this study was (1) to devise an implicit guidance method for weight bearing that uses vibratory cues (and is therefore low exertion) and (2) to determine if the implicit guidance method is feasible. The first experiment included 12 healthy subjects. We conducted an experiment to produce a Weber’s fraction capable of calculating a just-noticeable difference during a weight-shifting task. We then applied this Weber’s fraction to a weight-shifting task in a patient with stroke. Using the implicit guidance method, the patient did not perceive an increase in weight bearing while weight shifting. Furthermore, the implicit guidance method appeared to reduce anxiety during training. This implicit guidance system warrants further investigation.
Notes
Kazuhiro Yasuda and Kenta Saichi contributed equally to this paper
Abbreviations
BF
biofeedback
PC
personal computer
CoP
center of foot pressure
AR
associated reaction
WBB
Wii balance board
VAS
visual analog scale

Background

Reduced postural control, caused by sensory and motor impairments after stroke, impacts activities of daily life, and affects independent walking [1, 2]. Improving postural control in patients after stroke is extremely important and may help them lead independent lives, participate in society, and maintain optimum levels of health. Asymmetrical weight bearing and weight-shifting ability correlate with gait [3]. Stroke-related limitations in weight shifting manifest in the anterior–posterior (AP) and medio–lateral (ML) directions [4, 5], with marked limitations typically noted on the affected side [6, 7]. Additional limitations include reduced weight-shifting speed and accuracy [8, 9], reduced single leg support ability on the affected side [10, 11], and reduced floor reaction force on the affected side [12, 13]. Over time, patients who demonstrate these limitations form motor patterns that use only part of their support base as means of compensating for reduced postural control.
In clinical settings, these issues are often addressed through targeted weight-shifting training. Patients with stroke must expend considerable effort to weight shift, which often induces associated reactions (AR), and spasticity [14, 15]. One example of AR is where muscular strain in the paretic upper limb temporarily increases, upon exertion of the lower limb. Spasticity involves increased muscular strain caused by motor paralysis, and is exacerbated by muscle extension and exertion [14, 15].
Weight-shifting tasks can cause anxiety. Previous research into the relationships between emotional changes and postural control found that maintaining a standing position on an elevated surface causes “stiffness behavior” where the leg muscles display excessive tension [16, 17]. This phenomenon helps maintain postural stability during standing [18], whereas in complex, everyday environments it impedes trouble-free movement, increasing the risk of falls [19]. In patients with a history of stroke and hemiplegia, increased leg muscle tension exacerbates spasticity in the paretic limb, thereby impeding left–right symmetrical gait.
One means of engineering support for limited weight-shifting is through the use of visual or auditory biofeedback (BF). Previous studies examined foot center of pressure movement in a visual display [2023], the use of visual cues for recognizing asymmetry [24], or visual and auditory feedback [23, 25, 26]. These methods enable the perception of foot pressure and weight asymmetry, thereby allowing the conscious modification of movement and weight shifting. Mostly, visual BF is used in clinical practice; however, patients are highly dependent on vision in the early period following the initial onset of stroke [27, 28]. In detail, Marigold et al. reported that patients with stroke tend to more heavily rely on visual input to maintain frontal plane (mediolateral) sway [29]; thus, sensory supplementation, particularly in the form of tactile BF, may help avoid inadequate sensory integration. Furthermore, review articles concluded that visual or auditory BF does not improve functional balance ability [30, 31]. Several cases have used visual BF up until now during balance training; however, this training method has not been sufficiently effective and could also reinforce visual reliance.
In a clinical trial, vibrotactile BF improved body sway during quiet standing in patients with vestibular disorders and Parkinson disease [3236], and we recently found that the immediate beneficial effects on postural stability in patients with stroke [37]. In addition, several studies used haptic feedback as a navigation for weight-shifting [38, 39]. A study of Parkinson disease reported that weight induction by stimulus of visual and haptic feedback combined led to immediate increases in the anteroposterior and left–right limits of stability (LOS) [39]. However, no studies have applied haptic BF to weight shifting of the paretic leg in stroke patients. Further, no system has been devised to reduce the aforementioned AR, and the anxiety experienced, which are typical in stroke patients.
Thus, the purpose of this study was to devise a method of implicit weight-bearing guidance, using haptic stimulation. A secondary objective was to examine the feasibility of implicit guidance in patients with stroke, and verify the validity of the technique in advance of future, large-scale investigations.

Methods

Overview of the haptic-based weight-shift system

The system consists of a Wii balance board (Wiiboard, Nintendo, Japan) for capturing center of foot pressure (CoP) data. The subject was fitted with a vibration unit, with four vibrators worn around the pelvic girdle, which displayed CoP motion, coupled with a personal computer (PC) (Fig. 1). We measured CoP using a Wii balance board (WBB), with signals relayed to a PC by Bluetooth. CoP measurements (sampling) were taken every 20 ms, and the transmission period was 20 ms. The software screen displayed CoP and target weight bearing in real time (Fig. 2). The target weight-shift length can be set at any position from 0 to 1vibration motors (Z7AL2B169208200% of body weight by inputting the subject’s weight. Vibrations were transmitted to a vibration unit (i.e., pelvic belt) using a USB connection wireless module (nRF24LE1-F16Q32, Nordic, Norway). The communication cycle is 20 ms. The frequency bandwidth of the module ranges from 2400 to 2525 MHz. The vibration unit includes four built-in , KOTL, China). Vibration motors are mapped on a bony ridge (i.e., the bilateral anterior and posterior superior iliac spines) to efficiently convey vibrotactile stimulation (Fig. 3b). The rate of the vibration motor speed is 12,000 ± 2500 rpm. The intensity of vibrations can be changed by PC according to the degree of patient paralysis. Until the CoP position reaches the target weight-shift length, the device will continue to vibrate. Vibrations cease once the subject reaches the target weight-shift length.

Implicit weight-shift length-extension algorithm

Weight-shifting length is automatically determined by an implicit weight-shift length-extension. To reduce excessive voluntary movements and fear of falling, we applied Weber’s Law to the sense of weight, increasing the length of weight shifting [40]. This law enables calculation of the just-noticeable difference (JND), which is the minimum amount of change that can be perceived. In the perceptual literature, JND represents the smallest detectable change by which a subject can reliably discriminate between a comparator and an original stimulus. For example, in a size-discrimination task a participant might be asked to verbally report whether the length of a visually presented line (i.e., the comparator) differs from the length of a reference line. In this context, JNDs are defined statistically with the appropriate detection of a magnitude change based on an experimentally determined criterion [41]. Equation (1) represents Weber’s Law, where W is the current amount of weight bearing, K is the Weber ratio, and ΔW is the JND
$$\Delta \text{W = }\;\text{K}\; \times \;\text{W}$$
(1)

Experiment to derive the Weber ratio for weight-shift task

Because the Weber ratio for sense of weight bearing has not been used previously, we conducted an experiment with 12 young, healthy subjects to derive the Weber ratio. Subjects included 12 healthy adults who provided written consent for participation (mean age 21.25 years, SD 0.72, male 9 participants, female 3 participants). Body mass index (BMI) for each participant is shown in Table 1. The present study was performed with approval of the Ethical Review Board of Waseda University.
Table 1
Characteristic of the participants
Participants (n = 12)
Age
Gender
Height (cm)
Weight (kg)
BMI
Participant A
23
F
163
67
25.22
Participant B
21
F
156
54
22.19
Participant C
21
F
156
47
19.31
Participant D
20
M
176
57
18.4
Participant E
22
M
165
60
22.04
Participant F
21
M
182
90
27.17
Participant G
21
M
169
65
22.76
Participant H
21
M
174
60
19.82
Participant I
21
M
180
65
20.06
Participant J
22
M
171
63
21.55
Participant K
21
M
169
62
21.71
BMI body mass index
Subjects stood with their feet 30 cm apart, and the experimental task was repeated weight-shift task to both left and right leg (Fig. 3a). First, we instructed subjects to shift 60% of their body weight to the left side by using a vibratory cue on the left side waist (i.e., L1/2; Fig. 2). When the weight shift reached 60% of the body weight, vibrations for the left side stopped, and the vibrator activated on the right side; this process was repeated. After weight shifting once to the left and once to the right, we increased the percentage of weight shifting, based on the set Weber ratio (initial value 0.05), without communicating this change to the participants (Fig. 3c). The initial value of 0.05 was based on preliminary experiments that found a somatic sensory Weber ratio of 0.03–0.3 [42]. This procedure was repeated at 90-second intervals, and after the experiment subjects reported if they felt an increase in the amount of weight shifting, according to two conditions (“Yes” = it increased; “No” = unchanged). After performing the weight-shift task using the initial Weber ratio, the Weber ratio was either increased or decreased according to participants’ responses (decreased when the participant felt an increase in the amount of weight shifting; increased if the participant did not). Once we established the point where a change of 0.01 in the Weber ratio caused a change in response, decreases were made in 0.002 increments from the noticeable value to find the Weber ratio where increases for weight shifting were unnoticeable for each participant (Fig. 4).

Results

Table 2 shows the participants’ Weber ratios. The mean Weber ratio was 0.045, SD 0.0078, maximum 0.054, and minimum 0.024, with a 95% CI of 0.040–0.050.
Table 2
Weber ratio in 60% of the weight-bearing
Participants (n = 12)
Weber ratio
SD
Max value
Min value
95%CI
 
0.045
± 0.0078
0.054
0.024
0.040–0.050
n number of participants SD standard deviation CI confidence interval

Examination of the test validity in patients

As a preliminary step toward a large-scale clinical trial, we applied the device to one patient with stroke to examine method feasibility. In this examination, we conducted simulated weight-shift training using the derived Weber ratio, and examined the following variables: (1) perception of vibratory stimuli: whether or not vibration cues could be perceived on the paretic side, (2) perception of increased weight bearing: whether or not the patient could perceive increases in weight bearing, (3) anxiety: anxiety during the task, and referent data on the influence of training, (4) weight-bearing dose on the paretic side was measured before and after intervention, and comparisons made.
Table 3 shows patient characteristics. Our patient was a 73-year-old man with left hemiplegia (Brunstrom’s recovery stage; see Appendix [43]), caused by right cerebral infarction. The patient was able to walk independently, and had no cognitive or mental disorders (i.e., Mini-Mental State Examination score above 23 [44]; no dementia).
Table 3
Characteristic of the patient
Patient: F.M (n = 1)
 
Age
73
Gender
Male
Paralyzed side
Left
Days from onset
90
Superficial sensation
Moderate
Pain sensation
Normal
Vibratory sensation
Moderate
Proprioception
Moderate
Motor impairment
Brs; LE/VI
MMSE
28/30
Brs Brunstrom’s recovery stage, LE Lower extremity, MMSE Mini mental state examination
We familiarized the patient with the weight-bearing guidance by vibration device. (At this point, the patient was not informed that the amount of weight bearing would increase.) For the weight-shift task, the patient assumed a left–right symmetrical standing position, with feet 30 cm apart on the WBB, with eyes open. In these conditions, the patient focused on a marker placed approximately 2 m in front of him. In the pretest, the patient performed the task of weight shifting to the paretic limb five times in order to evaluate baseline weight-shift ability. After the pre-testing, a continuous weight-shift tasks of both left and right sides was repeated over 70 s × 4 sessions with one-min rest intervals. During the weight-shift task, when the vibrations for one side stopped, he shifted his weight to the other side. The target weight-bearing dose was set at pre-test value (baseline) × 0.8. In doing so, the amount of weight bearing was increased for each session based on the Weber ratio. Lastly, we performed a post-test of the weight-shift task identical to the pre-test (i.e., weight shifting to the paretic limb five times to evaluate weight-shift ability).
The patient was questioned about his perception of vibratory stimuli applied from the left and right, and his perception of increased weight bearing in each session after completion of the post-test. We used a visual analog scale (VAS) to measure anxiety after the pre-test, and after the weight-shift training [45]. The VAS is frequently used to measure pain intensity. The pain VAS is a continuous scale comprised of a horizontal (HVAS) or vertical (VVAS) line, usually 10 cm (100 mm) in length, anchored by two verbal descriptors, one for each symptom extreme. VAS is a valid measure of fall anxiety, according to a recent study [46]. We asked the patient to indicate his fear of falling, on a scale from 1 to 10. The amount of weight bearing in the pre- and post-test was calculated using the data obtained from the WBB. For weight bearing, variables were compared pre-and post-testing. Statistical analyses were performed using a Wilcoxon signed-rank test as a non-parametric test because of the small sample size [47]. Significance level was set at p < 0.05.

Results

The patient perceived the vibratory stimulation, applied from the left and right, on both the paretic and non-paretic side. Perception could therefore be used as a signal for weight shifting. The patient did not notice the increase in weight-shift between sessions. The VAS for anxiety was 5 (moderate) in the pre-test weight-shifting task, but 0 during the weight-shifting task. Compared with the pre-test, the amount of weight bearing was significantly increased from 67.34 (66.92–67.36) kg to 68.45 (68.44–69.24) kg in the post-test (p = 0.0431) with a medium effect size (r = 0.30) [48].

Discussion

In the present study, we applied a method of implicit weight-bearing guidance using vibratory cues. We calculated the Weber ratio during a weight-shift task, performed by young healthy individuals. In the range of 60% weight bearing, the Weber ratio was 0.045 (SD 0.0076). We applied a repeated weight-shift task with vibratory cues, using the calculated Weber ratio, to a patient with stroke. The patient did not notice any increase in weight bearing during the task. This investigation had no control conditions, therefore due care should be exercised during interpretation. The patient’s perceived level of anxiety was less during this task, compared with previous weight-shift tasks. Use of a weight-bearing guidance system, using vibratory stimulation, was feasible for use in a patient with stroke. Additional studies are needed to apply this system to larger cohorts of patients.
This was the first study to examine an implicit weight-bearing guidance system, using haptic-based feedback. It is also the first study to examine the feasibility of applying this system to patients with stroke. During the weight-shift task, the participant did not perceive subtle increases in the amount of weight bearing. Although the underlying mechanisms are unclear, we can postulate that sensory information is collected and maintained in the prefrontal cortex and in primary sensory cortices. This information is later collated with new sensory information. By applying the Weber ratio to the amount of weight bearing, subliminal sensory information may input into memory centers, enabling implicit weight-bearing guidance.
From a clinical perspective, providing visual information is a common training strategy for patients with stroke. Traditionally, this training involves checking postural swaying and the state of left–right leg weight bearing [20]. However visual BF appears ineffective for weight-bearing training [30, 31]. Furthermore, during high effort tasks, AR and spasticity temporarily increase, often causing anxiety, potentially reducing motivation to train [16, 17]. Future application of this system for enabling implicit weight shifting, through haptic-based feedback, may assist patients with stroke.
Furthermore, the positive effect described in this study may represent a reliable basis for future applications (e.g., turning the device from a training equipment into something that can be worn in daily life, using an insole-type pressure sensor). Our device does not interfere with other sensory modalities (i.e., visual or auditory); hence, we expect that patients and physical therapists alike will appreciate the potential of the device use in clinical setting or activities of daily life.
This experiment examined one stroke patient; therefore, future studies should examine patients with varying degrees of stroke symptom severity. Furthermore, observed effects require clarification in terms of the influences of spasticity, AR, and anxiety. There were no control conditions for the behavioral indices in the present experiment, and the impact of a potential placebo or learning effect cannot be entirely ruled out. Future studies should compare the implicit weight-bearing guidance system with conventional weight-shifting methods, in a patient population.

Conclusions

In the present study, we derived a Weber ratio during a weight-shifting task in young healthy subjects. Based on the derived Weber ratio, we applied a method of implicit weight-bearing training using vibratory cues in a stroke patient, to examine the feasibility of the method. The patient used vibratory cues to shift weight bearing to the left or right. The patient did not notice increases in weight bearing during training. Furthermore, this implicit method reduced anxiety during the weight-shifting task. Future studies should examine the effectiveness of this method, from different perspectives, in a cohort of patients with stroke in clinical settings.

Appendix

Brunnstrom’s recovery stages [43].
Lower extremity function.
Stage I
Flaccidity.
Stage II
Minimal voluntary movements.
Stage III
Hip flexion, knee flexion, and ankle dorsiflexion performed as a combined motion in sitting and standing.
Stage IV
In sitting: knee flexion beyond 90°; ankle dorsiflexion with the heel on the floor.
Stage V
In standing: isolated knee flexion with hip extended; isolated ankle dorsiflexion with knee extended.
Stage V
Almost normal movement but declining movement speed.

Authors’ contributions

KY and HI constructed the study concept and design. KS and YK collected and analyzed data. KY prepared the draft manuscript. All members verified the content of their contributions. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

Authors would like to thank Zenyu Ogawa for his support in designing hardware. Authors would like to thank Shuntarou Horikawa for help with software programming.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Written informed consent for publication of their clinical details and/or clinical images was obtained from the patient.
Ethical approval was granted by the Ethics Committee of Waseda University.

Funding

This study was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS), Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists (B) No. 15K21446 and supported by the Research Institute for Science and Engineering, Waseda University, Grant-in-Aid for Junior Researchers, and Global Robot Academia Institute, Waseda University.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Literature
1.
go back to reference Keenan MA, Perry J, Jordan C (1984) Factors affecting balance and ambulation following stroke. Clin Orthop Relat Res 182:165–171 Keenan MA, Perry J, Jordan C (1984) Factors affecting balance and ambulation following stroke. Clin Orthop Relat Res 182:165–171
2.
go back to reference Bohannon RW, Leary KM (1995) Standing balance and function over the course of acute rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit 76(11):994–996CrossRef Bohannon RW, Leary KM (1995) Standing balance and function over the course of acute rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit 76(11):994–996CrossRef
3.
go back to reference Hendrickson J et al (2014) Relationship between asymmetry of quiet standing balance control and walking post-stroke. Gait Posture 39(1):177–181CrossRef Hendrickson J et al (2014) Relationship between asymmetry of quiet standing balance control and walking post-stroke. Gait Posture 39(1):177–181CrossRef
4.
go back to reference Goldie PA et al (1996) Maximum voluntary weight-bearing by the affected and unaffected legs in standing following stroke. Clin Biomech 11(6):333–342CrossRef Goldie PA et al (1996) Maximum voluntary weight-bearing by the affected and unaffected legs in standing following stroke. Clin Biomech 11(6):333–342CrossRef
5.
go back to reference Dettmann MA, Linder MT, Sepic SB (1987) Relationships among walking performance, postural stability, and functional assessments of the hemiplegic patient. Am J Phys Med 66(2):77–90 Dettmann MA, Linder MT, Sepic SB (1987) Relationships among walking performance, postural stability, and functional assessments of the hemiplegic patient. Am J Phys Med 66(2):77–90
6.
go back to reference Turnbull GI, Charteris J, Wall JC (1996) Deficiencies in standing weight shifts by ambulant hemiplegic subjects. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit 77(4):356–362CrossRef Turnbull GI, Charteris J, Wall JC (1996) Deficiencies in standing weight shifts by ambulant hemiplegic subjects. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit 77(4):356–362CrossRef
7.
go back to reference Bohannon RW, Larkin PA (1985) Lower extremity weight bearing under various standing conditions in independently ambulatory patients with hemiparesis. Phys Ther 65(9):1323–1325CrossRef Bohannon RW, Larkin PA (1985) Lower extremity weight bearing under various standing conditions in independently ambulatory patients with hemiparesis. Phys Ther 65(9):1323–1325CrossRef
8.
go back to reference Dault MC et al (2003) Effects of visual center of pressure feedback on postural control in young and elderly healthy adults and in stroke patients. Hum Mov Sci 22(3):221–236CrossRef Dault MC et al (2003) Effects of visual center of pressure feedback on postural control in young and elderly healthy adults and in stroke patients. Hum Mov Sci 22(3):221–236CrossRef
9.
go back to reference Badke MB, Duncan PW, Di Fabio RP (1987) Influence of prior knowledge on automatic and voluntary postural adjustments in healthy and hemiplegic subjects. Phys Ther 67(10):1495–1500CrossRef Badke MB, Duncan PW, Di Fabio RP (1987) Influence of prior knowledge on automatic and voluntary postural adjustments in healthy and hemiplegic subjects. Phys Ther 67(10):1495–1500CrossRef
10.
go back to reference Rogers MW, Hedman LD, Pai YC (1993) Kinetic analysis of dynamic transitions in stance support accompanying voluntary leg flexion movements in hemiparetic adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit 74(1):19–25 Rogers MW, Hedman LD, Pai YC (1993) Kinetic analysis of dynamic transitions in stance support accompanying voluntary leg flexion movements in hemiparetic adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit 74(1):19–25
11.
go back to reference Kirker SG et al (2000) Stepping before standing: hip muscle function in stepping and standing balance after stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 68(4):458–464CrossRef Kirker SG et al (2000) Stepping before standing: hip muscle function in stepping and standing balance after stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 68(4):458–464CrossRef
12.
go back to reference Cameron DM et al (2003) Physical impairments related to kinetic energy during sit-to-stand and curb-climbing following stroke. Clin Biomech 18(4):332–340CrossRef Cameron DM et al (2003) Physical impairments related to kinetic energy during sit-to-stand and curb-climbing following stroke. Clin Biomech 18(4):332–340CrossRef
13.
go back to reference Chou SW et al (2003) Postural control during sit-to stand and gait in stroke patients. Am J Phys Med Rehabilit 82(1):42–47CrossRef Chou SW et al (2003) Postural control during sit-to stand and gait in stroke patients. Am J Phys Med Rehabilit 82(1):42–47CrossRef
14.
go back to reference Dvir Z, Penturin E, Prop I (1996) The effect of graded effort on the severity of associated reactions in hemiplegic patients. Clin Rehabilit 10(2):155–158CrossRef Dvir Z, Penturin E, Prop I (1996) The effect of graded effort on the severity of associated reactions in hemiplegic patients. Clin Rehabilit 10(2):155–158CrossRef
15.
go back to reference Bhakta BB et al (2001) Quantifying associated reactions in the paretic arm in stroke and their relationship to spasticity. Clin Rehabilit 15(2):195–206CrossRef Bhakta BB et al (2001) Quantifying associated reactions in the paretic arm in stroke and their relationship to spasticity. Clin Rehabilit 15(2):195–206CrossRef
16.
go back to reference Adkin AL et al (2000) Postural control is scaled to level of postural threat. Gait Posture 12(2):87–93CrossRef Adkin AL et al (2000) Postural control is scaled to level of postural threat. Gait Posture 12(2):87–93CrossRef
17.
go back to reference Carpenter MG, Frank JS, Silcher CP (1999) Surface height effects on postural control: a hypothesis for a stiffness strategy for stance. J Vestib Res 9(4):277–286 Carpenter MG, Frank JS, Silcher CP (1999) Surface height effects on postural control: a hypothesis for a stiffness strategy for stance. J Vestib Res 9(4):277–286
18.
go back to reference Huffman JL et al (2009) Does increased postural threat lead to more conscious control of posture? Gait Posture 30(4):528–532CrossRef Huffman JL et al (2009) Does increased postural threat lead to more conscious control of posture? Gait Posture 30(4):528–532CrossRef
19.
go back to reference Zettel JL et al (2007) Gaze behavior governing balance recovery in an unfamiliar and complex environment. Neurosci Lett 422(3):207–212CrossRef Zettel JL et al (2007) Gaze behavior governing balance recovery in an unfamiliar and complex environment. Neurosci Lett 422(3):207–212CrossRef
20.
go back to reference Shumway-Cook A, Anson D, Haller S (1988) Postural sway biofeedback: its effect on reestablishing stance stability in hemiplegic patients. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit 69(6):395–400 Shumway-Cook A, Anson D, Haller S (1988) Postural sway biofeedback: its effect on reestablishing stance stability in hemiplegic patients. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit 69(6):395–400
21.
go back to reference Sackley CM, Lincoln NB (1997) Single blind randomized controlled trial of visual feedback after stroke: effects on stance symmetry and function. Disabil Rehabilit 19(12):536–546CrossRef Sackley CM, Lincoln NB (1997) Single blind randomized controlled trial of visual feedback after stroke: effects on stance symmetry and function. Disabil Rehabilit 19(12):536–546CrossRef
22.
go back to reference Winstein CJ et al (1989) Standing balance training: effect on balance and locomotion in hemiparetic adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit 70(10):755–762 Winstein CJ et al (1989) Standing balance training: effect on balance and locomotion in hemiparetic adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit 70(10):755–762
23.
go back to reference Lee MY, Wong MK, Tang FT (1996) Clinical evaluation of a new biofeedback standing balance training device. J Med Eng Technol 20(2):60–66CrossRef Lee MY, Wong MK, Tang FT (1996) Clinical evaluation of a new biofeedback standing balance training device. J Med Eng Technol 20(2):60–66CrossRef
24.
go back to reference Matjacic Z, Hesse S, Sinkjaer T (2003) BalanceReTrainer: a new standing-balance training apparatus and methods applied to a chronic hemiparetic subject with a neglect syndrome. NeuroRehabilitation 18(3):251–259 Matjacic Z, Hesse S, Sinkjaer T (2003) BalanceReTrainer: a new standing-balance training apparatus and methods applied to a chronic hemiparetic subject with a neglect syndrome. NeuroRehabilitation 18(3):251–259
25.
go back to reference Wong AM et al (1997) The development and clinical evaluation of a standing biofeedback trainer. J Rehabilit Res Dev 34(3):322–327 Wong AM et al (1997) The development and clinical evaluation of a standing biofeedback trainer. J Rehabilit Res Dev 34(3):322–327
26.
go back to reference De Nunzio A et al (2014) Biofeedback rehabilitation of posture and weightbearing distribution in stroke: a center of foot pressure analysis. Funct Neurol 29(2):127–134 De Nunzio A et al (2014) Biofeedback rehabilitation of posture and weightbearing distribution in stroke: a center of foot pressure analysis. Funct Neurol 29(2):127–134
27.
go back to reference Bonan I et al (2006) Visual dependence after recent stroke. Ann Readapt Med Phys 49(4):166–171CrossRef Bonan I et al (2006) Visual dependence after recent stroke. Ann Readapt Med Phys 49(4):166–171CrossRef
28.
go back to reference Yelnik AP et al (2006) Postural visual dependence after recent stroke: assessment by optokinetic stimulation. Gait Posture 24(3):262–269CrossRef Yelnik AP et al (2006) Postural visual dependence after recent stroke: assessment by optokinetic stimulation. Gait Posture 24(3):262–269CrossRef
29.
go back to reference Marigold DS, Eng JJ (2006) The relationship of asymmetric weight-bearing with postural sway and visual reliance in stroke. Gait Posture 23(2):249–255CrossRef Marigold DS, Eng JJ (2006) The relationship of asymmetric weight-bearing with postural sway and visual reliance in stroke. Gait Posture 23(2):249–255CrossRef
30.
go back to reference Barclay-Goddard R et al (2004) Force platform feedback for standing balance training after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 4:CD004129 Barclay-Goddard R et al (2004) Force platform feedback for standing balance training after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 4:CD004129
31.
go back to reference Van Peppen RP et al (2006) Effects of visual feedback therapy on postural control in bilateral standing after stroke: a systematic review. J Rehabilit Med 38(1):3–9CrossRef Van Peppen RP et al (2006) Effects of visual feedback therapy on postural control in bilateral standing after stroke: a systematic review. J Rehabilit Med 38(1):3–9CrossRef
32.
go back to reference Horak FB et al (2009) Vibrotactile biofeedback improves tandem gait in patients with unilateral vestibular loss. Ann NY Acad Sci 1164:279–281CrossRef Horak FB et al (2009) Vibrotactile biofeedback improves tandem gait in patients with unilateral vestibular loss. Ann NY Acad Sci 1164:279–281CrossRef
33.
go back to reference Sienko KH et al (2010) Assessment of vibrotactile feedback on postural stability during pseudorandom multidirectional platform motion. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 57(4):944–952CrossRef Sienko KH et al (2010) Assessment of vibrotactile feedback on postural stability during pseudorandom multidirectional platform motion. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 57(4):944–952CrossRef
34.
go back to reference Wall C 3rd (2010) Application of vibrotactile feedback of body motion to improve rehabilitation in individuals with imbalance. J Neurol Phys Ther 34(2):98–104CrossRef Wall C 3rd (2010) Application of vibrotactile feedback of body motion to improve rehabilitation in individuals with imbalance. J Neurol Phys Ther 34(2):98–104CrossRef
35.
go back to reference Lee BC et al (2012) Cell phone based balance trainer. J Neuroeng Rehabilit 9:10CrossRef Lee BC et al (2012) Cell phone based balance trainer. J Neuroeng Rehabilit 9:10CrossRef
36.
go back to reference Nanhoe-Mahabier W et al (2012) The effects of vibrotactile biofeedback training on trunk sway in Parkinson’s disease patients. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 18(9):1017–1021CrossRef Nanhoe-Mahabier W et al (2012) The effects of vibrotactile biofeedback training on trunk sway in Parkinson’s disease patients. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 18(9):1017–1021CrossRef
37.
go back to reference Yasuda K et al (2017) The effect of a haptic biofeedback system on postural control in patients with stroke: an experimental pilot study. Somatosens Motor Res 34(2):65–71 Yasuda K et al (2017) The effect of a haptic biofeedback system on postural control in patients with stroke: an experimental pilot study. Somatosens Motor Res 34(2):65–71
38.
go back to reference Lee BC, Chen S, Sienko KH (2011) A wearable device for real-time motion error detection and vibrotactile instructional cuing. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabilit Eng 19(4):374–381CrossRef Lee BC, Chen S, Sienko KH (2011) A wearable device for real-time motion error detection and vibrotactile instructional cuing. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabilit Eng 19(4):374–381CrossRef
39.
go back to reference Lee BC et al (2015) The effects of different sensory augmentation on weight-shifting balance exercises in Parkinson’s disease and healthy elderly people: a proof-of-concept study. J Neuroeng Rehabilit 12:75CrossRef Lee BC et al (2015) The effects of different sensory augmentation on weight-shifting balance exercises in Parkinson’s disease and healthy elderly people: a proof-of-concept study. J Neuroeng Rehabilit 12:75CrossRef
40.
go back to reference Davarpanah Jazi S, Heath M (2014) Weber’s law in tactile grasping and manual estimation: feedback-dependent evidence for functionally distinct processing streams. Brain Cognit 86:32–41CrossRef Davarpanah Jazi S, Heath M (2014) Weber’s law in tactile grasping and manual estimation: feedback-dependent evidence for functionally distinct processing streams. Brain Cognit 86:32–41CrossRef
41.
go back to reference Ganel T, Chajut E, Algom D (2008) Visual coding for action violates fundamental psychophysical principles. Curr Biol 18(14):R599–R601CrossRef Ganel T, Chajut E, Algom D (2008) Visual coding for action violates fundamental psychophysical principles. Curr Biol 18(14):R599–R601CrossRef
42.
43.
go back to reference Arya KN et al (2014) Does the motor level of the paretic extremities affect balance in poststroke subjects? Rehabilit Res Pract 2014:767859 Arya KN et al (2014) Does the motor level of the paretic extremities affect balance in poststroke subjects? Rehabilit Res Pract 2014:767859
44.
go back to reference Lancu I, Olmer A (2006) The minimental state examination–an up-to-date review. Harefuah 145(9):687–690 Lancu I, Olmer A (2006) The minimental state examination–an up-to-date review. Harefuah 145(9):687–690
45.
go back to reference Myles PS et al (2017) Measuring acute postoperative pain using the visual analog scale: the minimal clinically important difference and patient acceptable symptom state. Br J Anaesth 118(3):424–429CrossRef Myles PS et al (2017) Measuring acute postoperative pain using the visual analog scale: the minimal clinically important difference and patient acceptable symptom state. Br J Anaesth 118(3):424–429CrossRef
46.
go back to reference Scheffer AC et al (2010) Reliability and validity of the visual analogue scale for fear of falling in older persons. J Am Geriatr Soc 58(11):2228–2230CrossRef Scheffer AC et al (2010) Reliability and validity of the visual analogue scale for fear of falling in older persons. J Am Geriatr Soc 58(11):2228–2230CrossRef
47.
go back to reference Fagerland MW (2012) t-tests, non-parametric tests, and large studies--a paradox of statistical practice? BMC Med Res Methodol. 12(78)CrossRef Fagerland MW (2012) t-tests, non-parametric tests, and large studies--a paradox of statistical practice? BMC Med Res Methodol. 12(78)CrossRef
48.
go back to reference Parker RI, Hagan-Burke S (2007) Useful effect size interpretations for single case research. Behav Ther 38(1):95–105CrossRef Parker RI, Hagan-Burke S (2007) Useful effect size interpretations for single case research. Behav Ther 38(1):95–105CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Development of an implicit method for directing weight shifting to the affected side in patients with stroke: a proof of concept study
Authors
Kazuhiro Yasuda
Kenta Saichi
Yu Kitaji
Hiroaki Harashima
Hiroyasu Iwata
Publication date
01-12-2017
Publisher
Springer International Publishing
Published in
ROBOMECH Journal / Issue 1/2017
Electronic ISSN: 2197-4225
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s40648-017-0094-2

Other articles of this Issue 1/2017

ROBOMECH Journal 1/2017 Go to the issue