Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Technology, Knowledge and Learning 1/2015

01-04-2015 | Work-in-progress

Virtual Experiments or Worked Examples? How to Learn the Control of Variable Strategy

Author: Shiyu Liu

Published in: Technology, Knowledge and Learning | Issue 1/2015

Log in

Activate our intelligent search to find suitable subject content or patents.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

This research investigates the role of virtual experiments and worked examples in the learning of the control of variable strategy (CVS). Sixty-nine seventh-grade students participated in this study over a span of 6 weeks and were engaged in worked example learning and/or virtual experimentation to study the knowledge and procedures associated with CVS. The results show that learning from only worked examples or virtual experiments yields minimal knowledge gain on CVS. While worked examples can be effective learning tools, additional instructional support and hands-on experiences are critical to elicit deeper understanding of CVS. Moreover, virtual experimentation alone is not sufficient given the cognitive demands it requires for students to discover CVS on their own. However, when integrated with worked example learning, conducting virtual experiments with computer visualizations can lead to positive learning outcomes. This work corresponds to the growing research on productive failure, and opens up further discussions on optimal strategies for learning CVS.

Dont have a licence yet? Then find out more about our products and how to get one now:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 390 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe




 

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Literature
go back to reference Alfieri, L., Brooks, P., Aldrich, N., & Tenenbaum, H. (2011). Does discovery-based instruction enhance learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 1–18.CrossRef Alfieri, L., Brooks, P., Aldrich, N., & Tenenbaum, H. (2011). Does discovery-based instruction enhance learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 1–18.CrossRef
go back to reference Ardac, D., & Akaygun, S. (2005). Using static and dynamic visuals to represent chemical change at molecular level. International Journal of Science Education, 27(11), 1269–1298.CrossRef Ardac, D., & Akaygun, S. (2005). Using static and dynamic visuals to represent chemical change at molecular level. International Journal of Science Education, 27(11), 1269–1298.CrossRef
go back to reference Atkinson, R., & Catrambone, R. (2000). Subgoal learning and the effect of conceptual vs. computational equations on transfer. In L. R. Gleitman & A. K. Joshi (Eds.), Proceedings of the 22th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.591–596). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Atkinson, R., & Catrambone, R. (2000). Subgoal learning and the effect of conceptual vs. computational equations on transfer. In L. R. Gleitman & A. K. Joshi (Eds.), Proceedings of the 22th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.591–596). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Atkinson, R., Derry, S., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. (2000). Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70(2), 181–214.CrossRef Atkinson, R., Derry, S., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. (2000). Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70(2), 181–214.CrossRef
go back to reference Carroll, W. (1994). Using worked examples as an instructional support in the algebra classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3), 360–367.CrossRef Carroll, W. (1994). Using worked examples as an instructional support in the algebra classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3), 360–367.CrossRef
go back to reference Catrambone, R., & Holyyoak, K. J. (1989). Overcoming contextual limitations on problem-solving transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15(6), 1147–1156. Catrambone, R., & Holyyoak, K. J. (1989). Overcoming contextual limitations on problem-solving transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15(6), 1147–1156.
go back to reference Catrambone, R., & Yuasa, M. (2006). Acquisition of procedures: The effects of example elaborations and active learning exercises. Learning and Instruction, 16, 139–153.CrossRef Catrambone, R., & Yuasa, M. (2006). Acquisition of procedures: The effects of example elaborations and active learning exercises. Learning and Instruction, 16, 139–153.CrossRef
go back to reference Chen, Z., & Klahr, D. (1999). All other things being equal: Acquisition and transfer of the control of variables strategy. Child Development, 70(5), 1098–1120.CrossRef Chen, Z., & Klahr, D. (1999). All other things being equal: Acquisition and transfer of the control of variables strategy. Child Development, 70(5), 1098–1120.CrossRef
go back to reference Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 5, 145–182.CrossRef Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 5, 145–182.CrossRef
go back to reference Chiu, J., & Linn, M. C. (2012). The role of self-monitoring in learning chemistry with dynamic visualizations. In A. Zohar & Y. J. Dori (Eds.), Metacognition in science education (pp. 133–163). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.CrossRef Chiu, J., & Linn, M. C. (2012). The role of self-monitoring in learning chemistry with dynamic visualizations. In A. Zohar & Y. J. Dori (Eds.), Metacognition in science education (pp. 133–163). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Crippen, K. J., & Earl, B. L. (2004). Considering the efficacy of web-based worked examples in introductory chemistry. Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching, 23(2), 151–167. Crippen, K. J., & Earl, B. L. (2004). Considering the efficacy of web-based worked examples in introductory chemistry. Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching, 23(2), 151–167.
go back to reference Darabi, A. A., Nelson, D. W., & Palanki, S. (2007). Acquisition of troubleshooting skills in a computer simulation: Worked example vs. conventional problem solving instructional strategies. Computers in Human Behavior, 23(4), 1809–1819.CrossRef Darabi, A. A., Nelson, D. W., & Palanki, S. (2007). Acquisition of troubleshooting skills in a computer simulation: Worked example vs. conventional problem solving instructional strategies. Computers in Human Behavior, 23(4), 1809–1819.CrossRef
go back to reference de Jong, T. (1991). Learning and instruction with computer simulations. Education and Computing, 6, 217–229.CrossRef de Jong, T. (1991). Learning and instruction with computer simulations. Education and Computing, 6, 217–229.CrossRef
go back to reference de Jong, T. (2006). Technological advances in inquiry learning. Science, 312, 532–533.CrossRef de Jong, T. (2006). Technological advances in inquiry learning. Science, 312, 532–533.CrossRef
go back to reference Dean, D., & Kuhn, D. (2007). Direct instruction vs. discovery: The long view. Science Education, 91(3), 384–397.CrossRef Dean, D., & Kuhn, D. (2007). Direct instruction vs. discovery: The long view. Science Education, 91(3), 384–397.CrossRef
go back to reference Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Catrambone, R. (2006). Can learning from molar and modular worked examples be enhanced by providing instructional explanations and prompting self-explanations? Learning and Instruction, 16(2), 104–121.CrossRef Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Catrambone, R. (2006). Can learning from molar and modular worked examples be enhanced by providing instructional explanations and prompting self-explanations? Learning and Instruction, 16(2), 104–121.CrossRef
go back to reference Hegarty, M. (2004). Dynamic visualizations and learning: Getting to the difficult questions. Learning and Instruction, 14(3), 343–351.CrossRef Hegarty, M. (2004). Dynamic visualizations and learning: Getting to the difficult questions. Learning and Instruction, 14(3), 343–351.CrossRef
go back to reference Jonassen, D. (1997). Instructional design models for well-structured and III-structured problem-solving learning outcomes. Educational Technology Research and Development, 45(1), 65–94.CrossRef Jonassen, D. (1997). Instructional design models for well-structured and III-structured problem-solving learning outcomes. Educational Technology Research and Development, 45(1), 65–94.CrossRef
go back to reference Jonassen, D. (2011). Supporting problem solving in PBL. Interdisciplinary Journal of Problem-based Learning, 5(2), 8.CrossRef Jonassen, D. (2011). Supporting problem solving in PBL. Interdisciplinary Journal of Problem-based Learning, 5(2), 8.CrossRef
go back to reference Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Sweller, J., & Tuovinen, J. (2001). When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 579–588.CrossRef Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Sweller, J., & Tuovinen, J. (2001). When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 579–588.CrossRef
go back to reference Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26(3), 379–424.CrossRef Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26(3), 379–424.CrossRef
go back to reference Kapur, M. (2010). Productive failure in mathematical problem solving. Instructional Science, 38(6), 523–550.CrossRef Kapur, M. (2010). Productive failure in mathematical problem solving. Instructional Science, 38(6), 523–550.CrossRef
go back to reference Kapur, M. (2012). Productive failure in learning the concept of variance. Instructional Science, 40(4), 651–672.CrossRef Kapur, M. (2012). Productive failure in learning the concept of variance. Instructional Science, 40(4), 651–672.CrossRef
go back to reference Klahr, D., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Dual space search during scientific reasoning. Cognitive Science, 12, 1–48.CrossRef Klahr, D., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Dual space search during scientific reasoning. Cognitive Science, 12, 1–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Klahr, D., & Li, J. (2005). Cognitive research and elementary science instruction: From the laboratory, to the classroom, and back. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 14(2), 217–238.CrossRef Klahr, D., & Li, J. (2005). Cognitive research and elementary science instruction: From the laboratory, to the classroom, and back. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 14(2), 217–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychological Science, 15(10), 661–667.CrossRef Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychological Science, 15(10), 661–667.CrossRef
go back to reference Klahr, D., Triona, L. M., & Williams, C. (2007). Hands on what? The relative effectiveness of physical versus virtual materials in an engineering design project by middle school children. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44(1), 183–203.CrossRef Klahr, D., Triona, L. M., & Williams, C. (2007). Hands on what? The relative effectiveness of physical versus virtual materials in an engineering design project by middle school children. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44(1), 183–203.CrossRef
go back to reference Kohn, A. S. (1993). Preschoolers’ reasoning about density: Will it float? Child Development, 64(6), 1637–1650.CrossRef Kohn, A. S. (1993). Preschoolers’ reasoning about density: Will it float? Child Development, 64(6), 1637–1650.CrossRef
go back to reference Kuhn, D., & Angelev, J. (1976). An experimental study of the development of formal operational thought. Child Development, 47(3), 697–706. Kuhn, D., & Angelev, J. (1976). An experimental study of the development of formal operational thought. Child Development, 47(3), 697–706.
go back to reference Kuhn, D., Iordanou, K., Pease, M., & Wirkala, C. (2008). Beyond control of variables: What needs to develop to achieve skilled scientific thinking? Cognitive Development, 23, 435–451.CrossRef Kuhn, D., Iordanou, K., Pease, M., & Wirkala, C. (2008). Beyond control of variables: What needs to develop to achieve skilled scientific thinking? Cognitive Development, 23, 435–451.CrossRef
go back to reference Lee, K., Nicoll, G., & Brooks, D. (2004). A comparison of inquiry and worked example web-based instruction using physlets. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 13(1), 81–88.CrossRef Lee, K., Nicoll, G., & Brooks, D. (2004). A comparison of inquiry and worked example web-based instruction using physlets. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 13(1), 81–88.CrossRef
go back to reference Linn, M. C., Chang, H.-Y., Chiu, J. L., Zhang, H., & McElhaney, K. (2010). Can desirable difficulties overcome deceptive clarity in scientific visualizations? In A. Benjamin (Ed.), Successful remembering and successful forgetting: A Festschrift in honor of Robert A. Bjork (pp. 239–262). New York, NY: Routledge. Linn, M. C., Chang, H.-Y., Chiu, J. L., Zhang, H., & McElhaney, K. (2010). Can desirable difficulties overcome deceptive clarity in scientific visualizations? In A. Benjamin (Ed.), Successful remembering and successful forgetting: A Festschrift in honor of Robert A. Bjork (pp. 239–262). New York, NY: Routledge.
go back to reference Linn, M., Clark, D., & Slotta, J. (2003). WISE design for knowledge integration. Science Education, 87(4), 517–538.CrossRef Linn, M., Clark, D., & Slotta, J. (2003). WISE design for knowledge integration. Science Education, 87(4), 517–538.CrossRef
go back to reference Matlen, B. J., & Klahr, D. (2013). Sequential effects of high and low instructional guidance on children’s acquisition of experimentation skills: Is it all in the timing? Instructional Science, 41(3), 621–634.CrossRef Matlen, B. J., & Klahr, D. (2013). Sequential effects of high and low instructional guidance on children’s acquisition of experimentation skills: Is it all in the timing? Instructional Science, 41(3), 621–634.CrossRef
go back to reference Miller, G. (1956). The magic number sever, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. The Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97.CrossRef Miller, G. (1956). The magic number sever, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. The Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97.CrossRef
go back to reference Moreno, R. (2006). When worked examples don’t work: Is cognitive load theory at an Impasse? Learning and Instruction, 16(2), 170–181.CrossRef Moreno, R. (2006). When worked examples don’t work: Is cognitive load theory at an Impasse? Learning and Instruction, 16(2), 170–181.CrossRef
go back to reference National Research Council. (1996). National science education standards. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. National Research Council. (1996). National science education standards. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
go back to reference National Research Council. (2000). Inquiry and the national science standards. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. National Research Council. (2000). Inquiry and the national science standards. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
go back to reference National Research Council. (2012). A framework for K-12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. Committee on a Conceptual Framework for New K-12 Science Education Standards. Board on Science Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: The National Academics Press. National Research Council. (2012). A framework for K-12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. Committee on a Conceptual Framework for New K-12 Science Education Standards. Board on Science Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: The National Academics Press.
go back to reference Pathak, S., Jacobson, M., Kim, B., Zhang, B., & Deng, F. (2008). Learning the physics of electricity with agent-based models: The paradox of productive failure. In T. -W. Chan, G. Biswas, F. -C. Chen, S. Chen, C. Chou, M. Jacobson, Kinshuk, F. Klett, C. -K. Looi, T. Mitrovic, R. Mizoguchi, K. Nakabayashi, P. Reimann, D. Suthers, S. Yang & J. -C. Yang (Eds.), International conference on computers in education (pp. 221–228). Taipei, Taiwan. Pathak, S., Jacobson, M., Kim, B., Zhang, B., & Deng, F. (2008). Learning the physics of electricity with agent-based models: The paradox of productive failure. In T. -W. Chan, G. Biswas, F. -C. Chen, S. Chen, C. Chou, M. Jacobson, Kinshuk, F. Klett, C. -K. Looi, T. Mitrovic, R. Mizoguchi, K. Nakabayashi, P. Reimann, D. Suthers, S. Yang & J. -C. Yang (Eds.), International conference on computers in education (pp. 221–228). Taipei, Taiwan.
go back to reference Renkl, A., Stark, R., Gruber, H., & Mandl, H. (1998). Learning from worked-out examples: The effects of example variability and elicited self-explanations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23(1), 90–108.CrossRef Renkl, A., Stark, R., Gruber, H., & Mandl, H. (1998). Learning from worked-out examples: The effects of example variability and elicited self-explanations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23(1), 90–108.CrossRef
go back to reference Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. (2009). Compared with what? The effects of different comparisons on conceptual knowledge and procedural flexibility for equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 529–544.CrossRef Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. (2009). Compared with what? The effects of different comparisons on conceptual knowledge and procedural flexibility for equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 529–544.CrossRef
go back to reference Ross, A. J. (1988). Controlling variables: A meta-analysis of training studies. Review of Educational Research, 57, 405–437.CrossRef Ross, A. J. (1988). Controlling variables: A meta-analysis of training studies. Review of Educational Research, 57, 405–437.CrossRef
go back to reference Sao Pedro, M. A., Gobert, J. D., & Raziuddin, J. (2010). Comparing pedagogical approaches for the acquisition and long-term robustness of the control of variables strategy. In K. Gomez, L. Lyons, & J. Radinsky (Eds.), Learning in the disciplines: Proceedings of the 9th international conference of the learning sciences, ICLS 2010 (Vol. 1, pp. 1024–1031)., Full Papers Chicago, IL: International Society of the Learning Sciences. Sao Pedro, M. A., Gobert, J. D., & Raziuddin, J. (2010). Comparing pedagogical approaches for the acquisition and long-term robustness of the control of variables strategy. In K. Gomez, L. Lyons, & J. Radinsky (Eds.), Learning in the disciplines: Proceedings of the 9th international conference of the learning sciences, ICLS 2010 (Vol. 1, pp. 1024–1031)., Full Papers Chicago, IL: International Society of the Learning Sciences.
go back to reference Schauble, L. (1996). The development of scientific reasoning in knowledge-rich contexts. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 102–119.CrossRef Schauble, L. (1996). The development of scientific reasoning in knowledge-rich contexts. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 102–119.CrossRef
go back to reference Stark, R., Kopp, V., & Fischer, M. R. (2011). Case-based learning with worked examples in complex domains: Two experimental studies in undergraduate medical education. Learning and Instruction, 21(1), 22–33. Stark, R., Kopp, V., & Fischer, M. R. (2011). Case-based learning with worked examples in complex domains: Two experimental studies in undergraduate medical education. Learning and Instruction, 21(1), 22–33.
go back to reference Strand-Cary, M., & Klahr, D. (2008). Developing elementary science skills: Instructional effectiveness and path independence. Cognitive Development, 23(4), 488–511.CrossRef Strand-Cary, M., & Klahr, D. (2008). Developing elementary science skills: Instructional effectiveness and path independence. Cognitive Development, 23(4), 488–511.CrossRef
go back to reference Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognition Science, 12, 257–285.CrossRef Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognition Science, 12, 257–285.CrossRef
go back to reference Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1991). Evidence for cognitive load theory. Cognitive and Instruction, 8, 351–362.CrossRef Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1991). Evidence for cognitive load theory. Cognitive and Instruction, 8, 351–362.CrossRef
go back to reference Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction, 12(3), 185–233.CrossRef Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction, 12(3), 185–233.CrossRef
go back to reference Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J., & Paas, F. G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3), 251–296.CrossRef Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J., & Paas, F. G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3), 251–296.CrossRef
go back to reference Toth, E., Klahr, D., & Chen, Z. (2000). Bridging research and practice: A cognitively based classroom intervention based classroom intervention for teaching experimentation skills to elementary school children. Cognition and Instruction, 18(4), 423–459.CrossRef Toth, E., Klahr, D., & Chen, Z. (2000). Bridging research and practice: A cognitively based classroom intervention based classroom intervention for teaching experimentation skills to elementary school children. Cognition and Instruction, 18(4), 423–459.CrossRef
go back to reference van Gog, T., Kester, L., & Paas, F. (2011). Effects of worked examples, example-problem, and problem-example on novices’ learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 212–218.CrossRef van Gog, T., Kester, L., & Paas, F. (2011). Effects of worked examples, example-problem, and problem-example on novices’ learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 212–218.CrossRef
go back to reference van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding. Instructional Science, 32(1–2), 83–98. van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding. Instructional Science, 32(1–2), 83–98.
go back to reference van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. (2006). Effects of process-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer performance. Learning and Instruction, 16(2), 154–164.CrossRef van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. (2006). Effects of process-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer performance. Learning and Instruction, 16(2), 154–164.CrossRef
go back to reference Varma, K., & Linn, M. (2012). Using Interactive technology to support students’ understanding of the greenhouse effect and global warming. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 20(1), 453–464.CrossRef Varma, K., & Linn, M. (2012). Using Interactive technology to support students’ understanding of the greenhouse effect and global warming. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 20(1), 453–464.CrossRef
go back to reference Veermans, K., Joolingen, W. V., & de Jong, T. (2006). Use of heuristics to facilitate scientific discovery learning in a simulation learning environment in a physics domain. International Journal of Science Education, 28(4), 341–361.CrossRef Veermans, K., Joolingen, W. V., & de Jong, T. (2006). Use of heuristics to facilitate scientific discovery learning in a simulation learning environment in a physics domain. International Journal of Science Education, 28(4), 341–361.CrossRef
go back to reference Yin, Y., Tomita, M. K., & Shavelson, R. J. (2008). Diagnosing and dealing with student misconceptions: Floating and sinking. Science Scope, 31(8), 34–39. Yin, Y., Tomita, M. K., & Shavelson, R. J. (2008). Diagnosing and dealing with student misconceptions: Floating and sinking. Science Scope, 31(8), 34–39.
go back to reference Zimmerman, C. (2007). The development of scientific thinking skills in elementary and middle school. Developmental Review, 27(2), 172–223.CrossRef Zimmerman, C. (2007). The development of scientific thinking skills in elementary and middle school. Developmental Review, 27(2), 172–223.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Virtual Experiments or Worked Examples? How to Learn the Control of Variable Strategy
Author
Shiyu Liu
Publication date
01-04-2015
Publisher
Springer Netherlands
Published in
Technology, Knowledge and Learning / Issue 1/2015
Print ISSN: 2211-1662
Electronic ISSN: 2211-1670
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10758-014-9240-6

Other articles of this Issue 1/2015

Technology, Knowledge and Learning 1/2015 Go to the issue

Premium Partners