Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Schmalenbach Business Review 3-4/2016

12-08-2016 | Original Article

Do Affective States Influence Risk Preferences?

Evidence from Incentive-Compatible Experiments

Authors: Theresa Treffers, Philipp D. Koellinger, Arnold Picot

Published in: Schmalenbach Business Review | Issue 3-4/2016

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Recent discussions in decision sciences and economics stress the potential impact of affect on decision outcomes. In this study, we conducted incentive-compatible laboratory experiments (N = 253) to investigate whether affect causes temporary fluctuations in risk preferences. In particular, we employed film clips to induce participants into joyful, fearful and sad affective states and subsequently elicited risk preferences by asking the participants to make choices among different lotteries. The financial consequences of the lottery choices varied randomly among the fixed-, low-, and high-stakes treatment groups. We find only weak evidence that affective states influence risk preferences. In particular, we find some evidence that sadness leads to risk aversion, but we find no effects for joy and fear. Our findings question recent claims in the literature that the relationship between affect and risk preferences is strong and unambiguous.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
Affect is often employed as an umbrella term that refers to the current moods and emotions of individuals. Moods are generally described as low-intensity, diffuse, and relatively enduring affective states that often arise for no particularly salient reason. Compared with moods, emotions are considered to be more intense and short-lived affective states that generally have a definite cause and clear cognitive content (Davidson 1994; Gray and Watson 2001). The distinctions between moods and emotions are largely theoretical rather than empirical in nature. In research practice, identical methods are often used to induce both moods and emotions (Fredrickson 2002). In this paper, we use the terms “affect”, “affective state”, and “mood” interchangeably. Nevertheless, we believe that the definition of mood – low-intensity, diffuse, and not object-directed – is more appropriate to our empirical examination than the definition of emotion – intense, object-directed, and with clear cognitive content.
 
2
Pham (2007) presents a critical review on the empirical evidence on the relationship between emotion and rationality.
 
3
According to Thomson Reuter’s web of science, 188 articles have been published on the relationship between affect (emotion, mood) and risk references in the social sciences since 1980 until 2014.
 
4
The Journal of Business & Psychology edited a special issue addressing null results and several articles in this special issue served as excellent examples when null results can be meaningful.
 
5
Overall, the internal reliabilities for extroversion (Cronbach’s α = 0.74), agreeableness (α = 0.23), conscientiousness (α = 0.71), emotional stability (α = 0.69), and openness (α = 0.52) were not satisfactory. Therefore, we conducted a rotated factor analysis and extracted four components with eigenvalues that were greater than 1, that is, extroversion, emotional stability, conscientiousness, and openness. Agreeableness was not found to be a separate factor and was not included as a control variable in the analysis. The principal component analysis (PCA) considers all available information, whereas sum scores of the items for one personality dimension would ignore the personality differences among participants. The factor scores and the sum scores were nearly perfectly correlated for four of the five personality dimensions examined (rs > 0.90, ps < 0.001).
 
6
We pre-tested film clips from “Silence of the Lambs” and “Halloween” as suggested by Hewig et al. (2005) to induce fear in a sample of 50 students. These film clips did not significantly induce a fearful state in our participants. Thus, we chose to use a film clip from “Paranormal Activity” that successfully induced the desired fearful state.
 
7
We used factor scores for the analysis because they consider all the available information, whereas sum scores of the items for one mood type would ignore the mood differences among participants that arose from changes in fear or sadness. We included the same PANAS-X items in the principal component analysis (PCA) factor scores as in the sum scores. Varimax rotation is applied. The factor scores and sum scores were nearly perfectly correlated for all three of the examined moods (r > 0.95, p < 0.001). The results are robust to using sum scores instead of factor scores.
 
8
d = 0.20 denotes a small effect, d = 0.50 a medium effect, and d = 0.80 a large effect (Cohen 1988).
 
9
Detailed correlation results available upon request.
 
10
No evidence against poolability was produced by a poolability analysis across the factor of gender.
 
11
We only consider α = 0.05 given that none of our results survives the conservative Bonferroni correction. Statistical power was calculated using formulas derived from first principals, which yields results that are identical to those obtained from using standard software packages such as GPOWER (Erdfelder et al. 1996).
 
12
Ibid.
 
Literature
go back to reference Allen, N.B., and P.B.T. Badcock. 2003. The social risk hypothesis of depressed mood: evolutionary, psychosocial, and neurobiological perspectives. Psychological Bulletin 129:887–913.CrossRef Allen, N.B., and P.B.T. Badcock. 2003. The social risk hypothesis of depressed mood: evolutionary, psychosocial, and neurobiological perspectives. Psychological Bulletin 129:887–913.CrossRef
go back to reference Andersen, S., G.W. Harrison, M.I. Lau, and E.E. Rutstroem. 2006. Elicitation using multiple price list formats. Experimental Economics 9:383–405.CrossRef Andersen, S., G.W. Harrison, M.I. Lau, and E.E. Rutstroem. 2006. Elicitation using multiple price list formats. Experimental Economics 9:383–405.CrossRef
go back to reference Anderson, C.J. 2003. The psychology of doing nothing: forms of decision avoidance result from reason and emotion. Psychological Bulletin 129:139–167.CrossRef Anderson, C.J. 2003. The psychology of doing nothing: forms of decision avoidance result from reason and emotion. Psychological Bulletin 129:139–167.CrossRef
go back to reference Ashenfelter, O., C. Harmon, and H. Oosterbeek. 1999. A review of estimates of the schooling/earnings relationship, with tests for publication bias. Labour Economics 6:453–470.CrossRef Ashenfelter, O., C. Harmon, and H. Oosterbeek. 1999. A review of estimates of the schooling/earnings relationship, with tests for publication bias. Labour Economics 6:453–470.CrossRef
go back to reference Blavatskyy, P.R. 2009. Betting on own knowledge: experimental test of overconfidence. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 38:39–49.CrossRef Blavatskyy, P.R. 2009. Betting on own knowledge: experimental test of overconfidence. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 38:39–49.CrossRef
go back to reference Botzen, W.J.W., J. de Boer, and T. Terpstra. 2013. Framing of risk and preferences for annual and multi-year flood insurance. Journal of Economic Psychology 39:357–375.CrossRef Botzen, W.J.W., J. de Boer, and T. Terpstra. 2013. Framing of risk and preferences for annual and multi-year flood insurance. Journal of Economic Psychology 39:357–375.CrossRef
go back to reference Callen, M., M. Isaqzadeh, J.D. Long, and C. Sprenger. 2014. Violence and risk preference: experimental evidence from Afghanistan. American Economic Review 104:123–148.CrossRef Callen, M., M. Isaqzadeh, J.D. Long, and C. Sprenger. 2014. Violence and risk preference: experimental evidence from Afghanistan. American Economic Review 104:123–148.CrossRef
go back to reference Camerer, C.F., and R.M. Hogarth. 1999. The effects of financial incentives in experiments: a review and capital-labor-production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 19:7–42.CrossRef Camerer, C.F., and R.M. Hogarth. 1999. The effects of financial incentives in experiments: a review and capital-labor-production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 19:7–42.CrossRef
go back to reference Carver, C.S., and E. Harmon-Jones. 2009. Anger is an approach-related affect: evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin 135:183–204.CrossRef Carver, C.S., and E. Harmon-Jones. 2009. Anger is an approach-related affect: evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin 135:183–204.CrossRef
go back to reference Chiles, T.H., and J.F. McMackin. 1996. Integrating variable risk preferences, trust, and transaction cost theory. Academy of Management Review 21:73–99. Chiles, T.H., and J.F. McMackin. 1996. Integrating variable risk preferences, trust, and transaction cost theory. Academy of Management Review 21:73–99.
go back to reference Chow, G.C. 1960. Tests of equality between sets of coefficients in two linear regressions. Econometrica 28:591–605.CrossRef Chow, G.C. 1960. Tests of equality between sets of coefficients in two linear regressions. Econometrica 28:591–605.CrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cohen, J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Cohn, A., J. Engelmann, E. Fehr, and M.A. Maréchal. 2015. Evidence for countercyclical risk aversion: an experiment with financial professionals. American Economic Review 105(2):860–885.CrossRef Cohn, A., J. Engelmann, E. Fehr, and M.A. Maréchal. 2015. Evidence for countercyclical risk aversion: an experiment with financial professionals. American Economic Review 105(2):860–885.CrossRef
go back to reference Croson, R., and U. Gneezy. 2009. Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature 47:448–474.CrossRef Croson, R., and U. Gneezy. 2009. Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature 47:448–474.CrossRef
go back to reference Davidson, R. 1994. On emotion, mood, and related affective constructs. In The nature of emotion: fundamental questions, ed. P. Ekman, R. Davidson, 51–55. New York: Oxford University Press. Davidson, R. 1994. On emotion, mood, and related affective constructs. In The nature of emotion: fundamental questions, ed. P. Ekman, R. Davidson, 51–55. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Dickersin, K., and Y.I. Min. 1993. Publication bias: the problem that won’t go away. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 703:135–148.CrossRef Dickersin, K., and Y.I. Min. 1993. Publication bias: the problem that won’t go away. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 703:135–148.CrossRef
go back to reference Dohmen, T., A. Falk, D. Huffman, U. Sunde, J. Schupp, and G.G. Wagner. 2011. Individual risk attitudes: measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association 9:522–550.CrossRef Dohmen, T., A. Falk, D. Huffman, U. Sunde, J. Schupp, and G.G. Wagner. 2011. Individual risk attitudes: measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association 9:522–550.CrossRef
go back to reference Eckel, C.C., M.A. El-Gamal, and R.K. Wilson. 2009. Risk loving after the storm: a Bayesian-network study of Hurricane Katrina evacuees. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 69:110–124.CrossRef Eckel, C.C., M.A. El-Gamal, and R.K. Wilson. 2009. Risk loving after the storm: a Bayesian-network study of Hurricane Katrina evacuees. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 69:110–124.CrossRef
go back to reference Ekman, P. 1992. An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion 6:169–200.CrossRef Ekman, P. 1992. An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion 6:169–200.CrossRef
go back to reference Erdfelder, E., F. Faul, and A. Buchner. 1996. GPOWER: a general power analysis program. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, & Computer 28:1–11.CrossRef Erdfelder, E., F. Faul, and A. Buchner. 1996. GPOWER: a general power analysis program. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, & Computer 28:1–11.CrossRef
go back to reference Fehr-Duda, H., T. Epper, A. Bruhin, and R. Schubert. 2011. Risk and rationality: the effects of mood and decision rules on probability weighting. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 78:14–24.CrossRef Fehr-Duda, H., T. Epper, A. Bruhin, and R. Schubert. 2011. Risk and rationality: the effects of mood and decision rules on probability weighting. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 78:14–24.CrossRef
go back to reference Finucane, M.L., A. Alhakami, P. Slovic, and S.M. Johnson. 2000. The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 13:1–17.CrossRef Finucane, M.L., A. Alhakami, P. Slovic, and S.M. Johnson. 2000. The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 13:1–17.CrossRef
go back to reference Fischbacher, U. 2007. z‑Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics 10:171–178.CrossRef Fischbacher, U. 2007. z‑Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics 10:171–178.CrossRef
go back to reference Fischer, P., T. Greitemeyer, A. Kastenmüller, C. Vogrincic, and A. Sauer. 2011. The effects of risk-glorifying media exposure on risk-positive cognitions, emotions, and behaviors: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 137:367–390.CrossRef Fischer, P., T. Greitemeyer, A. Kastenmüller, C. Vogrincic, and A. Sauer. 2011. The effects of risk-glorifying media exposure on risk-positive cognitions, emotions, and behaviors: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 137:367–390.CrossRef
go back to reference Forgas, J.P. 1995. Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin 117:39–66.CrossRef Forgas, J.P. 1995. Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin 117:39–66.CrossRef
go back to reference Fredrickson, B. 2002. Positive emotions. In Handbook of positive psychology, ed. C. Snyder, S. Lopez, 120–134. London: Oxford University Press. Fredrickson, B. 2002. Positive emotions. In Handbook of positive psychology, ed. C. Snyder, S. Lopez, 120–134. London: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Gerrards-Hesse, A., K. Spies, and F.W. Hesse. 1994. Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: a review. British Journal of Psychology 85:55–78.CrossRef Gerrards-Hesse, A., K. Spies, and F.W. Hesse. 1994. Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: a review. British Journal of Psychology 85:55–78.CrossRef
go back to reference Goeree, J.K., C.A. Holt, and T.R. Palfrey. 2003. Risk averse behavior in generalized matching pennies games. Games & Economic Behavior 45:97–113.CrossRef Goeree, J.K., C.A. Holt, and T.R. Palfrey. 2003. Risk averse behavior in generalized matching pennies games. Games & Economic Behavior 45:97–113.CrossRef
go back to reference Göritz, A.S., and K. Moser. 2006. Web-based mood induction. Cognition & Emotion 20:887–896.CrossRef Göritz, A.S., and K. Moser. 2006. Web-based mood induction. Cognition & Emotion 20:887–896.CrossRef
go back to reference Gosling, S.D., P.J. Rentfrow, and W.B. Swann Jr.. 2003. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality 37:504–528.CrossRef Gosling, S.D., P.J. Rentfrow, and W.B. Swann Jr.. 2003. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality 37:504–528.CrossRef
go back to reference Grable, J.E., and M.J. Roszkowski. 2008. The influence of mood on the willingness to take financial risks. Journal of Risk Research 11:905–923.CrossRef Grable, J.E., and M.J. Roszkowski. 2008. The influence of mood on the willingness to take financial risks. Journal of Risk Research 11:905–923.CrossRef
go back to reference Gray, E., and D. Watson. 2001. Emotion, mood, and temperament: similarities, differences and a synthesis. In Emotions at work: theory, research and applications in management, ed. R. Payne, C. Cooper, 21–34. New York: Wiley. Gray, E., and D. Watson. 2001. Emotion, mood, and temperament: similarities, differences and a synthesis. In Emotions at work: theory, research and applications in management, ed. R. Payne, C. Cooper, 21–34. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Greiner, B. 2004. The online recruitment system ORSEE – a guide for the organization of experiments in economics. In Forschung und wissenschaftliches Rechnen 2003. GWDG Bericht 63, ed. K. Kremer, V. Macho, 79–93. Göttingen: Gesellschaft für Wissenschaftliche Datenverarbeitung. Research and scientific computation 2003. GWDG report 63. Greiner, B. 2004. The online recruitment system ORSEE – a guide for the organization of experiments in economics. In Forschung und wissenschaftliches Rechnen 2003. GWDG Bericht 63, ed. K. Kremer, V. Macho, 79–93. Göttingen: Gesellschaft für Wissenschaftliche Datenverarbeitung. Research and scientific computation 2003. GWDG report 63.
go back to reference Gross, J.J., and R.W. Levenson. 1995. Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition & Emotion 9:87–108.CrossRef Gross, J.J., and R.W. Levenson. 1995. Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition & Emotion 9:87–108.CrossRef
go back to reference Gujarati, D. 1970. Use of dummy variables in testing for equality between sets of coefficients in linear regressions: a generalization. American Statistician 24:18–22. Gujarati, D. 1970. Use of dummy variables in testing for equality between sets of coefficients in linear regressions: a generalization. American Statistician 24:18–22.
go back to reference Harmon-Jones, E. 2003. Anger and the behavioral approach system. Personality & Individual Differences 35:995–1005.CrossRef Harmon-Jones, E. 2003. Anger and the behavioral approach system. Personality & Individual Differences 35:995–1005.CrossRef
go back to reference Harrison, G.W., M.I. Lau, and E.E. Rutstroem. 2009. Risk attitudes, randomization to treatment, and self-selection into experiments. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 70:498–507.CrossRef Harrison, G.W., M.I. Lau, and E.E. Rutstroem. 2009. Risk attitudes, randomization to treatment, and self-selection into experiments. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 70:498–507.CrossRef
go back to reference Haushofer, J., and E. Fehr. 2014. On the psychology of poverty. Science 344:862.CrossRef Haushofer, J., and E. Fehr. 2014. On the psychology of poverty. Science 344:862.CrossRef
go back to reference Heilman, R.M., L.G. Crişan, D. Houser, M. Miclea, and A.C. Miu. 2010. Emotion regulation and decision making under risk and uncertainty. Emotion 10:257–265.CrossRef Heilman, R.M., L.G. Crişan, D. Houser, M. Miclea, and A.C. Miu. 2010. Emotion regulation and decision making under risk and uncertainty. Emotion 10:257–265.CrossRef
go back to reference Hewig, J., D. Hagemann, J. Seifert, M. Gollwitzer, E. Naumann, and D. Bartussek. 2005. A revised film set for the induction of basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion 19:1095–1109.CrossRef Hewig, J., D. Hagemann, J. Seifert, M. Gollwitzer, E. Naumann, and D. Bartussek. 2005. A revised film set for the induction of basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion 19:1095–1109.CrossRef
go back to reference Hey, J. 2001. Does repetition improve consistency? Experimental Economics 4:5–54.CrossRef Hey, J. 2001. Does repetition improve consistency? Experimental Economics 4:5–54.CrossRef
go back to reference Hirshleifer, D., and T. Shumway. 2003. Good day sunshine: stock returns and the weather. Journal of Finance 58:1009–1032.CrossRef Hirshleifer, D., and T. Shumway. 2003. Good day sunshine: stock returns and the weather. Journal of Finance 58:1009–1032.CrossRef
go back to reference Hockey, G.R.J., A.J. Maule, P.J. Clough, and L. Bdzola. 2000. Effects of negative mood states on risk in everyday decision making. Cognition & Emotion 14:823–855.CrossRef Hockey, G.R.J., A.J. Maule, P.J. Clough, and L. Bdzola. 2000. Effects of negative mood states on risk in everyday decision making. Cognition & Emotion 14:823–855.CrossRef
go back to reference Holt, C.A., and S.K. Laury. 2002. Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review 92:1644–1655.CrossRef Holt, C.A., and S.K. Laury. 2002. Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review 92:1644–1655.CrossRef
go back to reference Ifcher, J., and H. Zarghamee. 2011. Happiness and time preference: the effect of positive affect in a random-assignment experiment. American Economic Review 101:3109–3129.CrossRef Ifcher, J., and H. Zarghamee. 2011. Happiness and time preference: the effect of positive affect in a random-assignment experiment. American Economic Review 101:3109–3129.CrossRef
go back to reference Ioannidis, J.P. 2005. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Medicine 2:e124.CrossRef Ioannidis, J.P. 2005. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Medicine 2:e124.CrossRef
go back to reference Ioannidis, J.P. 2008. Why most discovered true associations are inflated. Epidemiology 19(5):640–648.CrossRef Ioannidis, J.P. 2008. Why most discovered true associations are inflated. Epidemiology 19(5):640–648.CrossRef
go back to reference Isen, A.M. 2000. Positive affect and decision making. In Handbook of emotion, ed. M. Lewis, J.M. Haviland-Jones, 417–435. New York: Guilford. Isen, A.M. 2000. Positive affect and decision making. In Handbook of emotion, ed. M. Lewis, J.M. Haviland-Jones, 417–435. New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Isen, A.M., and R. Patrick. 1983. The effect of positive feelings on risk taking: when the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 31:194–202.CrossRef Isen, A.M., and R. Patrick. 1983. The effect of positive feelings on risk taking: when the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 31:194–202.CrossRef
go back to reference Johansson-Stenman, O. 2010. Risk aversion and expected utility of consumption over time. Games & Economic Behavior 68:208–219.CrossRef Johansson-Stenman, O. 2010. Risk aversion and expected utility of consumption over time. Games & Economic Behavior 68:208–219.CrossRef
go back to reference Kamstra, M.J., L.A. Kramer, and M.D. Levi. 2000. Losing sleep at the market: the daylight saving anomaly. American Economic Review 90:1005–1011.CrossRef Kamstra, M.J., L.A. Kramer, and M.D. Levi. 2000. Losing sleep at the market: the daylight saving anomaly. American Economic Review 90:1005–1011.CrossRef
go back to reference Kamstra, M.J., L.A. Kramer, and M.D. Levi. 2003. Winter blues: a SAD stock market cycle. American Economic Review 93:324–343.CrossRef Kamstra, M.J., L.A. Kramer, and M.D. Levi. 2003. Winter blues: a SAD stock market cycle. American Economic Review 93:324–343.CrossRef
go back to reference Kim, M.Y., and R. Kanfer. 2009. The joint influence of mood and a cognitively demanding task on risk-taking. Motivation & Emotion 33:362–372.CrossRef Kim, M.Y., and R. Kanfer. 2009. The joint influence of mood and a cognitively demanding task on risk-taking. Motivation & Emotion 33:362–372.CrossRef
go back to reference Kliger, D., and O. Levy. 2003. Mood-induced variation in risk preferences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 52:573–584.CrossRef Kliger, D., and O. Levy. 2003. Mood-induced variation in risk preferences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 52:573–584.CrossRef
go back to reference Kugler, T., T. Connolly, and L.A. Ordóñez. 2012. Emotion, decision, and risk: betting on gambles versus betting on people. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 25:123–134.CrossRef Kugler, T., T. Connolly, and L.A. Ordóñez. 2012. Emotion, decision, and risk: betting on gambles versus betting on people. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 25:123–134.CrossRef
go back to reference Landis, R.S., L.R. James, C.E. Lance, C.A. Pierce, and S.G. Rogelberg. 2014. When is nothing something? Editorial for the null results special issue of journal of business and psychology. Journal of Business & Psychology 29:163–167.CrossRef Landis, R.S., L.R. James, C.E. Lance, C.A. Pierce, and S.G. Rogelberg. 2014. When is nothing something? Editorial for the null results special issue of journal of business and psychology. Journal of Business & Psychology 29:163–167.CrossRef
go back to reference Leith, K.P., and R.F. Baumeister. 1996. Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk tasking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71:1250–1267.CrossRef Leith, K.P., and R.F. Baumeister. 1996. Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk tasking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71:1250–1267.CrossRef
go back to reference Lerner, J.S., and D. Keltner. 2000. Beyond valence: toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition & Emotion 14:473–493.CrossRef Lerner, J.S., and D. Keltner. 2000. Beyond valence: toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition & Emotion 14:473–493.CrossRef
go back to reference Lerner, J.S., and D. Keltner. 2001. Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81:146–159.CrossRef Lerner, J.S., and D. Keltner. 2001. Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81:146–159.CrossRef
go back to reference Lerner, J.S., D.A. Small, and G. Loewenstein. 2004. Heart strings and purse strings carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological Science 15:337–341.CrossRef Lerner, J.S., D.A. Small, and G. Loewenstein. 2004. Heart strings and purse strings carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological Science 15:337–341.CrossRef
go back to reference Levy, O., and I. Galili. 2008. Stock purchase and the weather: individual differences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 67:755–767.CrossRef Levy, O., and I. Galili. 2008. Stock purchase and the weather: individual differences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 67:755–767.CrossRef
go back to reference Levy, Y., and J. Yagil. 2011. Air pollution and stock returns in the US. Journal of Economic Psychology 32:374–383.CrossRef Levy, Y., and J. Yagil. 2011. Air pollution and stock returns in the US. Journal of Economic Psychology 32:374–383.CrossRef
go back to reference Loewenstein, G.F., E.U. Weber, C.K. Hsee, and N. Welch. 2001. Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin 127:267–286.CrossRef Loewenstein, G.F., E.U. Weber, C.K. Hsee, and N. Welch. 2001. Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin 127:267–286.CrossRef
go back to reference Mukherjee, K. 2010. A dual system model of preferences under risk. Psychological Review 177:243–255.CrossRef Mukherjee, K. 2010. A dual system model of preferences under risk. Psychological Review 177:243–255.CrossRef
go back to reference Open Science Collaboration. 2015. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349(6251):aac4716.CrossRef Open Science Collaboration. 2015. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349(6251):aac4716.CrossRef
go back to reference Pham, M.T. 2004. The logic of feeling. Journal of Consumer Psychology 14:360–369.CrossRef Pham, M.T. 2004. The logic of feeling. Journal of Consumer Psychology 14:360–369.CrossRef
go back to reference Pham, M.T. 2007. Emotion and rationality: a critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Review of General Psychology 11:155–178.CrossRef Pham, M.T. 2007. Emotion and rationality: a critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Review of General Psychology 11:155–178.CrossRef
go back to reference Raghunathan, R., and M.T. Pham. 1999. All negative moods are not equal: motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 79:56–77.CrossRef Raghunathan, R., and M.T. Pham. 1999. All negative moods are not equal: motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 79:56–77.CrossRef
go back to reference Rietveld, C.A., et al, 2014. Common genetic variants associated with cognitive performance identified using the proxy-phenotype method. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111(38):13790–13794.CrossRef Rietveld, C.A., et al, 2014. Common genetic variants associated with cognitive performance identified using the proxy-phenotype method. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111(38):13790–13794.CrossRef
go back to reference Saunders Jr., E.M. 1993. Stock prices and Wall Street weather. American Economic Review 83:1337–1345. Saunders Jr., E.M. 1993. Stock prices and Wall Street weather. American Economic Review 83:1337–1345.
go back to reference Scherer, K.R. 2005. What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information 44:695–729.CrossRef Scherer, K.R. 2005. What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information 44:695–729.CrossRef
go back to reference Schwarz, N., and G.L. Clore. 1983. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45:513–523.CrossRef Schwarz, N., and G.L. Clore. 1983. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45:513–523.CrossRef
go back to reference Shu, H.C. 2010. Investor mood and financial markets. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 76:267–282.CrossRef Shu, H.C. 2010. Investor mood and financial markets. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 76:267–282.CrossRef
go back to reference De Silva, D.G., R.A.J. Pownall, and L. Wolk. 2012. Does the sun ‘shine’on art prices? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 82:167–178.CrossRef De Silva, D.G., R.A.J. Pownall, and L. Wolk. 2012. Does the sun ‘shine’on art prices? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 82:167–178.CrossRef
go back to reference Smith, V.L. 1982. Microeconomic systems as an experimental science. American Economic Review 72:923–955. Smith, V.L. 1982. Microeconomic systems as an experimental science. American Economic Review 72:923–955.
go back to reference Stanton, A.L., S.B. Kirk, C.L. Cameron, and S. Danoff-Burg. 2000. Coping through emotional approach: scale construction and validation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78:1150–1169.CrossRef Stanton, A.L., S.B. Kirk, C.L. Cameron, and S. Danoff-Burg. 2000. Coping through emotional approach: scale construction and validation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78:1150–1169.CrossRef
go back to reference Stigler, G.J., and G.S. Becker. 1977. De gustibus non est disputandum. American Economic Review 67:76–90. Stigler, G.J., and G.S. Becker. 1977. De gustibus non est disputandum. American Economic Review 67:76–90.
go back to reference Sunstein, C.R. 2003. Terrorism and probability neglect. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 26:121–136.CrossRef Sunstein, C.R. 2003. Terrorism and probability neglect. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 26:121–136.CrossRef
go back to reference Verheyen, C., and A.S. Göritz. 2009. Plain texts as an online mood-induction procedure. Social Psychology 40:6–15.CrossRef Verheyen, C., and A.S. Göritz. 2009. Plain texts as an online mood-induction procedure. Social Psychology 40:6–15.CrossRef
go back to reference Viscusi, W.K. 2009. Valuing risks of death from terrorism and natural disasters. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 38:191–213.CrossRef Viscusi, W.K. 2009. Valuing risks of death from terrorism and natural disasters. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 38:191–213.CrossRef
go back to reference Voors, M.J., E.E.M. Nillesen, P. Verwimp, E.H. Bulte, R. Lensink, and D.P. Van Soest. 2012. Violent conflict and behavior: a field experiment in Burundi. American Economic Review 102:941–964.CrossRef Voors, M.J., E.E.M. Nillesen, P. Verwimp, E.H. Bulte, R. Lensink, and D.P. Van Soest. 2012. Violent conflict and behavior: a field experiment in Burundi. American Economic Review 102:941–964.CrossRef
go back to reference Watson, D., and L.A. Clark. 1994. The PANAS-X: manual for the positive and negative affect schedule – expanded form. Iowa City: University of Iowa. Watson, D., and L.A. Clark. 1994. The PANAS-X: manual for the positive and negative affect schedule – expanded form. Iowa City: University of Iowa.
go back to reference Watson, D., L.A. Clark, and A. Tellegen. 1988. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54:1063–1070.CrossRef Watson, D., L.A. Clark, and A. Tellegen. 1988. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54:1063–1070.CrossRef
go back to reference Weber, E.U., A. Blais, and N.E. Betz. 2002. A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 15:263–290.CrossRef Weber, E.U., A. Blais, and N.E. Betz. 2002. A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 15:263–290.CrossRef
go back to reference Wegener, D.T., and R.E. Petty. 1994. Mood management across affective states: the hedonic contingency hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66:1034–1048.CrossRef Wegener, D.T., and R.E. Petty. 1994. Mood management across affective states: the hedonic contingency hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66:1034–1048.CrossRef
go back to reference Westermann, R., K. Spies, G. Stahl, and F.W. Hesse. 1996. Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: a meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology 26:557–580.CrossRef Westermann, R., K. Spies, G. Stahl, and F.W. Hesse. 1996. Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: a meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology 26:557–580.CrossRef
go back to reference Yuan, K., L. Zheng, and Q. Zhu. 2006. Are investors moonstruck? Lunar phases and stock returns. Journal of Empirical Finance 13:1–23.CrossRef Yuan, K., L. Zheng, and Q. Zhu. 2006. Are investors moonstruck? Lunar phases and stock returns. Journal of Empirical Finance 13:1–23.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Do Affective States Influence Risk Preferences?
Evidence from Incentive-Compatible Experiments
Authors
Theresa Treffers
Philipp D. Koellinger
Arnold Picot
Publication date
12-08-2016
Publisher
Springer International Publishing
Published in
Schmalenbach Business Review / Issue 3-4/2016
Print ISSN: 1439-2917
Electronic ISSN: 2194-072X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s41464-016-0018-3

Other articles of this Issue 3-4/2016

Schmalenbach Business Review 3-4/2016 Go to the issue