Skip to main content
Top
Published in:

10-12-2021

Effects of linguistic style on persuasiveness of word-of-mouth messages with anonymous vs. identifiable sources

Authors: Jie Chen, Wenjian Fan, Junlong Wei, Zunli Liu

Published in: Marketing Letters | Issue 4/2022

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Online word-of-mouth messages can use either powerful or powerless linguistic styles and come from either anonymous or identifiable sources. Do differences in linguistic power affect the persuasiveness of such messages, and if so, how? We propose that the effects of linguistic style depend on the identifiability of the source. In three experiments, consumers receive word-of-mouth messages varying in linguistic style (powerful vs. powerless) and source identifiability (anonymous vs. identifiable). Across the experiments, an anonymous source paired with a powerful style and an identifiable source paired with a powerless style violate expectancy, stimulate cognitive elaboration, and enhance persuasiveness. Furthermore, need for cognition moderates this joint effect. Thus, an anonymous (identifiable) source can attract interest and gain influence by using a powerful (powerless) linguistic style. These findings shed new light on the persuasiveness of online word-of-mouth messages and show how companies can flexibly design effective word-of-mouth campaigns.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
For robustness, we also performed a two-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) by including the control variables (age, gender, income, and education) in the model. The results were robust (interaction between linguistic style and source identifiability: F (1, 140) = 7.48, p = .007).
 
2
We conducted a two-way ANCOVA by including the control variables (knowledge of, familiarity with, and experience of Japanese Restaurants, Evaluation APP, and Social Media) in the model. The results were robust (F (1, 98) = 7.51, p = .007).
 
Literature
go back to reference Alvídrez, S., & Rodríguez, O. F. (2016). Powerful communication style on twitter: Effects on credibility and civic participation. Comunicar, 24(47), 89–97.CrossRef Alvídrez, S., & Rodríguez, O. F. (2016). Powerful communication style on twitter: Effects on credibility and civic participation. Comunicar, 24(47), 89–97.CrossRef
go back to reference Areni, C. S., & Sparks, J. R. (2005). Language power and persuasion. Psychology & Marketing, 22, 507–525.CrossRef Areni, C. S., & Sparks, J. R. (2005). Language power and persuasion. Psychology & Marketing, 22, 507–525.CrossRef
go back to reference Blankenship, K. L., & Craig, T. Y. (2007). Language and persuasion: Tag questions as powerless speech or as interpreted in context. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(1), 112–118.CrossRef Blankenship, K. L., & Craig, T. Y. (2007). Language and persuasion: Tag questions as powerless speech or as interpreted in context. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(1), 112–118.CrossRef
go back to reference Blankenship, K. L., & Holtgraves, T. (2005). The role of different markers of linguistic powerlessness in persuasion. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 24(1), 3–24.CrossRef Blankenship, K. L., & Holtgraves, T. (2005). The role of different markers of linguistic powerlessness in persuasion. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 24(1), 3–24.CrossRef
go back to reference Bradac, J. J., & Mulac, A. (1984). A molecular view of powerful and powerless speech styles: Attributional consequences of specific language features and communicator intentions. Communications Monographs, 51(4), 307–319.CrossRef Bradac, J. J., & Mulac, A. (1984). A molecular view of powerful and powerless speech styles: Attributional consequences of specific language features and communicator intentions. Communications Monographs, 51(4), 307–319.CrossRef
go back to reference Carpenter, C. J. (2015). A meta-analysis of the ELM’s argument quality × processing type predictions. Human Communication Research, 41, 501–534.CrossRef Carpenter, C. J. (2015). A meta-analysis of the ELM’s argument quality × processing type predictions. Human Communication Research, 41, 501–534.CrossRef
go back to reference Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116–131.CrossRef Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Morris, K. (1983). Effects of need for cognition on message evaluation, recall, and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(4), 805–818.CrossRef Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Morris, K. (1983). Effects of need for cognition on message evaluation, recall, and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(4), 805–818.CrossRef
go back to reference Chaiken, S., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212–252). Guilford. Chaiken, S., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212–252). Guilford.
go back to reference Chen, J., Teng, L., Yu, Y., & Yu, X. (2016). The effect of online information sources on purchase intentions between consumers with high and low susceptibility to informational influence. Journal of Business Research, 69(2), 467–475.CrossRef Chen, J., Teng, L., Yu, Y., & Yu, X. (2016). The effect of online information sources on purchase intentions between consumers with high and low susceptibility to informational influence. Journal of Business Research, 69(2), 467–475.CrossRef
go back to reference Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil, C., Gamon, M., & Dumais, S. (2011). Mark my words! Linguistic style accommodation in social media. In Proceedings of the 20th International Conference on World Wide Web (pp. 745–754). ACM. Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil, C., Gamon, M., & Dumais, S. (2011). Mark my words! Linguistic style accommodation in social media. In Proceedings of the 20th International Conference on World Wide Web (pp. 745–754). ACM.
go back to reference Dubois, D., Rucker, D. D., & Tormala, Z. L. (2011). From rumors to facts, and facts to rumors: The role of certainty decay in consumer communications. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(6), 1020–1032.CrossRef Dubois, D., Rucker, D. D., & Tormala, Z. L. (2011). From rumors to facts, and facts to rumors: The role of certainty decay in consumer communications. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(6), 1020–1032.CrossRef
go back to reference Fiedler, K., Harris, C., & Schott, M. (2018). Unwarranted inferences from statistical mediation tests–an analysis of articles published in 2015. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 75, 95–102.CrossRef Fiedler, K., Harris, C., & Schott, M. (2018). Unwarranted inferences from statistical mediation tests–an analysis of articles published in 2015. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 75, 95–102.CrossRef
go back to reference Gotlieb, J. B., & Sarel, D. (1991). Comparative advertising effectiveness: The role of involvement and source credibility. Journal of Advertising, 20(1), 38–45.CrossRef Gotlieb, J. B., & Sarel, D. (1991). Comparative advertising effectiveness: The role of involvement and source credibility. Journal of Advertising, 20(1), 38–45.CrossRef
go back to reference Holtgraves, T., & Bonnefon, J. F. (2017). Experimental approaches to linguistic (im) politeness. In The Palgrave handbook of linguistic (im)politeness (pp. 381–401). London: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRef Holtgraves, T., & Bonnefon, J. F. (2017). Experimental approaches to linguistic (im) politeness. In The Palgrave handbook of linguistic (im)politeness (pp. 381–401). London: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRef
go back to reference Jensen, J. D. (2008). Scientific uncertainty in news coverage of cancer research: Effects of hedging on scientists’ and journalists’ credibility. Human Communication Research, 34(3), 347–369.CrossRef Jensen, J. D. (2008). Scientific uncertainty in news coverage of cancer research: Effects of hedging on scientists’ and journalists’ credibility. Human Communication Research, 34(3), 347–369.CrossRef
go back to reference Karmarkar, U. R., & Tormala, Z. L. (2010). Believe me, I have no idea what I’m talking about: The effects of source certainty on consumer involvement and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(6), 1033–1049.CrossRef Karmarkar, U. R., & Tormala, Z. L. (2010). Believe me, I have no idea what I’m talking about: The effects of source certainty on consumer involvement and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(6), 1033–1049.CrossRef
go back to reference Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 123–205). Academic Press. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 123–205). Academic Press.
go back to reference Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. (1981). Personal involvement as a determinant of argument-based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(5), 847–855.CrossRef Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. (1981). Personal involvement as a determinant of argument-based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(5), 847–855.CrossRef
go back to reference Pfeiffer, B. E., Deval, H., Silvera, D. H., Cronley, M. L., & Kardes, F. R. (2019). The effect of message credibility, need for cognitive closure, and information sufficiency on thought-induced attitude change. Marketing Letters, 30(2), 193–205.CrossRef Pfeiffer, B. E., Deval, H., Silvera, D. H., Cronley, M. L., & Kardes, F. R. (2019). The effect of message credibility, need for cognitive closure, and information sufficiency on thought-induced attitude change. Marketing Letters, 30(2), 193–205.CrossRef
go back to reference Pogacar, R., Angle, J., Lowrey, T. M., Shrum, L. J., & Kardes, F. R. (2021). Is Nestlé a lady? The feminine brand name advantage. Journal of Marketing, 85(6), 101–117.CrossRef Pogacar, R., Angle, J., Lowrey, T. M., Shrum, L. J., & Kardes, F. R. (2021). Is Nestlé a lady? The feminine brand name advantage. Journal of Marketing, 85(6), 101–117.CrossRef
go back to reference Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42(1), 185–227.CrossRef Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42(1), 185–227.CrossRef
go back to reference Pulford, B. D., Colman, A. M., Buabang, E. K., & Krockow, E. M. (2018). The persuasive power of knowledge: Testing the confidence heuristic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(10), 1431–1444.CrossRef Pulford, B. D., Colman, A. M., Buabang, E. K., & Krockow, E. M. (2018). The persuasive power of knowledge: Testing the confidence heuristic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(10), 1431–1444.CrossRef
go back to reference Rains, S. A. (2007). The impact of anonymity on perceptions of source credibility and influence in computer-mediated group communication: A test of two competing hypotheses. Communication Research, 34(1), 100–125.CrossRef Rains, S. A. (2007). The impact of anonymity on perceptions of source credibility and influence in computer-mediated group communication: A test of two competing hypotheses. Communication Research, 34(1), 100–125.CrossRef
go back to reference Semin, G. R., & Fiedler, K. (1988). The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 558–568.CrossRef Semin, G. R., & Fiedler, K. (1988). The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 558–568.CrossRef
go back to reference Siering, M., Muntermann, J., & Rajagopalan, B. (2018). Explaining and predicting online review helpfulness: The role of content and reviewer-related signals. Decision Support Systems, 108, 1–12.CrossRef Siering, M., Muntermann, J., & Rajagopalan, B. (2018). Explaining and predicting online review helpfulness: The role of content and reviewer-related signals. Decision Support Systems, 108, 1–12.CrossRef
go back to reference Sparks, J. R., & Areni, C. S. (2008). Style versus substance: Multiple roles of language power in persuasion. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(1), 37–60.CrossRef Sparks, J. R., & Areni, C. S. (2008). Style versus substance: Multiple roles of language power in persuasion. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(1), 37–60.CrossRef
go back to reference Srull, T. K., Lichtenstein, M., & Rothbart, M. (1985). Associative storage and retrieval processes in person memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11(2), 316–345. Srull, T. K., Lichtenstein, M., & Rothbart, M. (1985). Associative storage and retrieval processes in person memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11(2), 316–345.
go back to reference Teng, L., & Laroche, M. (2007). Building and testing models of consumer purchase intention in competitive and multicultural environments. Journal of Business Research, 60(3), 260–268.CrossRef Teng, L., & Laroche, M. (2007). Building and testing models of consumer purchase intention in competitive and multicultural environments. Journal of Business Research, 60(3), 260–268.CrossRef
go back to reference Winter, S., Krämer, N. C., Rösner, L., & Neubaum, G. (2015). Don’t keep it (too) simple: How textual representations of scientific uncertainty affect layperson’s attitudes. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 34(3), 252–272.CrossRef Winter, S., Krämer, N. C., Rösner, L., & Neubaum, G. (2015). Don’t keep it (too) simple: How textual representations of scientific uncertainty affect layperson’s attitudes. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 34(3), 252–272.CrossRef
go back to reference Wu, L., Shen, H., Fan, A., & Mattila, A. S. (2017). The impact of language style on consumers’ reactions to online reviews. Tourism Management, 59, 590–596.CrossRef Wu, L., Shen, H., Fan, A., & Mattila, A. S. (2017). The impact of language style on consumers’ reactions to online reviews. Tourism Management, 59, 590–596.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Effects of linguistic style on persuasiveness of word-of-mouth messages with anonymous vs. identifiable sources
Authors
Jie Chen
Wenjian Fan
Junlong Wei
Zunli Liu
Publication date
10-12-2021
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Marketing Letters / Issue 4/2022
Print ISSN: 0923-0645
Electronic ISSN: 1573-059X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-021-09602-7