1 Introduction
With consumers’ rising awareness of poverty, climate change, environmental pollution, and unethical production conditions, practitioners increasingly add and communicate altruistic product attributes that primarily benefit others (e.g., donations or animal welfare) to their products. Still, the impact of these attributes on consumers’ decision-making is not evident. While research suggests altruistic attributes are welcomed by consumers (Chernev & Blair,
2015; Romani et al.,
2013), consumers often do not have stable preferences and rely on context to determine their decision (Amir & Levav,
2008; Bettman et al.,
1998; Huber et al.,
1982; Tversky & Simonson,
1993). However, whether and how the presence of altruistic attributes influences how consumers make trade-offs is unclear. We address this gap by focusing on the compromise effect (Kim & Kim,
2016; Simonson,
1989) and demonstrating the general difficulty that consumers experience in accepting trade-offs for altruistic attributes.
Prior research on the compromise effect focuses almost exclusively on trade-offs between two utilitarian product attributes (hereafter, UU trade-offs), with a notable exception of considering hedonic ones (Kim & Kim,
2016). However, products increasingly feature altruistic attributes, such as fair trade or charitable donations. Consequently, consumers often face trade-offs between two altruistic product attributes (hereafter, AA trade-offs) and trade-offs between altruistic and utilitarian attributes (hereafter, AU trade-offs) in many purchase decisions (Lin & Chang,
2012).
This research sheds light on trade-offs involving altruistic attributes and their influence on the compromise effect. First, we examine AA trade-offs, replicating the well-established compromise effect. Next, we demonstrate that the compromise effect diminishes and even reverses in AU trade-offs. Finally, we address the moderating role of impression management.
2 Theoretical background and hypotheses
The compromise effect refers to the tendency of individuals to prefer a middle or compromise option over extreme alternatives, even when the extreme options are more favorable, i.e., higher quality or value (Simonson,
1989). Uncertainty is the key reason consumers prefer a middle option. In particular, the uncertainty of decision-making revolves around justifying a decision towards oneself or others due to uncertain preferences (Simonson,
1989; Wernerfelt,
1995). Choosing compromises reduces the associated risk and complexity of trade-off decisions, thus lightening the perceived decision difficulty (Dhar & Simonson,
2003; Luce et al.,
2001).
Prior research on potential moderators of the compromise effect is represented by two major literature streams, focusing on choice framing and personal characteristics. Choice framing refers to how choice options are presented to the consumer, such as the availability of a no-choice option (e.g., Dhar & Simonson,
2003), availability of time pressure (e.g., Dhar et al.,
2000) or binding settings (e.g., Müller et al.,
2012). Increasingly, studies focus on the influence of personal characteristics on the compromise effect, such as self-regulatory focus (Mourali et al.,
2007), mood (Lin et al.,
2006), brand familiarity (Sinn et al.,
2007), maximizing tendencies (Mao,
2016), the need for justification (Chernev,
2005; Simonson,
1989), or the need to provide reasons (Briley et al.,
2000). This study contributes to the emerging third stream that started to investigate product-related factors (Kim,
2017; Kim & Kim,
2016). For example, Kim and Kim (
2016) detect stronger compromise effects in utilitarian (vs. hedonic) consumption situations.
We distinguish between altruistic and utilitarian product attributes. We define altruistic product attributes as primarily benefiting others (Melnyk et al.,
2022). These benefits can occur directly (e.g., donations to charity, support of animal welfare) or indirectly (e.g., contribution to environmental protection). Altruistic product attributes convey information, such as pollution, working conditions, or animal welfare, that is perceived as abstract and distant to the self but benefitting others (White et al.,
2019). Despite being commonly quantifiable (e.g., amount of a donation, carbon footprint of a product), consumers often struggle to evaluate such attributes (Grinstein et al.,
2018).
In contrast, utilitarian product attributes are driven by functionality goals and are evaluated primarily based on functional, instrumental, and practical benefits to consumers (e.g., Kim & Kim,
2016; Melnyk et al.,
2022). The functional nature of such attributes (e.g., printing speed, optical zoom) makes them relatively easy to understand and compare.
We expect consumers to have difficulty accepting and making trade-offs for altruistic attributes. Consumers search for reasons to justify an alternative towards others or oneself (Simonson,
1989). Altruistic product attributes often involve the prevention of ‘bad things’ (e.g., poverty or pollution) or directly helping others, making such trade-offs harder to justify relative to common utilitarian attributes. For instance, it is harder to justify an alternative that compromises charity donations for environmental protection than an alternative that compromises printing quality for printing speed.
Moreover, when a choice benefits others, social norms become more impactful (Melnyk et al.,
2022), accentuating a moral component that resists compromise. Therefore, consumers find it challenging to weigh altruistic attributes against each other and make compromises. In contrast, consumers are used to making trade-offs regarding utilitarian attributes, e.g., giving up quality for a lower price. Consequently, we expect consumers’ decisions in AA trade-offs to result less often in the compromise compared to UU trade-offs.
H1
: The compromise effect will be stronger under a utilitarian (UU) versus an altruistic (AA) trade-off.
Nowadays, besides UU and AA trade-offs, consumers also face AU trade-offs in many purchase decisions (Lin & Chang,
2012), pitting individual practical goals (e.g., car functionality) against collective or other-benefitting goals (e.g., environmental protection). Thus, consumers are torn between utilitarian product alternatives, serving self-interest, and altruistic product alternatives (Schuitema & de Groot,
2015).
While altruistic product attributes improve consumers’ liking and product evaluation (Arora & Henderson,
2007; Chernev & Blair,
2015), consumers are often unwilling to trade altruistic for utilitarian attributes, particularly regarding functionality (Barone et al.,
2000; Luchs & Kumar,
2017). Hence, AU trade-offs create social dilemmas implying high decision difficulty. To address this dilemma, consumers must first prioritize between utilitarian and altruistic goals. This initial choice defines subsequent choice behavior. Importantly, such a two-step process for trade-off decisions increases the likelihood of choosing an extreme option due to the amplification of the consumers’ initial preferences relative to a one-step choice process (Lei & Zhang,
2021).
Moreover, altruistic attributes have a moral component that resists compromise. Those who value the moral code are unwilling to diminish it for a utilitarian benefit. Those who favor utilitarian benefits tend to disregard the entire moral code, also ignoring its levels.
Consequently, we expect consumers in AU trade-offs to use a two-step process in their decision-making. First, consumers must decide on the dominant attribute type, i.e., the utilitarian or altruistic attribute. Second, consumers stick to their dominant attribute type and choose the more extreme option regarding this attribute. Regardless of the choice in the first step, the two-step process is likely to result in choosing an extreme alternative (Lei & Zhang,
2021), which contradicts the well-established compromise effect. Thus, we expect the compromise effect in AU trade-offs to turn negative, leading to a reverse compromise effect, i.e., an increase in preference for extreme options.
H2
: The compromise effect will be stronger under a utilitarian (UU) and reverses under an altruistic-utilitarian (AU) trade-off.
Expanding upon the need for self- or other-justification (Simonson,
1989; Wernerfelt,
1995), we expect that a need for impression management amplifies these preference structures. High impression management refers to using deliberate and strategic behaviors to create a positive image of oneself to others, while low impression management involves minimal use of such behaviors and prioritizes authenticity (Peloza et al.,
2013; Schlenker,
1980).
Impression management theory states that individuals are generally motivated to make a good impression on others and strive to present themselves positively (Goffman,
1959). Consumers tend to choose products that align with their desired self-image and conform to the behaviors of their favored in-group while avoiding products associated with distanced out-groups (Cialdini & Goldstein,
2004; Peloza et al.,
2013; Schlenker,
1980).
When consumers engage in impression management, they are more likely to choose alternatives that are intermediate between two extremes in UU trade-offs because they may be perceived as more balanced and easier to justify (Peloza et al.,
2013; Simonson,
1989). Consequently, impression management is likely to enhance the compromise effect, promoting middle compared to extreme options. Thus, we expect a stronger compromise effect in UU trade-offs for consumers with a high need for impression management.
In contrast, we expect a stronger preference for extreme options in AU trade-offs for consumers with a high need for impression management. In such situations, to create a positive image of themselves to others, consumers must first identify the dominant attribute type, i.e., utilitarian or altruistic, of their favored in-group and then choose the alternative with the most extreme expression of this attribute as they want to gain admiration (Cialdini & Goldstein,
2004). If they expect the utilitarian attribute to be dominant, they will compromise on the altruistic attribute, and vice versa (Lei & Zhang,
2021; Peloza et al.,
2013). This rationale implies that choosing the middle option may have strategic negative impacts. Consequently, in AU trade-offs, individuals with a high need for impression management are more likely to choose the alternative with the most extreme expression of the dominant attribute.
H3
: Impression management moderates the compromise effect under utilitarian (UU) and altruistic-utilitarian (AU) trade-offs.
4 General discussion
This research demonstrates that the compromise effect varies systematically depending on the trade-off type and that consumers face general difficulty accepting trade-offs for altruistic attributes. While the well-established compromise effect predicts a preference for middle options over extreme options in UU trade-offs, we show that the compromise effect is systematically stronger for UU compared to AA trade-offs. Further, we provide pioneering evidence that consumers prefer extreme options in AU trade-offs and replicate these findings across different product categories. These findings have several theoretical and managerial implications.
First, we uncovered a previously unaddressed boundary condition of the compromise effect, i.e., the presence of altruistic attributes, which becomes increasingly common in the marketplace. By investigating the compromise effect in UU, AA, and AU trade-offs and demonstrating the general difficulty consumers face accepting trade-offs for altruistic attributes, we contribute to the emerging literature on consumer reactions to altruistic attributes (Melnyk et al.,
2022; Pfeifer & Schreiner,
2024), as well as research on the product-related attributes of the compromise effect (Kim,
2017; Kim & Kim,
2016). Second, we investigate the moderating role of impression management. In particular, we show that impression management increases the compromise effect for UU trade-offs and decreases the compromise effect for AU trade-offs. Finally, our results suggest that consumers may solve AU trade-offs in two steps instead of directly trading off altruistic and utilitarian attributes, ultimately resulting in a lexicographic processing of the attributes, contributing to the literature on the routes consumers take to reach their final choice (Lei & Zhang,
2021).
The findings offer important insights for brand managers, highlighting potential unintended consequences of communicating altruistic product attributes. From a practical perspective, implications derived from the well-established compromise effect are no longer valid when altruistic product attributes are present in the decision-making process. Hence, marketers should anticipate preference for extreme options in their assortment when communicating both utilitarian and altruistic attributes. For instance, our results suggest that highlighting altruistic attributes can make more self-focused consumers prefer extreme utilitarian options. At the same time, marketers should consider the moderating role of impression management and the related social norm (Melnyk et al.,
2022) by emphasizing the attributes most important to the corresponding in-groups (e.g., “most successful millennials prefer …”).
To summarize, we provide the first evidence of the interplay of utilitarian and altruistic product attributes in the compromise effect, ultimately resulting in preferences for extreme options. This research’s limitations offer opportunities for future research to address the underlying mechanisms (e.g., manipulation of impression management) of the uncovered effect and potentially extend to other context effects. Another promising avenue is the role of altruistic values and product experience on the compromise effects in the presence of altruistic trade-offs. Finally, like many studies on context effects, we did not measure actual buying behavior. Hence, the robustness of our results, including alternative manipulation of impression management, should be tested based on consequential buying behavior.
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.