Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Political Behavior 2/2011

01-06-2011 | Original Paper

In the Eye of the Beholder? Motivated Reasoning in Disputed Elections

Authors: Kyle C. Kopko, Sarah McKinnon Bryner, Jeffrey Budziak, Christopher J. Devine, Steven P. Nawara

Published in: Political Behavior | Issue 2/2011

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

This study uses an experimental design to simulate the ballot counting process during a hand-recount after a disputed election. Applying psychological theories of motivated reasoning to the political process, we find that ballot counters’ party identification conditionally influences their ballot counting decisions. Party identification’s effect on motivated reasoning is greater when ballot counters are given ambiguous, versus specific, instructions for determining voter intent. This study’s findings have major implications for ballot counting procedures throughout the United States and for the use of motivated reasoning in the political science literature.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
The data reported by Jenkins (2004, 117) suggest that there were as many as 471 disputed election cases in the US House where election officials could have influenced the election’s outcome. Similarly, there were 60 disputed elections in the US Senate during this period in which election administrators could have affected the outcome (Jenkins 2005, 59).
 
2
Unfortunately, there does not appear to be systematic data available on state legislative and local disputed elections. However, given the frequency of contested congressional, gubernatorial, and other statewide elections, and the substantially larger number of state legislative and local elected offices, disputed elections across all levels of government are not uncommon occurrences.
 
3
For example, the state of Oregon allows a great degree of freedom in determining voter intent—“Any vote from which it is impossible to determine the elector’s choice for the office or measure may not be counted” (ORS 254 § 505). In New Hampshire, a majority vote by poll workers is enough to determine voter intent—“If a ballot is marked for any office in a way which does not readily admit of counting … then the ballot shall be counted for that office in accordance with the majority vote of the election officials present and counting votes…” (RSA 63 § 659:64). In Michigan, even if voter intent can be discerned, a ballot could be rejected if a voter fails to make the proper “X” or check mark on the ballot—“Marks other than crosses or check marks used to designate the intention of the voter shall not be counted” (MCL 116 § 803.1(c)). Thus, there are many different standards for determining voter intent and counting ambiguous ballots.
 
4
Aside from polls workers, county/state boards of election are important players in disputed elections. If a candidate challenges the initial ballot-counting decision of poll workers, it is possible for a county/state board of election (or a court) to review these decisions. These individuals may or may not be political elites. For example, in Ohio, the county board of election is comprised of four individuals, selected and equally balanced among the major political parties (R.C. 3501.06–3501.07). In Pennsylvania, a county board of elections is comprised of the county commissioners, who are elected officials who represent the two major political parties (25 P.S. §§ 2641–2642). Additionally, membership on the state board of canvassers must be evenly divided between the major political parties in Michigan (MCL § 168.22), and nominees to the state canvassing board are submitted by the central committees of the major political parties (Ibid.). A similar law applies to the state board of elections in North Carolina (N.C.G.S. § 163-19), and the county boards of election in New York (New York Election Law § 3–204). Thus, in several states, members of boards of elections are either selected by or obtained their positions with significant support from the major political parties. As such, these individuals should be likely to support the interests of their political party when reviewing disputed ballots.
 
5
The data reported in the 2008 Election Administration and Voting Survey sponsored by the US Election Assistance Commission shows that 10.5% of all poll workers surveyed nationally were under the age of 25 (US Election Assistance Commission 2008). These data a similar to another 2008 national survey of poll workers conducted by the Election Administration Research Center at the University of California, Berkeley and the Verified Voting Foundation. In this survey, 11.1% of poll workers surveyed were under the age of 29 (Election Administration Research Center 2008).
 
7
Challenges came from either of the two major party candidates on the ballot. Two other minor party candidates also appeared on the ballot. Participants cast only four of 6,816 total votes in favor of the two minor party candidates (we assume this to be a function of respondent error). Participants’ choices were therefore restricted to awarding challenged ballots to one of the two candidates involved in the challenges or discarding the ballot altogether.
 
8
In Ohio, college students are permitted to list a school address when registering to vote (http://​www.​sos.​state.​oh.​us/​SOS/​elections/​voterInformation​/​regToVote.​aspx), July 12, 2010.
 
9
Kilroy and Stivers engaged in an expensive campaign, spending a combined $5 million, making it the most expensive 2008 House race in Ohio. July 12, 2010. http://​www.​fec.​gov/​DisclosureSearch​/​HSProcessCandLis​t.​do?​ull.
 
10
Participants were presented with a female candidate only in the real candidate condition. While presenting these participants with a female candidate might seem problematic because participants in the other candidate conditions were presented only with male candidates, Kilroy’s inclusion was necessary to test Hypotheses 2. We control for the gender of participants in our statistical models. Models estimated without a gender control are not substantively different.
 
11
These ballot guides are not assumed to be representative of all possible state ballot guidelines. They are simply examples of two different ways states choose to inform ballot counters and are intended to represent two points on a hypothetical “ambiguity” continuum of recount guides.
 
12
The Minnesota 2008 Recount Guide, July 12, 2010: http://​www.​sos.​state.​mn.​us/​Modules/​ShowDocument.​aspx?​documentid=​5236. Specifically, we provided respondents with portions of section 13, entitled “Determining Voter Intent” (8–10). See the “Web Appendix” for more details.
 
13
The North Carolina standards, July 12, 2010: http://​www.​sboe.​state.​nc.​us/​getdocument.​aspx?​id=​219. See the “Web Appendix” for more details.
 
14
Copies of the ballot counting rules and the ballots themselves are in the online “Web Appendix”.
 
15
These nine questions included the five standard political knowledge questions advocated by Delli Carpini and Keeter (1997), as well as four additional and similar questions. We included the four extra questions in order to create more variation among participants.
 
16
Pooling the data into a single model and using several interactions would provide the most efficient test of the equality (or lack thereof) of these coefficients. Unfortunately, due to severe multicollinearity between the necessary interaction and lower-order terms we cannot follow this approach. However, Allison (1999, 186) shows that properly specified interactive models and separate regressions are equivalent. Hoetker (2007) demonstrates that although problems of residual variation can occur when comparing logit and probit coefficients, “the researcher can—at a minimum—compare the statistical significance of the coefficients across groups” (338). This is the approach we follow here.
 
17
Ballot counters used in the analysis were coded as female with mean levels of knowledge, trust, cognition, and closure.
 
18
This figure was taken from the 2008 Minnesota US Senate race. After the recount, Franken held a 225 vote lead, although his lead was expanded to 312 votes after further legal action.
 
Literature
go back to reference Agrawal, N., & Maheswaran, D. (2005). Motivated reasoning in outcome-bias effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 798–805.CrossRef Agrawal, N., & Maheswaran, D. (2005). Motivated reasoning in outcome-bias effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 798–805.CrossRef
go back to reference Allison, P. D. (1999). Comparing logit and probit coefficients across groups. Sociological Methods & Research, 28, 198–208.CrossRef Allison, P. D. (1999). Comparing logit and probit coefficients across groups. Sociological Methods & Research, 28, 198–208.CrossRef
go back to reference Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2006). See what you want to see: Motivational influences on visual perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 612–625.CrossRef Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2006). See what you want to see: Motivational influences on visual perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 612–625.CrossRef
go back to reference Braman, E., & Nelson, T. E. (2007). Mechanism of motivated reasoning? Analogical perception in US discrimination disputes. American Journal of Political Science, 51, 940–956.CrossRef Braman, E., & Nelson, T. E. (2007). Mechanism of motivated reasoning? Analogical perception in US discrimination disputes. American Journal of Political Science, 51, 940–956.CrossRef
go back to reference Bruner, J. S., & Goodman, C. C. (1947). Value and need as organizing factors in perception. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 42, 33–44.CrossRef Bruner, J. S., & Goodman, C. C. (1947). Value and need as organizing factors in perception. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 42, 33–44.CrossRef
go back to reference Bruner, J. S., & Mintern, L. A. (1955). Perceptual identification and conceptual organization. Journal of General Psychology, 53, 21–28.CrossRef Bruner, J. S., & Mintern, L. A. (1955). Perceptual identification and conceptual organization. Journal of General Psychology, 53, 21–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–131.CrossRef Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley. Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Chen, S. H., Shechter, D. E., & Chaiken, S. (1996). Getting at the truth or getting along: Accuracy- versus impression- motivated heuristic and systematic processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 262–275.CrossRef Chen, S. H., Shechter, D. E., & Chaiken, S. (1996). Getting at the truth or getting along: Accuracy- versus impression- motivated heuristic and systematic processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 262–275.CrossRef
go back to reference Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. A. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent (pp. 206–261). New York: The Free Press. Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. A. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent (pp. 206–261). New York: The Free Press.
go back to reference Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1997). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven: Yale University Press. Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1997). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.
go back to reference Dyck, J. J., & Seabrook, N. R. (2009). The problem with vote-by-mail. Paper presented at the 2009 Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association. Chicago, IL. Dyck, J. J., & Seabrook, N. R. (2009). The problem with vote-by-mail. Paper presented at the 2009 Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association. Chicago, IL.
go back to reference Erikson, R. S., & Tedin, K. L. (2007). American public opinion (7th ed.). New York: Pearson Longman. Erikson, R. S., & Tedin, K. L. (2007). American public opinion (7th ed.). New York: Pearson Longman.
go back to reference Fischle, M. (2000). Mass response to the Lewinsky scandal: Motivated reasoning or Bayesian updating? Political Psychology, 21(1), 135–159.CrossRef Fischle, M. (2000). Mass response to the Lewinsky scandal: Motivated reasoning or Bayesian updating? Political Psychology, 21(1), 135–159.CrossRef
go back to reference Foley, E. B. (2008). The original Bush v. Gore: An historical perspective on disputed elections. University Distinguished Lecture delivered at The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio. Foley, E. B. (2008). The original Bush v. Gore: An historical perspective on disputed elections. University Distinguished Lecture delivered at The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio.
go back to reference Foley, E. B. (2009). Gubernatorial contests. Lecture delivered at the past, present, and future of disputed elections, sponsored by The University of Miami Law Review, University of Miami, Miami, FL. Foley, E. B. (2009). Gubernatorial contests. Lecture delivered at the past, present, and future of disputed elections, sponsored by The University of Miami Law Review, University of Miami, Miami, FL.
go back to reference Green, D. P., & Palmquist, B. (1994). How stable is party identification? Political Behavior, 16, 437–466.CrossRef Green, D. P., & Palmquist, B. (1994). How stable is party identification? Political Behavior, 16, 437–466.CrossRef
go back to reference Green, D. P., Palmquist, B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds: Political parties and the social identities of voters. New Haven: Yale University Press. Green, D. P., Palmquist, B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds: Political parties and the social identities of voters. New Haven: Yale University Press.
go back to reference Greene, S. (2004). Social identity theory and party identification. Social Science Quarterly, 65, 136–153.CrossRef Greene, S. (2004). Social identity theory and party identification. Social Science Quarterly, 65, 136–153.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoetker, G. (2007). The use of logit and probit models in strategic management research: Critical issues. Strategic Management Journal, 28, 331–343.CrossRef Hoetker, G. (2007). The use of logit and probit models in strategic management research: Critical issues. Strategic Management Journal, 28, 331–343.CrossRef
go back to reference Jenkins, J. A. (2004). Partisanship and contested election cases in the House of Representatives, 1789–2002. Studies in American Political Development, 18, 112–135.CrossRef Jenkins, J. A. (2004). Partisanship and contested election cases in the House of Representatives, 1789–2002. Studies in American Political Development, 18, 112–135.CrossRef
go back to reference Jenkins, Jeffrey. A. (2005). Partisanship and contested election cases in the Senate, 1789–2002. Studies in American Political Development, 19, 53–74.CrossRef Jenkins, Jeffrey. A. (2005). Partisanship and contested election cases in the Senate, 1789–2002. Studies in American Political Development, 19, 53–74.CrossRef
go back to reference Klaczynski, P. A., & Robinson, B. (2000). Personal theories, intellectual ability, and epistemological beliefs: Adult age differences in everyday reasoning biases. Psychology and Aging, 15, 400–416.CrossRef Klaczynski, P. A., & Robinson, B. (2000). Personal theories, intellectual ability, and epistemological beliefs: Adult age differences in everyday reasoning biases. Psychology and Aging, 15, 400–416.CrossRef
go back to reference Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: “Seizing and freezing”. Psychological Review, 103, 263–283.CrossRef Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: “Seizing and freezing”. Psychological Review, 103, 263–283.CrossRef
go back to reference Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 480–498.CrossRef Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 480–498.CrossRef
go back to reference Lewis-Beck, M., Norpoth, H., Jacoby, W., Converse, P., & Weisberg, H. (2008). The American voter revisited. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press. Lewis-Beck, M., Norpoth, H., Jacoby, W., Converse, P., & Weisberg, H. (2008). The American voter revisited. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press.
go back to reference Redlawsk, D. (2002). Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. Journal of Politics, 64, 1021–1044. Redlawsk, D. (2002). Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. Journal of Politics, 64, 1021–1044.
go back to reference Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in big five personality traits across 55 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 168–182.CrossRef Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in big five personality traits across 55 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 168–182.CrossRef
go back to reference Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 515–530.CrossRef Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 515–530.CrossRef
go back to reference Stuart, G. (2004). Databases, felons, and voting: Bias and partisanship of the Florida felon list in the 2000 Elections. Political Science Quarterly, 119, 453–475.CrossRef Stuart, G. (2004). Databases, felons, and voting: Bias and partisanship of the Florida felon list in the 2000 Elections. Political Science Quarterly, 119, 453–475.CrossRef
go back to reference Taber, C., Cann, D., & Kucsova, S. (2009). The motivated processing of political arguments. Political Behavior, 31, 137–155.CrossRef Taber, C., Cann, D., & Kucsova, S. (2009). The motivated processing of political arguments. Political Behavior, 31, 137–155.CrossRef
go back to reference Taber, C., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755–769.CrossRef Taber, C., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755–769.CrossRef
go back to reference Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049–1062.CrossRef Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049–1062.CrossRef
go back to reference Wells, C., Reedy, J., Gastil, J., & Lee, C. (2009). Information distortion and voting choices: The origins and effects of factual beliefs in initiative elections. Political Psychology, 30(6), 953–969.CrossRef Wells, C., Reedy, J., Gastil, J., & Lee, C. (2009). Information distortion and voting choices: The origins and effects of factual beliefs in initiative elections. Political Psychology, 30(6), 953–969.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
In the Eye of the Beholder? Motivated Reasoning in Disputed Elections
Authors
Kyle C. Kopko
Sarah McKinnon Bryner
Jeffrey Budziak
Christopher J. Devine
Steven P. Nawara
Publication date
01-06-2011
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Political Behavior / Issue 2/2011
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-010-9133-x

Other articles of this Issue 2/2011

Political Behavior 2/2011 Go to the issue