Skip to main content
Top
Published in:

Open Access 14-10-2024

National differences in gambling-driven stock trading behavior: evidence from a simulated trading game

Authors: Moritz Mosenhauer, Jakob Windisch

Published in: Financial Markets and Portfolio Management | Issue 4/2024

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

There are significant differences in real-life gambling behavior between Germany and Italy. Contemporary theories suggest that these differences in individual preferences would translate to differences in trading behavior. We elicited trading behavior from participants of both Italian and German language groups using a stylized hold-or-trade scenario distributed via a survey. Additionally, we collected data on their self-stated gambling preferences. Our findings confirm the existence of large and significant differences in gambling consumption, problem gambling, and trading behavior between the two nationalities. Furthermore, we observed that gambling preferences explain most of the national differences. This suggests that established factors of financial decision-making play a more prominent role in explaining national differences in trading behavior than any latent cultural factors.
Notes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

1 Introduction

The accumulation of wealth is a crucial objective for private households, as it enables them to make profitable investments, such as purchasing a car, pursuing educational opportunities, or smoothing their consumption over economic downturns and retirement. In many economies where house prices are soaring, the equity market remains the only accessible avenue for long-term investment, particularly for those with limited savings. However, numerous studies show that private households often make mistakes when trading on equity markets, resulting in suboptimal or even negative returns (see Barber et al. 2013, forareview).
In this study, we focus on one trading anomaly commonly known as ’excessive trading,’ as identified by Barber and Odean (2000). Private households have a tendency to excessively trade by frequently selling shares and buying into new ones. Since they often lack the ability to distinguish winning from losing stocks, the trading activity does not lead to changes in the gross returns of their portfolio. However, fees associated with these trades (e.g., commission fees or bid-ask spreads) can significantly reduce their net returns, leading to suboptimal performance.1 A number of studies (Dorn and Sengmueller 2009; Grinblatt and Keloharju 2009) identify an individual’s tendency to gamble as an important factor contributing to excessive trading. Furthermore, even when controlling for other influencing factors, Liu et al. (2022) find that this relationship remains relevant
Drawing on previous research, we aim to conduct a national comparison to investigate the relationship between gambling behavior and the tendency to trade excessively in different European countries. There is substantial variation in gambling activity across European countries, and Italy has a much more pronounced gambling sector than Germany, as evidenced by average citizen spending of over 2000€ per year in Italy, compared to just over 600€ in Germany (Agenzia delle Dogane 2019, 2020; Statista 2021). We aim to examine whether these national differences in gambling behavior translate into differences in the tendency to engage in excessive trading across these two countries.
We conducted a survey to quantitatively answer our research question. Participants play a non-incentivized trading game following the method of Mosenhauer (2021), which features a stylized and simplified hold-or-trade scenario. For this task, participants are instructed to aim for the highest possible returns. Based on real-world historical stock prices, participants are randomly assigned a stock and can choose to keep it to earn its returns or exchange it for another one at a known commission fee. The participants’ behavior yielded metrics of their trading activity across trading rounds and profitability (see Sect. 3 for a detailed description of the design). We correlate this behavior with personal characteristics obtained from a follow-up questionnaire, which includes two established constructs of gambling activity: the 'Consumption Screen for Problem Gambling' (Rockloff 2012) and the 'Problem Gambling Severity Index' (Miller et al. 2013; Loo et al. 2011). We supplement these data with demographics, most notably the participants’ nationality grouped into German-speaking (Germany, Austria and German-speaking South Tyrol) and Italian-speaking (Italy except South Tyrol) participants.
We obtain 55 responses from participants belonging to the German language group (mainly Austria and Germany) and 36 from Italy. Regarding both our survey metrics on gambling activity, we confirm large differences in gambling behavior between the national groups, with Italians reporting a much higher affinity to gambling. As predicted by our theory, we also find that Italians show a significantly higher turnover in the trading game. Interestingly, while both the 'Consumption Screen for Problem Gambling' and the 'Problem Gambling Severity Index' predict trading activity across national groups, we find that the 'Problem Gambling Severity Index' dominates the 'Consumption Screen for Problem Gambling' when considered as determinants simultaneously. This suggests that gambling on stock markets should rather be seen as a harmful addiction than a utility-generating entertainment. Lastly, we confirm that the tendency to gamble explains most of the observed national differences in trading activity to the point where the participant’s nation loses significance as an explanatory variable at the 5% level when controlling for gambling preferences. Our results thus suggest that cultural differences between Italians and German-speaking individuals, apart from gambling habits, can be disregarded when predicting the discussed trading behavior.
We believe that our results have important implications, particularly for the policymaking of the Italian government. Grinblatt and Keloharju (2009) suggest that both traditional forms of gambling and highly active stock trading are motivated by a drive for 'sensation seeking.' Gao and Lin (2015) further suggest that individuals with such a drive will attempt to satisfy their 'gambling fix' but are willing to substitute among different forms of gambling, i.e., they may engage in less day trading when lottery jackpots are high. Similarly, Kumar et al. (2021) found that gambling in stock markets increased strongly when casinos were forced to close in response to the contact restrictions brought on by the Covid-19 epidemic. All this implies that if the Italian government aims to curtail the harmful consequences of gambling activity, policies should target the root of the problem, namely an individual’s drive to gamble, rather than its symptoms. Moreover, the results suggest that an important avenue of wealth creation for Italians is severely undermined as long as national preferences toward gambling remain unchanged.

2 Background and literature review

The central hypothesis of this paper is that there are differences in trading behavior between German and Italian language groups, which are related to their respective gambling behavior. To outline possible correlations, we distinguish two strands of literature. In past studies, evidence for positive correlations (Dorn and Sengmueller 2009; Grinblatt and Keloharju 2009; Mosenhauer et al. 2021) between gambling and trading can be found, describing how a person’s preferences for sensation-seeking affect both their gambling and trading behavior. A second strand documents negative correlations (Dorn et al. 2015; Gao and Lin 2015), describing the substitutability of gambling and some forms of stock trading. After a brief description of the state of gambling in the discussed nations, we discuss the related literature.

2.1 Gambling in Italy and Germany

When comparing the gambling activity of Italy and Germany, differences can be observed. In 2019, the gambling sector in Italy recorded a total turnover of 110.5 billion euros (Agenzia delle Dogane 2019). In comparison, Germany only spent 44.23 billion euros (Statista 2021). This shows that expenditure in Italy is more than twice as high. However, when taking into account the adult population entitled to gamble, the differences become even clearer. In Italy, the number of adult citizens is 49.8 million (ISTAT 2021), while in Germany this figure is 69.4 million (DESTATIS 2021). Hence, there are significant differences in per capita spending on gambling across the two countries: the average adult citizen in Italy spends 2215€ per year on gambling, while a German citizen spends only 637€. This represents over 70% less spending than their Italian counterpart.

2.2 Effect of an individual’s gambling preferences on trading behavior

According to Grinblatt and Keloharju (2009), gambling and trading attract people with similar character traits. Individuals who are prone to sensation seeking are more likely to gamble, attempting to chase the next big win. The same principle guides the trading decisions of sensation seekers, making them more likely to trade excessively than the average population. This connection can be found especially in high-risk investments in financial markets (Delfabbro et al. 2021). Dorn and Sengmueller (2009) demonstrate that the most active traders turn over their portfolios at an annual rate of more than 250%. In addition, Mosenhauer et al. (2021) directly attest to a linkage between problem gambling and trading frequency from self-stated data. Standard reasoning, such as re-balancing, risk management, or savings, cannot fully explain such trading behavior. In general, the turnover rate of people who report that they enjoy trading is twice as high as that of the average private investor (Dorn and Sengmueller 2009).

2.3 Different channels of gambling can act as substitutes

Certain financial market products, especially those involving high risk, are often considered substitutes for gambling. Speculative trading shares similarities with gambling: many decisions are often based on incomplete information, short-term profit hopes are usually the motivation to participate, and highly volatile and unpredictable results are the consequence (Delfabbro et al. 2021). For instance, Gao and Lin (2015) demonstrate that on days when the state lottery jackpot exceeds 500 million Taiwan Dollars, trading volume in the country’s financial markets decreases by an average of 7.2%. A similar phenomenon can be observed in the USA and Germany. Increases in state lottery jackpots coincide with a significant reduction in participation in small transactions on the stock market. However, these variations mainly affect activity in risk-intensive assets such as options and individual stocks, but not bonds, mutual funds, or pension funds; here, activity remains unchanged regardless of the lottery jackpot (Dorn et al. 2015).

3 Methods

This study uses a survey to investigate how national differences in gambling behavior drive differences in trading behavior. It employs a convenience sample of self-selected participants. Using a link, anyone could participate in a survey created in German, Italian, and English, from which potential participants could choose as per their preference. The link was shared via email among students of the authors’ institution and via social media. We expect that some of the participants invited others to participate, too.
Following Mosenhauer (2021), the survey experiment simulates trading decisions based on past returns in an abstracted hold-or-trade scenario. In ten different rounds of trading, participants are assigned random stocks from the 'Standard & Poor’s 500' index in a randomly determined month within the time period from January 1960 to July 2017. The only information available to the participants was the stock’s past performance in the last 10 months. In each round, the participant has to make a binary decision to either hold the allotted stock or trade it. If the participant decides to hold the assigned stock, they would simply earn the real-world returns of that stock during the allotted month. On the other hand, if the participant decides to trade their stock, they are assigned another random stock in a random month out of the basket. Additionally, a commission fee of 5% (percentage points) is deducted from the yielded returns of the new stock. So, in case of trading stocks, the participant receives a random return and an additional − 5% fee. The returns of all trading rounds are accumulated and displayed at the end of the experiment, and participants are instructed to try to maximize this number, although no monetary or other external incentives were provided. In any case, each round starts with a new stock allocation, and trading rounds were independent in the sense that a participant’s decisions in a given round could never influence another round.
To measure gambling behavior in the sample, two established benchmarks, the 'Consumption Screen for Problem Gambling' (Rockloff 2012) and 'Problem Gambling Severity Index' (Miller et al. 2013; Loo et al. 2011), are included in the questionnaire. The Consumption Screen for Problem Gambling (CSPG) is based on the conceptual analogue 'Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test—Consumption' (AUDIT-C) (Daniel R et al. 1998), which measures gambling consumption rather than the negative consequences caused by gambling. Participants answer three brief multiple-choice questions regarding their gambling behavior, and according to their answers, they are assigned a score ranging from 0 (lowest) to 4 or 5 (highest). The Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI) is another benchmark used in this study to measure gambling consumption and study problem gambling further (Miller et al. 2013; Loo et al. 2011). Similar to the CSPG, this benchmark also uses 9 multiple-choice questions with items on an ordinal scale. The items are associated with scores from 0 to 3, which are accumulated at the end.
Preceding the information discussed above, participants are asked to provide some personal information including their gender, age, and language affiliation. Gender is coded as a dichotomous variable using 0 and 1, while age is reported as a numerical value by the participants. We code the participants’ nationalities in groups according to their self-stated mother tongue. The variable nation takes the value 0 if a participant states that their mother tongue is German and 1 if a participant states that their mother tongue is Italian. We mainly choose this grouping to account for participants with an Italian nationality, including both the German-speaking region of South Tyrol and all other regions within Italy-speaking Italian.
The trading experiment takes place after the personal questions, where the decision to trade (1) or hold (0) the stock is coded as the dichotomous variable Trading. To measure gambling behavior, the two benchmarks mentioned above are used with CSPG scores of 4+ considered indicative of problematic gambling. The PGSI, on the other hand, categorizes the population into four different groups: non-problem gambler (score 0), low-risk gambler (score 1–2), moderate-risk gambler (score 3–7), and problem gambler (score above 8). While the categories themselves are irrelevant for further analysis, as the score is used in its numeric value, they are useful for quickly evaluating an individual’s gambling behavior. Both scores are divided by the maximum possible score to normalize them, resulting in the final variables named CSPG and PGSI.
The sample consists of a total of 106 participants. The sample consists of 55 male and 37 female participants; additionally 14 participants did not declare their gender. Diverse gender has not been recorded. Of the 106 participants, 55 belonged to the German language group, 36 to the Italian language group, and 1 to another language group. 14 persons did not provide any information on their language affiliation or dropped out of the questionnaire before this information was collected. Table 1 shows some key descriptive statistics of the sample.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics
 
Mean
Std. dev.
Min.
Max.
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
Panel A: trading outcomes
    
Net returns
−12.53
32.94
−80.95
58.98
Trading
0.33
0.17
0
0.7
Panel B: individual characteristics
    
PGSI
0.03
0.08
0
0.37
CSPG
0.09
0.14
0
0.62
Male
59.78%
0
1
Age (in years)
38.02
12.92
19
68
This table provides descriptive statistics for some key variables within the sample. Trading outcomes are noted at the decision level (10 per participant), while individual characteristics are stated at the participant level. Net returns measure the participant’s final performance in percentage after deducting trading fees. Trading is a binary variable, measuring 1 if a participant chooses to trade. PGSI is also a binary variable, measuring 1 for the highest possible outcome for problem gambling and 0 for not gambling at all. CSPG, analogous to PGSI, measures gambling behavior in a range between 0 and 1, where 1 denotes the highest possible gambling activity and 0 denotes non-gambling. Male is a binary variable, measuring 1 for self-identified males and 0 for all other genders

4 Results

To test for differences between Austrians or Germans, on the one hand, and Italians, on the other hand, in their gambling and trading behavior, we use two-sample t-tests. We test for differences in the PGSI variable (for gambling behavior) and TRADE variable (for trading behavior) with respect to groups formed according to the NATION variable. As shown in Table 2, both tests reveal significant national differences at \(\alpha <0.01\).
Comparing the arithmetic means of the PGSI variable between German-speaking and Italian-speaking participants, we find that the Italian average (PGSI = 0.0824) is almost 8 times higher than the average of German-speaking participants (PGSI = 0.0105). The TRADE variable takes on an average value of 29.31% for German-speaking participants and an average value of 41.25% for Italian-speaking participants. This difference is highly significant with a p-value of 0.002, falling within the 99% range. The results of these two t-tests are shown graphically in Fig. 1. We thus confirm national differences within the sample in both gambling and trading behavior.
Table 2
Results of t-Test for national differences in gambling and trading
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
 
average
T
dof
alternative
p-val
 
German
Italian
    
PGSI
0.0105
0.0824
2.90
22.28
Two-sided
0.0080
TRADE
0.2931
0.4125
−3.09
459.14
Two-sided
0.0020
This table provides the results of a T-test comparing trading- and gambling activity of German- and Italian-speaking participants
The differences in trading behavior can be further analyzed with a regression analysis. Table 3 presents the coefficient estimates from various logit regression models. Different columns correspond to different model specifications. In each case, trade is the dependent variable, while we interchange different combinations of nation, PGSI, and CSPG as independent variables. Our unit of observation is a participant’s trading decision in a given round of the trading game. Each participant provided ten such decisions.
The first three columns of Table 3 present the respective univariate regressions of TRADE on NATION, PGSI, and CSPG. The first column displays a significant coefficient of 0.1193 (\(\alpha <0.01\)), indicating an 11.93% increase in trading activity among Italian-speaking participants compared to their German counterparts. This result is consistent with the previously conducted t-test. The influence of problem gambling (PGSI) is highly significant with a p-value of 0.004%. Multiplying its regression coefficient with the respective standard deviation reveals that 'problem gamblers' trade on average 7.54% more than those who do not gamble. The influence of gambling activity in general (CSPG) is also significant with a p-value of 1.6%. Analogous to the previous calculation, the standardized effect of CSPG gambling consumption on trading activity is 6.02%.
The fourth column in Table 3 presents the results of a logit regression analysis that examines the influences of the two variables on trading behavior. Interestingly, it shows that the variable measuring general gambling activity, CSPG, no longer has a significant effect on trading behavior when controlling for PGSI (p-value = 0.8267). Moreover, the coefficient is now negative, indicating that individuals with high levels of general gambling activity tend to trade less. This result is not consistent with the univariate regression analysis conducted between CSPG and TRADE. On the other hand, the variable PGSI increases its influence on trading behavior to a coefficient of 1.0647 and remains significant. This suggests that the effect of general gambling activity is 'absorbed' by pathological gambling and that it is not gambling in general, but rather pathological gambling, that has a significant influence on trading behavior. This finding highlights the importance of considering different levels of gambling behavior when examining its relationship with trading behavior.
Table 3
Coefficient estimates of different logit regression models estimated at the decision level
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
 
Trading
Trading
Trading
Trading
Trading
Nation
0.574\(^{***}\)
   
0.336
 
(3.30)
   
(1.71)
PGSI
 
4.509\(^{***}\)
 
4.906\(^{*}\)
3.505\(^{**}\)
  
(3.89)
 
(2.22)
(2.71)
CSPG
  
1.846\(^{**}\)
−0.234
 
   
(3.23)
(−0.21)
 
N
640
640
630
630
640
\(R^{2}\)
0.0133
0.0190
0.0129
0.0193
0.0225
This table provides the coefficient estimates of the conducted logit regressions. On the x-axis, different conducted regression analysis are shown, while on the y-axis the independent variables are situated. We conduct all analyses at the decision level. The asterisks represent 3 different levels of significance (*0.05, **0.01, ***0.001), while the t-statistics for each coefficient is written in parentheses below. The two bottom rows show the number of observations (N) and the regression fit, represented by the pseudo \(R^{2}\), for each regression model
Lastly, we test for interdependencies between the participants’ nationality, gambling behavior, and trading behavior. Since PSGI dominates CSPG in its effect on trading activity, we will not consider CSPG further as a measure of gambling activity. Column five of Table 3 shows the effect of nation on trade while controlling for gambling activity. Strikingly, we find that the influence of the nation variable on trading behavior falls to 7.37% from 11.93% when considered in isolation. The influence of gambling also diminishes in magnitude.
We take this as evidence that the measured effects overlap, indicating that some of the observed effect of national differences on trading behavior can be attributed to differences in gambling behavior among participants. Crucially, PGSI appears to have a more dominant effect than nation, as the effect of nation does not reach significance at the 0.05% level when controlling for gambling preferences while problem gambling remains significant at the 0.01% level. This provides further evidence that the effect on trading observed for language affiliation is primarily due to the variable PGSI or problem gambling and that the main factor influencing excessive trading is not language affiliation but rather the participants’ involvement in the gambling market.
It should be noted that our categorization of nationalities according to participants’ mother tongues may not perfectly capture their cultural affiliation. Some participants may have grown up speaking one language but due to migration lived large shares of their lives in countries speaking another language, so that cultural backgrounds are mixed. We partly attempt to account for such dynamics by recoding our nation variable according to the language each participant chose for answering the survey (e.g., German, Italian, or English). No participants with German as their mother tongue answered the survey in Italian, while two participants with Italian as their mother tongue answered the survey in German. 8 participants answered in English. We generate a new nation variable by dropping all English-answered entries and coding all answers provided in Italian with 1 and 0 for the answers in German. Using this new nation variable, we rerun our main regression exercise shown in Table 3 and find that our findings qualitatively remain unchanged except that the effect of a participant’s language affiliation on trading activity, while sharply decreasing in magnitude, remains significant at the 5% level when controlling for problem gambling.

5 Conclusion

The study aims to investigate the national differences between German- and Italian-speaking countries in Europe regarding their trading activity on a stylized stock market, with a particular focus on whether the gambling preferences of individuals could explain these observed differences. The research confirms substantial and significant differences in both gambling and trading behavior across the language groups. Moreover, gambling-motivated trading should be seen as an addiction rather than entertainment. The findings suggest that the observed national differences are mostly due to differences in gambling preferences rather than other cultural attributes.
We believe that these findings have significant implications for households for two reasons. Firstly, they highlight that pathological gambling not only has a direct negative impact on household finances, but it also leads to excessive trading behavior, which further depletes the household budget (Barber and Odean 2000). Secondly, the research underscores the importance of educating individuals not only about financial literacy but also about the consequences of gambling, especially pathological gambling. The regression analyses presented in Table 2 suggest that gambling behaviors transfer to trading behavior, emphasizing the need to address the issue of pathological gambling to prevent financial losses on the stock market.
For future studies, it would be very interesting to investigate what happens when comparing a group of problem gamblers with a group of non-gamblers without making distinctions based on language. The effects shown could be even stronger and the differences even more pronounced, which would give this study further scientific relevance. Additionally, it would be useful to determine whether pathological gambling genuinely causes excessive behavior or whether excessive behavior conversely increases the tendency toward pathological gambling. This study only showed a positive correlation. Studies with larger, multinational, and multicultural samples could be conducted to serve this purpose.

Acknowledgements

We thank Antje Bierwisch, Yevgen Bogodistov and Martin Dinter for their helpful comments. We thank an anonymous reviewer for their attentive and constructive remarks which helped improve the paper. We have received no funding for our research. All authors affiliated with Management Center Innsbruck.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

None.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Appendix

Appendix A

Demographic data

1.
My gender is
  • Male (1)
  • Female (2)
  • Non-binary/third gender (3)
 
2.
My year of birth is... <insert year of birth>
 
3.
My nationality is
  • Italian (1)
  • German (2)
  • Austrian- or German-speaking South Tyrol (3)
  • Other (4)
    • specify... <insert nationality>
 

Instructions

In this experiment, you will act as a stock trader on a simulated financial market. Please read these instructions carefully as they may be vital for your performance. If you make profitable decisions and earn high returns, your account within the experiment will rise. The goal of this experiment is to achieve the highest returns possible. Please try to keep this objective in mind and try to make your decisions accordingly.
Throughout all rounds, the percentage points of returns which you earn with your trading activity will be added to or deducted from your account. For example, if you finish the first round by earning +3% returns and then finish the following round by earning − 5% returns, you will have accumulated a total of − 2% returns at this stage. If you finish the following round by earning +7% returns, you will have accumulated a total of +5% at this stage and so forth.
The experiment will last a total of 10 rounds. Each of these rounds is played in isolation, meaning that the actions you take in any of the rounds do not affect what happens in any of the other rounds in any way. Also, your actions do not affect what happens to any of the other participants and their actions do not affect you.
You will trade on the basis of real stock data. In each round, you are randomly allocated a stock from the Standard and Poor’s 500-index that you will be holding for a random month during the years 1960–2017 (the Standard and Poor’s 500-index consists of the five hundred largest firms on the American stock market).
As some arbitrary examples, you may be randomly selected to hold stocks from General Motors in August 2014 or Abbott Laboratories in March 1967. The average monthly return for the entire bundle over the entire time is approximately 1.4%. However, depending on which stock you were allocated at which time, your stock may well yield much higher or lower returns than this, including negative returns.
You then have the choice between two Options: OPTION 1 and OPTION 2. Please make sure that you understand the outcomes of these options well, as this is the only way you can affect your earnings within the experiment.
If you choose OPTION 1, you will simply incur the returns that your allocated stock yields for the allocated time period. In terms of the two previous arbitrary examples, this would be the returns of General Motors in August 2014 or Abbott Laboratories in March 1967. If the stock generates positive returns, the percentage points will be added to your account. If the stock generates negative returns, the percentage points will be deducted from your account
If, on the other hand, you choose OPTION 2, your previous stock holdings will be erased and you will be randomly allocated a new stock from the bundle. For this new stock, your account will then rise or fall according to the returns it generates in the same manner as for OPTION 1. However, in order to conduct such an exchange of stocks, a fixed cost of 5% (in percentage points) will be deducted from your earnings for the round in any case. For example, if your newly allocated stock would have generated +4% returns, your trading account would change by − 1% for the round.
Remember that, no matter how you choose, you will not retain any of your stock holdings from one round to another. Instead, you will be randomly allocated a new stock in each following round and the only lasting effect of your actions is on your trading account.
To inform your decision between OPTION 1 and OPTION 2, you will be shown the last ten monthly returns of your allocated stock in the form of a table. Below you see an example of how this will typically look.
Example:
  • 10 months prior: 30.6667
  • 09 months prior: −16.1565
  • 08 months prior: 2.2989
  • 07 months prior: 23.265
  • 06 months prior: −0.5898
  • 05 months prior: 5.9331
  • 04 months prior: −12.0163
  • 03 months prior: −22.6852
  • 02 months prior: 9.2066
  • 01 month prior: −6.3742
That is all. In order for you to acquaint yourself with the mechanism, you will get the chance to play 2 trial trading rounds after these instructions. These two rounds will function exactly like the usual trading rounds, except that no profits or losses from these rounds will affect your trading account for this experiment.
Feel free to reread any part you may not have understood perfectly. Click continue if you are ready for the trial rounds.
4.
I have read and understood the conditions.
  • continue (1)
 
5.
My English is sufficient to grasp the main idea of the experiment.
  • Yes, fully (1)
  • To a great extent (2)
  • No, unfortunately (3)
 

The trading experiment

The following question was shown 10 times to the participants, with values and stocks changing each round randomly.
6.
In this round, your allocated stock yielded the following returns over the past 10 months:
  • Data Block:
  • ${e://Field/ID1_text}: ${e://Field/ID1_value}
  • ${e://Field/PH1_text}: ${e://Field/PH1_value}
  • what is your choice?
    • Option 1
    • Option 2
If the participant selected option 1, the following message was displayed:
  • You chose option 1: 
  • Your stock has generated the following returns:
  • ${e://Field/AP1_value}
If the participant on the other hand selected option 2, the following message was displayed:
  • You chose option 2:
  • Your newly allocated stock has generated the following returns:
  • ${e://Field/AAP1_value}
  • Additionally, five percentage points (5.0) have been deducted from these returns (${e://Field/AAP1_value} −5).
  • Thus a total of $e{${e://Field/AAP1_value} −5} have been added to/ subtracted from your account.
At the end of 10 rounds the final score is displayed to the participants:
  • Your total score is: ${e://Field/ind_score}
 

Follow-up questionnaire

7.
At some point in my life, I held shares of some stock.
  • Yes (1)
  • No (2)
 
8.
I have personally bought or sold shares of some stock during the last twelve months.
  • never (0)
  • 1–10 times (1)
  • 10–20 times (2)
  • more than 20 times (3)
 
9.
Which percentage of your net worth is invested in stocks (approximately)? <move slider accordingly from 0% to 100%>
 
10.
Rank these asset classes from biggest position in your portfolio to smallest position
  • Cash
  • Real estate
  • Equity (f.e. Stocks, Equity Funds, ETF’s, Index Funds)
  • Commodities (f.e. Gold, Silver)
  • Fixed Income (f.e. Bonds, Debt funds, fixed Deposits)
 

Gambling questionnaire

In the following questions you are asked about your gambling activity. Important: the following categories are considered gambling in this study: lottery tickets, instant scratch tickets or raffles, casino (for example poker, black jack, roulette), sports betting, slot machines, bingo.
11.
How often did you gamble in the last 12 months?
  • I have NEVER gambled OR I have not gambled at all in the past 12 months (1)
  • Monthly or less (2)
  • 2 to 4 times a month (3)
  • 2 to 3 times a week (4)
  • 4 to 5 times a week (5)
  • 6 or more times a week (6)
 
12.
How much time did you spend gambling on a typical day in which you gambled in the past 12 months?
  • None or less than 30 min (1)
  • More than 30 min but less than an hour (2)
  • More than 1 h but less than 2 h (3)
  • More than 2 h but less than 3 h (4)
  • More than 3 h (5)
 
13.
How often did you spend more than 2 h gambling (on a single occasion) in the past 12 months?
  • Never (1)
  • Less than monthly (2)
  • Monthly (3)
  • Weekly (4)
  • Daily or almost daily (5)
 
14.
Have you bet more than you could really afford to lose?
  • Never (1)
  • Sometimes (2)
  • Most of the time (3)
  • Always (4)
 
15.
Have you needed to gamble with larger amounts of money to get the same feeling of excitement?
  • Never (1)
  • Sometimes (2)
  • Most of the time (3)
  • Always (4)
 
16.
Have you gone back on another day to try to win back the money you lost?
  • Never (1)
  • Sometimes (2)
  • Most of the time (3)
  • Always (4)
 
17.
Have you borrowed money or sold anything to gamble?
  • Never (1)
  • Sometimes (2)
  • Most of the time (3)
  • Always (4)
 
18.
Have you felt that you might have a problem with gambling?
  • Never (1)
  • Sometimes (2)
  • Most of the time (3)
  • Always (4)
 
19.
Have people criticized your betting or told you that you had a gambling problem, whether or not you thought it was true?
  • Never (1)
  • Sometimes (2)
  • Most of the time (3)
  • Always (4)
 
20.
Have you felt guilty about the way you gamble or what happens when you gamble?
  • Never (1)
  • Sometimes (2)
  • Most of the time (3)
  • Always (4)
 
21.
Has gambling caused you any health problems, including stress or anxiety?
  • Never (1)
  • Sometimes (2)
  • Most of the time (3)
  • Always (4)
 
22.
Has your gambling caused any financial problems for you or your household?
  • Never (1)
  • Sometimes (2)
  • Most of the time (3)
  • Always (4)
 
Footnotes
1
Different authors document the behavioral pattern in different countries. For evidence, see Barber et al. (2008); Linnainmaa et al. (2021); Koestner et al. (2017).
 
Literature
go back to reference Agenzia delle Dogane e dei Monopoli “Libro Blu 2019, Relazione,” (2019) Agenzia delle Dogane e dei Monopoli “Libro Blu 2019, Relazione,” (2019)
go back to reference Agenzia delle Dogane e dei Monopoli, “Libro Blu 2020, Relazione,” (2020) Agenzia delle Dogane e dei Monopoli, “Libro Blu 2020, Relazione,” (2020)
go back to reference Barber, Brad, Odean, Terrance: Trading is hazardous to your wealth: the common stock investment performance of individual investors. The journal of Finance 55(2), 773–806 (2000)CrossRef Barber, Brad, Odean, Terrance: Trading is hazardous to your wealth: the common stock investment performance of individual investors. The journal of Finance 55(2), 773–806 (2000)CrossRef
go back to reference Barber, Brad M., Odean, Terrance: Chapter 22 - The Behavior of Individual Investors. In: Constantinides, George M., Harris, Milton, Stulz, Rene M. (eds.) Handbook of the Economics of Finance, of Handbook of the Economics of Finance. Elsevier (2013) Barber, Brad M., Odean, Terrance: Chapter 22 - The Behavior of Individual Investors. In: Constantinides, George M., Harris, Milton, Stulz, Rene M. (eds.) Handbook of the Economics of Finance, of Handbook of the Economics of Finance. Elsevier (2013)
go back to reference Barber, Brad M., Odean, Terrance, Lee, Yi-Tsung., Liu, Yu-Jane., Odean, Terrance: Just how much do individual investors lose by trading? The Review of Financial Studies 22(2), 609–632 (2008)CrossRef Barber, Brad M., Odean, Terrance, Lee, Yi-Tsung., Liu, Yu-Jane., Odean, Terrance: Just how much do individual investors lose by trading? The Review of Financial Studies 22(2), 609–632 (2008)CrossRef
go back to reference Daniel R, Bush Kristen, Kivlahan, Mary B., McDonell, Stephan D., Fihn, Katharine A.: Bradley, and for the ambulatory care quality improvement project (ACQUIP), “the AUDIT alcohol consumption questions (AUDIT-C): an effective brief screening test for problem drinking,’’. Archives of Internal Medicine 158(16), 1789–1795 (1998)CrossRef Daniel R, Bush Kristen, Kivlahan, Mary B., McDonell, Stephan D., Fihn, Katharine A.: Bradley, and for the ambulatory care quality improvement project (ACQUIP), “the AUDIT alcohol consumption questions (AUDIT-C): an effective brief screening test for problem drinking,’’. Archives of Internal Medicine 158(16), 1789–1795 (1998)CrossRef
go back to reference Delfabbro, Paul, King, Daniel, Williams, Jennifer, Georgiou, Neophytos: Cryptocurrency trading, gambling and problem gambling. Addictive Behaviors 122, 107021 (2021)CrossRef Delfabbro, Paul, King, Daniel, Williams, Jennifer, Georgiou, Neophytos: Cryptocurrency trading, gambling and problem gambling. Addictive Behaviors 122, 107021 (2021)CrossRef
go back to reference Dorn, Anne, Dorn, Daniel, Sengmueller, Paul: Trading as gambling. Management Science 61(10), 2376–2393 (2015)CrossRef Dorn, Anne, Dorn, Daniel, Sengmueller, Paul: Trading as gambling. Management Science 61(10), 2376–2393 (2015)CrossRef
go back to reference Dorn, Daniel, Sengmueller, Paul: Trading as entertainment? Management Science 55(4), 591–603 (2009)CrossRef Dorn, Daniel, Sengmueller, Paul: Trading as entertainment? Management Science 55(4), 591–603 (2009)CrossRef
go back to reference Gao, Xiaohui, Lin, Tse-Chun.: Do individual investors treat trading as a fun and exciting gambling activity? Evidence from repeated natural experiments. The Review of Financial Studies 28(7), 2128–2166 (2015)CrossRef Gao, Xiaohui, Lin, Tse-Chun.: Do individual investors treat trading as a fun and exciting gambling activity? Evidence from repeated natural experiments. The Review of Financial Studies 28(7), 2128–2166 (2015)CrossRef
go back to reference Grinblatt, Mark, Keloharju, Matti: Sensation seeking, overconfidence, and trading activity. The Journal of Finance 64(2), 549–578 (2009)CrossRef Grinblatt, Mark, Keloharju, Matti: Sensation seeking, overconfidence, and trading activity. The Journal of Finance 64(2), 549–578 (2009)CrossRef
go back to reference Koestner, M., Loos, B., Meyer, S., Hackethal, A.: Do individual investors learn from their mistakes? Journal of Business Economics 87, 669–703 (2017)CrossRef Koestner, M., Loos, B., Meyer, S., Hackethal, A.: Do individual investors learn from their mistakes? Journal of Business Economics 87, 669–703 (2017)CrossRef
go back to reference Kumar, Alok., Huong, Nguyen., Talis, Putnins.: “Only Gamble in Town: Stock Market Gambling Around the World and Market Efficiency,” mimeo, (2021) Kumar, Alok., Huong, Nguyen., Talis, Putnins.: “Only Gamble in Town: Stock Market Gambling Around the World and Market Efficiency,” mimeo, (2021)
go back to reference Linnainmaa, Juhani T., Melzer, Brian T., Previtero, Alessandro: The misguided beliefs of financial advisors. The Journal of Finance 76(2), 587–621 (2021)CrossRef Linnainmaa, Juhani T., Melzer, Brian T., Previtero, Alessandro: The misguided beliefs of financial advisors. The Journal of Finance 76(2), 587–621 (2021)CrossRef
go back to reference Liu, Hongqi, Peng, Cameron, Xiong, Wei A., Xiong, Wei: Taming the bias zoo. Journal of Financial Economics 143(2), 716–741 (2022)CrossRef Liu, Hongqi, Peng, Cameron, Xiong, Wei A., Xiong, Wei: Taming the bias zoo. Journal of Financial Economics 143(2), 716–741 (2022)CrossRef
go back to reference Loo, Jasmine MY., Tian PS, Oei, Namrata, Raylu: Psychometric evaluation of the problem gambling severity index-Chinese version (PGSI-C). Journal of Gambling Studies 27(3), 453–466 (2011)CrossRef Loo, Jasmine MY., Tian PS, Oei, Namrata, Raylu: Psychometric evaluation of the problem gambling severity index-Chinese version (PGSI-C). Journal of Gambling Studies 27(3), 453–466 (2011)CrossRef
go back to reference Miller, Natalie V., Currie, Shawn R., Hodgins, David C., Casey, David: Validation of the problem gambling severity index using confirmatory factor analysis and rasch modelling. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 22(3), 245–255 (2013)CrossRef Miller, Natalie V., Currie, Shawn R., Hodgins, David C., Casey, David: Validation of the problem gambling severity index using confirmatory factor analysis and rasch modelling. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 22(3), 245–255 (2013)CrossRef
go back to reference Mosenhauer, Moritz, “Unlearning Investment Biases,” mimeo, 2021 Mosenhauer, Moritz, “Unlearning Investment Biases,” mimeo, 2021
go back to reference Mosenhauer, Moritz, Newall, Philip WS., Walasek, Lukasz: The stock market as a casino: associations between stock market trading frequency and problem gambling. Journal of Behavioral Addictions 10(3), 683–689 (2021)CrossRef Mosenhauer, Moritz, Newall, Philip WS., Walasek, Lukasz: The stock market as a casino: associations between stock market trading frequency and problem gambling. Journal of Behavioral Addictions 10(3), 683–689 (2021)CrossRef
go back to reference Rockloff, Matthew J.: Validation of the consumption screen for problem gambling (CSPG). Journal of Gambling Studies 28(2), 207–216 (2012)CrossRef Rockloff, Matthew J.: Validation of the consumption screen for problem gambling (CSPG). Journal of Gambling Studies 28(2), 207–216 (2012)CrossRef
Metadata
Title
National differences in gambling-driven stock trading behavior: evidence from a simulated trading game
Authors
Moritz Mosenhauer
Jakob Windisch
Publication date
14-10-2024
Publisher
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Published in
Financial Markets and Portfolio Management / Issue 4/2024
Print ISSN: 1934-4554
Electronic ISSN: 2373-8529
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11408-024-00460-7

Premium Partner