Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 1/2021

01-10-2020 | Original Paper

Movement Synchrony in the Mirror Game

Authors: Rinat Feniger-Schaal, Désirée Schönherr, Uwe Altmann, Bernhard Strauss

Published in: Journal of Nonverbal Behavior | Issue 1/2021

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

The significance of the body-mind link and the contribution of nonverbal communication in the context of psychotherapy is enjoying increased interest. Yet, the main measurements used in psychotherapy studies rely mostly on verbal measures, missing other channels of communication that would allow rich and implicit, nonverbal information. The present study is a first step toward using the mirror game (MG) as a standardized mimicry task that involves the assessment of movement synchrony as a nonverbal outcome measure for psychotherapy research. The aims of the study were twofold: (a) to investigate the possibility of automated analysis of synchrony in the MG, detecting the different roles of leadership in the game, and (b) to validate the synchrony quantified by computer algorithms with a human-rated prosocial scale. All participants (N = 33) played the MG with a gender-matched expert player. Body motions of interacting pairs were assessed using motion energy analysis. Frequency of movement synchrony was computed by windowed cross-lagged correlation and a peak-picking algorithm. Independent observers rated items with respect to prosocial parameters, using the MG scales. The algorithm allowed to differentiate quite accurately between the various roles of leadership. Significant correlations were found between the MG human-rated scales and computer-detected synchrony. High frequency of synchronized movements was linked with high levels of having fun, shared affect, and reference to the other, and with low negative affect. The MG provides a standardized and flexible paradigm for the investigation of movement synchrony. The use of both automated detection and human rating reveals the potential application of the MR in research and clinical use.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Footnotes
1
“Frame-differencing method“is a technical term in computer vision science. It means that two subsequent video frames are analysed. In the case of MEA: the number of pixels whose color intensity changed from t to t + 1 is counted.
 
2
“Grey intensity“is a technical term of computer vision science. A video consists of an ordered series of frames. A frame is a digital picture that consists of pixel. In a color video, a pixel consists of the intensity of red, blue and green. In black and white videos, the pixel consists only the grey intensity. Moreover, in a color video, we can convert the intensity of red, blue, and green into one grey value. This is done in MEA.
 
Literature
go back to reference Altmann, U. (2011). Investigation of movement synchrony using windowed crosslagged regression. In A. Esposito, A. Vinciarelli, K. Vicsi, C. Pelachaud, & A. Nijholt (Eds.), Analysis of verbal and nonverbal communication and enactment: The processing issue (pp. 344–354). Berlin: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-25775-9_31.CrossRef Altmann, U. (2011). Investigation of movement synchrony using windowed crosslagged regression. In A. Esposito, A. Vinciarelli, K. Vicsi, C. Pelachaud, & A. Nijholt (Eds.), Analysis of verbal and nonverbal communication and enactment: The processing issue (pp. 344–354). Berlin: Springer. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​978-3-642-25775-9_​31.CrossRef
go back to reference Altmann, U. (2013). Synchronisation nonverbalen Verhaltens: Weiterentwicklung und Anwendung zeitreihenanalytischer Identifikationsverfahren. [Synchronization of nonverbal behavior: Development and Application of time series analysis methods]. Berlin: Springer. Altmann, U. (2013). Synchronisation nonverbalen Verhaltens: Weiterentwicklung und Anwendung zeitreihenanalytischer Identifikationsverfahren. [Synchronization of nonverbal behavior: Development and Application of time series analysis methods]. Berlin: Springer.
go back to reference Altmann, U., Schoenherr, D., Paulick, J., Deisenhofer, A. K., Schwartz, B., Rubel, J. A., & Strauss, B. (2019). Associations between movement synchrony and outcome in patients with social anxiety disorder: Evidence for treatment specific effects. Psychotherapy Research, 1–17. Altmann, U., Schoenherr, D., Paulick, J., Deisenhofer, A. K., Schwartz, B., Rubel, J. A., & Strauss, B. (2019). Associations between movement synchrony and outcome in patients with social anxiety disorder: Evidence for treatment specific effects. Psychotherapy Research, 1–17.
go back to reference Bente, G., Donaghy, W. C., & Suwelack, D. (1998). Sex differences in body movement and visual attention: An integrated analysis of movement and gaze in mixed-sex dyads. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22(1), 31–58. Bente, G., Donaghy, W. C., & Suwelack, D. (1998). Sex differences in body movement and visual attention: An integrated analysis of movement and gaze in mixed-sex dyads. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22(1), 31–58.
go back to reference Berliner, P. F. (2009). Thinking in jazz: The infinite art of improvisation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Berliner, P. F. (2009). Thinking in jazz: The infinite art of improvisation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(2), 243–253. Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(2), 243–253.
go back to reference Boal, A. (2013). The rainbow of desire: The Boal method of theatre and therapy. Abingdon: Routledge. Boal, A. (2013). The rainbow of desire: The Boal method of theatre and therapy. Abingdon: Routledge.
go back to reference Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893.PubMed Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893.PubMed
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum Associates. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155.PubMed Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155.PubMed
go back to reference Feldman, R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(3–4), 329–354.PubMed Feldman, R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(3–4), 329–354.PubMed
go back to reference Feniger-Schaal, R., Hart, Y., Lotan, N., Koren-Karie, N., & Noy, L. (2018). The body speaks: Using the mirror game to link attachment and non-verbal behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1560.PubMedPubMedCentral Feniger-Schaal, R., Hart, Y., Lotan, N., Koren-Karie, N., & Noy, L. (2018). The body speaks: Using the mirror game to link attachment and non-verbal behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1560.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Feniger-Schaal, R., & Lotan, N. (2017). The embodiment of attachment: Directional and shaping movements in adults’ mirror game. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 53, 55–63. Feniger-Schaal, R., & Lotan, N. (2017). The embodiment of attachment: Directional and shaping movements in adults’ mirror game. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 53, 55–63.
go back to reference Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2010). Nonverbal synchrony or random coincidence? How to tell the difference. In C. N. A. Esposito, C. Vogel, A. Hussain, & A. Nijholt (Eds.), Development of multimodal interfaces: Active listening and synchrony (Vol. 5967, pp. 182–196). Berlin: Springer. Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2010). Nonverbal synchrony or random coincidence? How to tell the difference. In C. N. A. Esposito, C. Vogel, A. Hussain, & A. Nijholt (Eds.), Development of multimodal interfaces: Active listening and synchrony (Vol. 5967, pp. 182–196). Berlin: Springer.
go back to reference Gaggioli, A., Falletta, E. M., Ferrise, F., Graziosi, S., Gallace, A., D’Ausilio, A., et al. (2019). Effects of interpersonal sensorimotor synchronization on dyadic creativity: Gender matters. Frontiers in psychology, 9, 2604.PubMedPubMedCentral Gaggioli, A., Falletta, E. M., Ferrise, F., Graziosi, S., Gallace, A., D’Ausilio, A., et al. (2019). Effects of interpersonal sensorimotor synchronization on dyadic creativity: Gender matters. Frontiers in psychology, 9, 2604.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Gaziv, G., Noy, L., Liron, Y., & Alon, U. (2017). A reduced-dimensionality approach to uncovering dyadic modes of body motion in conversations. PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0170786.PubMedPubMedCentral Gaziv, G., Noy, L., Liron, Y., & Alon, U. (2017). A reduced-dimensionality approach to uncovering dyadic modes of body motion in conversations. PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0170786.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Gueugnon, M., Salesse, R. N., Coste, A., Zhao, Z., Bardy, B. G., & Marin, L. (2016). The acquisition of socio-motor improvisation in the mirror game. Human Movement Science, 46, 117–128.PubMed Gueugnon, M., Salesse, R. N., Coste, A., Zhao, Z., Bardy, B. G., & Marin, L. (2016). The acquisition of socio-motor improvisation in the mirror game. Human Movement Science, 46, 117–128.PubMed
go back to reference Harrigan, J., Rosenthal, R., Scherer, K. R., & Scherer, K. (Eds.). (2008). New handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Harrigan, J., Rosenthal, R., Scherer, K. R., & Scherer, K. (Eds.). (2008). New handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Harrist, A. W., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (1994). Dyadic synchrony in mother-child interaction: Relation with children’s subsequent kindergarten adjustment. Family Relations, 417–424. Harrist, A. W., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (1994). Dyadic synchrony in mother-child interaction: Relation with children’s subsequent kindergarten adjustment. Family Relations, 417–424.
go back to reference Hart, Y., Noy, L., Feniger-Schaal, R., Mayo, A. E., & Alon, U. (2014b). Individuality and togetherness in joint improvised motion. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e87213.PubMedPubMedCentral Hart, Y., Noy, L., Feniger-Schaal, R., Mayo, A. E., & Alon, U. (2014b). Individuality and togetherness in joint improvised motion. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e87213.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27(6), 949–960. Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27(6), 949–960.
go back to reference Isabella, R. A., & Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the origins of infant-mother attachment: A replication study. Child Development, 62(2), 373–384.PubMed Isabella, R. A., & Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the origins of infant-mother attachment: A replication study. Child Development, 62(2), 373–384.PubMed
go back to reference Julien, D., Brault, M., Chartrand, É., & Bégin, J. (2000). Immediacy behaviours and synchrony in satisfied and dissatisfied couples. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, 32(2), 84. Julien, D., Brault, M., Chartrand, É., & Bégin, J. (2000). Immediacy behaviours and synchrony in satisfied and dissatisfied couples. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, 32(2), 84.
go back to reference Kreyenbrink, I., Joraschky, P., Konstantinidis, I., Neumann, N., & Lausberg, H. (2017). Nonverbales Verhalten von Patienten mit sozialen Phobien und ihren Therapeuten in psychodynamischen Psychotherapien (Teilprojekt SOPHO-NET). Zeitschrift für Psychosomatische Medizin und Psychotherapie, 63(3), 297–313.PubMed Kreyenbrink, I., Joraschky, P., Konstantinidis, I., Neumann, N., & Lausberg, H. (2017). Nonverbales Verhalten von Patienten mit sozialen Phobien und ihren Therapeuten in psychodynamischen Psychotherapien (Teilprojekt SOPHO-NET). Zeitschrift für Psychosomatische Medizin und Psychotherapie, 63(3), 297–313.PubMed
go back to reference Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Rebing, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27(3), 145–162. Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Rebing, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27(3), 145–162.
go back to reference Leander, N. P., Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (2012). You give me the chills: Embodied reactions to inappropriate amounts of behavioral mimicry. Psychological Science, 23(7), 772–779.PubMed Leander, N. P., Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (2012). You give me the chills: Embodied reactions to inappropriate amounts of behavioral mimicry. Psychological Science, 23(7), 772–779.PubMed
go back to reference Lindsey, E. W., Colwell, M. J., Frabutt, J. M., Chambers, J. C., & MacKinnon-Lewis, C. (2008). Mother-child dyadic synchrony in European American and African American families during early adolescence: Relations with self-esteem and prosocial behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54(3), 289–315. Lindsey, E. W., Colwell, M. J., Frabutt, J. M., Chambers, J. C., & MacKinnon-Lewis, C. (2008). Mother-child dyadic synchrony in European American and African American families during early adolescence: Relations with self-esteem and prosocial behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54(3), 289–315.
go back to reference Lindsey, E. W., Cremeens, P. R., & Caldera, Y. M. (2010). Mother–child and father–child mutuality in two contexts: Consequences for young children’s peer relationships. Infant and Child Development, 19(2), 142–160. Lindsey, E. W., Cremeens, P. R., & Caldera, Y. M. (2010). Mother–child and father–child mutuality in two contexts: Consequences for young children’s peer relationships. Infant and Child Development, 19(2), 142–160.
go back to reference Lindsey, E. W., Cremeens, P. R., Colwell, M. J., & Caldera, Y. M. (2009). The structure of parent–child dyadic synchrony in toddlerhood and children’s communication competence and Self-control. Social Development, 18(2), 375–396. Lindsey, E. W., Cremeens, P. R., Colwell, M. J., & Caldera, Y. M. (2009). The structure of parent–child dyadic synchrony in toddlerhood and children’s communication competence and Self-control. Social Development, 18(2), 375–396.
go back to reference McDowall, J. J. (1978). Interactional synchrony: A reappraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(9), 963–975. McDowall, J. J. (1978). Interactional synchrony: A reappraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(9), 963–975.
go back to reference McGarry, L. M., & Russo, F. A. (2011). Mirroring in dance/movement therapy: Potential mechanisms behind empathy enhancement. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 38(3), 178–184. McGarry, L. M., & Russo, F. A. (2011). Mirroring in dance/movement therapy: Potential mechanisms behind empathy enhancement. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 38(3), 178–184.
go back to reference Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 13–20. Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 13–20.
go back to reference Noy, L., Dekel, E., & Alon, U. (2011). The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics of two people improvising motion together. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(52), 20947–20952. Noy, L., Dekel, E., & Alon, U. (2011). The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics of two people improvising motion together. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(52), 20947–20952.
go back to reference Noy, L., Levit-Binun, N., & Golland, Y. (2015). Being in the zone: physiological markers of togetherness in joint improvisation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 187.PubMedPubMedCentral Noy, L., Levit-Binun, N., & Golland, Y. (2015). Being in the zone: physiological markers of togetherness in joint improvisation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 187.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Paulick, J., Deisenhofer, A.-K., Ramseyer, F., Tschacher, W., Boyle, K., Rubel, J., et al. (2018a). Nonverbal synchrony: A new approach to better understand psychotherapeutic processes and drop-out. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 28(3), 367–384. https://doi.org/10.1037/int0000099.CrossRef Paulick, J., Deisenhofer, A.-K., Ramseyer, F., Tschacher, W., Boyle, K., Rubel, J., et al. (2018a). Nonverbal synchrony: A new approach to better understand psychotherapeutic processes and drop-out. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 28(3), 367–384. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​int0000099.CrossRef
go back to reference Paulick, J., Rubel, J. A., Deisenhofer, A. K., Schwartz, B., Thielemann, D., Altmann, U., et al. (2018b). Diagnostic features of nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: Comparing depression and anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42(5), 539–551. Paulick, J., Rubel, J. A., Deisenhofer, A. K., Schwartz, B., Thielemann, D., Altmann, U., et al. (2018b). Diagnostic features of nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: Comparing depression and anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42(5), 539–551.
go back to reference Rabinowitch, T. C., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2017). Synchronized movement experience enhances peer cooperation in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 160, 21–32.PubMed Rabinowitch, T. C., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2017). Synchronized movement experience enhances peer cooperation in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 160, 21–32.PubMed
go back to reference Rennung, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2016). Prosocial consequences of interpersonal synchrony. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 224(3), 168–189.PubMedPubMedCentral Rennung, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2016). Prosocial consequences of interpersonal synchrony. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 224(3), 168–189.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 969–992. Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 969–992.
go back to reference Schechner, R. (1994). Environmental theater. Wisconsin: Hal Leonard Corporation. Schechner, R. (1994). Environmental theater. Wisconsin: Hal Leonard Corporation.
go back to reference Schoenherr, D., Paulick, J., Strauss, B. M., Deisenhofer, A. K., Schwartz, B., Rubel, J. A., et al. (2019a). Identification of movement synchrony: Validation of windowed cross-lagged correlation and-regression with peak-picking algorithm. PLoS ONE, 14(2), e0211494.PubMedPubMedCentral Schoenherr, D., Paulick, J., Strauss, B. M., Deisenhofer, A. K., Schwartz, B., Rubel, J. A., et al. (2019a). Identification of movement synchrony: Validation of windowed cross-lagged correlation and-regression with peak-picking algorithm. PLoS ONE, 14(2), e0211494.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Schoenherr, D., Paulick, J., Strauss, B., Deisenhofer, A.-K., Schwartz, B., Rubel, J., Altmann, U. (2019b). Nonverbal synchrony predicts premature termination of psychotherapy for social phobic patients. Psychotherapy (Chicago, III.), Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/pst0000216. Schoenherr, D., Paulick, J., Strauss, B., Deisenhofer, A.-K., Schwartz, B., Rubel, J., Altmann, U. (2019b). Nonverbal synchrony predicts premature termination of psychotherapy for social phobic patients. Psychotherapy (Chicago, III.), Advance online publication. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​pst0000216.
go back to reference Seham, A. E. (2001). Whose improv is it anyway? Beyond Second City. Mississippi: Univ. Press of Mississippi. Seham, A. E. (2001). Whose improv is it anyway? Beyond Second City. Mississippi: Univ. Press of Mississippi.
go back to reference Sharon-David, H., Mizrahi, M., Rinott, M., Golland, Y., & Birnbaum, G. E. (2018). Being on the same wavelength: Behavioral synchrony between partners and its influence on the experience of intimacy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 1–26. Sharon-David, H., Mizrahi, M., Rinott, M., Golland, Y., & Birnbaum, G. E. (2018). Being on the same wavelength: Behavioral synchrony between partners and its influence on the experience of intimacy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 1–26.
go back to reference Słowiński, P., Alderisio, F., Zhai, C., Shen, Y., Tino, P., Bortolon, C., et al. (2017). Unravelling socio-motor biomarkers in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia, 3(1), 8.PubMed Słowiński, P., Alderisio, F., Zhai, C., Shen, Y., Tino, P., Bortolon, C., et al. (2017). Unravelling socio-motor biomarkers in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia, 3(1), 8.PubMed
go back to reference Spolin, V. (1999). Improvisation for the theater. Evanston: Northwestern University Press. Spolin, V. (1999). Improvisation for the theater. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.
go back to reference Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the construction of relationships. Relationships and development, 51, 72. Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the construction of relationships. Relationships and development, 51, 72.
go back to reference Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11(2), 262–266.PubMed Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11(2), 262–266.PubMed
go back to reference Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(4), 693–695. Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(4), 693–695.
go back to reference Van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami, K., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 15(1), 71–74.PubMed Van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami, K., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 15(1), 71–74.PubMed
go back to reference Varlet, M., Marin, L., Capdevielle, D., Del-Monte, J., Schmidt, R., Salesse, R., et al. (2014). Difficulty leading interpersonal coordination: towards an embodied signature of social anxiety disorder. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 29.PubMedPubMedCentral Varlet, M., Marin, L., Capdevielle, D., Del-Monte, J., Schmidt, R., Salesse, R., et al. (2014). Difficulty leading interpersonal coordination: towards an embodied signature of social anxiety disorder. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 29.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Vicaria, I. M., & Dickens, L. (2016). Meta-analyses of the intra-and interpersonal outcomes of interpersonal coordination. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40(4), 335–361. Vicaria, I. M., & Dickens, L. (2016). Meta-analyses of the intra-and interpersonal outcomes of interpersonal coordination. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40(4), 335–361.
go back to reference Watanabe, T. (1983). A study of motion-voice synchronization. Bulletin of JSME, 26(222), 2244–2250. Watanabe, T. (1983). A study of motion-voice synchronization. Bulletin of JSME, 26(222), 2244–2250.
go back to reference Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20(1), 1–5.PubMed Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20(1), 1–5.PubMed
go back to reference Zhai, C., Alderisio, F., Tsaneva-Atanasova, K., and di Bernardo, M. (2014). A novel cognitive architecture for a human-like virtual player in the mirror game. In 2014 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics (SMC). San Diego, CA (pp. 754–759). https://doi.org/10.1109/smc.2014.6974001. Zhai, C., Alderisio, F., Tsaneva-Atanasova, K., and di Bernardo, M. (2014). A novel cognitive architecture for a human-like virtual player in the mirror game. In 2014 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics (SMC). San Diego, CA (pp. 754–759). https://​doi.​org/​10.​1109/​smc.​2014.​6974001.
Metadata
Title
Movement Synchrony in the Mirror Game
Authors
Rinat Feniger-Schaal
Désirée Schönherr
Uwe Altmann
Bernhard Strauss
Publication date
01-10-2020
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior / Issue 1/2021
Print ISSN: 0191-5886
Electronic ISSN: 1573-3653
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-020-00341-3

Other articles of this Issue 1/2021

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 1/2021 Go to the issue