Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 2/2013

01.06.2013 | Original Paper

Inter-Institutional Dynamics: Assessments of the Supreme Court in a Separation of Powers Context

verfasst von: Kjersten R. Nelson, Eve M. Ringsmuth

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 2/2013

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Using data collected from a survey experiment, we examine whether information about the nature of the interactions between the Supreme Court and Congress influences respondents’ assessments of the Court. We find that political sophistication is key to understanding how individuals incorporate the separation of powers context into their evaluations of the Court. Political sophisticates give the Court its highest assessments when told that the Court and Congress are often in disagreement, and that Congress is most responsible for this disagreement. Assessments of the Court are significantly lower, however, when sophisticates believe that high levels of disagreement between the Court and Congress are due to the Court’s actions and when these respondents believe that the Court and Congress agree a high proportion of the time. These results suggest that for political sophisticates, the Court’s institutional standing is related to the balance it strikes between deference to Congress and judicial independence.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
We note that not all scholars share McCloskey’s view of the Court’s role. For alternative perspectives see, e.g., Carolene Products (1938), Footnote 4 (304 U.S. 144) and Friedman (2009). In the interest of clarity, we focus on McCloskey’s characterization as a first step in testing for an empirical connection between the separation of powers context and individuals’ assessments of the Court’s legitimacy.
 
2
See Mondak (1992) for a discussion of the link between policy legitimation and institutional support.
 
3
“Court legitimacy” has been variously referred to as several different terms throughout the literature. For the purposes of this article, we will interchangeably use the terms “court legitimacy,” “institutional support,” “diffuse support,” and “assessments of the Court.”
 
4
Empirically, scholars have drawn dramatically different conclusions about the nature of public response to Court opinions depending upon whether diffuse or specific support is being measured (e.g., Murphy, Tanenhaus, and Kastner 1973; Caldeira and Gibson 1992; Durr et al. 2000). Further, specific support has been shown to be one factor which may contribute to institutional support (Gibson et al. 1998).
 
5
In a somewhat related vein, Clark (2009) also posits that Court legitimacy is connected to the interplay between the Court and Congress. Specifically, he examines whether Court behavior in terms of the number of laws struck down each year is responsive to signals about its legitimacy via the interaction between Court-curbing attempts and public support for the Court.
 
6
See Hoekstra (2003) for the importance of case salience on the public impact of a case.
 
7
In this instance, the relationship between knowledge and institutional support holds in bivariate analysis but not when included in a multiple regression. However, this appears to be due to multicollinearity with independent variables measuring adherence to democratic norms and level of education (p. 59).
 
8
To be clear, Gibson (2007) notes that higher levels of Court-specific knowledge are positively related to measures of the Court’s institutional support (as do Gibson and Caldiera 2009a and Caldeira and Gibson 1995). Here, we rely on general political knowledge questions as the basis for a distinction between political sophisticates and non-sophisticates.
 
9
Others appear to share the sense that those with low knowledge of courts will respond differently to stimuli about the Courts. Gibson et al. (1998), for instance, focus their analyses of institutional support on individuals who have at least a moderate level of awareness of the Court, assuming that this group will be distinctive from those with low or no knowledge of the Court.
 
10
We use the situation where Congress is at fault for high levels of disagreement as our comparison group because we do not have theoretical reasons to believe that respondents would be more disappointed with the Court when it fails to uphold its delicate balance in one way more than the other. In other words, we do not necessarily expect legitimacy to be harmed more in the situation where the Court is at fault for high levels of disagreement than in the situation where the Court and Congress agree most of the time.
 
11
While any differences between the demographics of our sample and the general population limit the generalizability of specific levels of institutional support for the Court, the power of random assignment gives us confidence in the comparisons between experimental conditions, and thus any causal relationships detected (see footnote 14).
 
12
While data was collected near the end of the Court’s 2008 term, we explored this and we found no empirical evidence to suggest that any individual decision released during this time period influenced respondents' assessments of diffuse support for the Court.
 
13
Scholars have extensively studied whether those who are sophisticated about politics—and, therefore, have more information about politics and issues—will be more or less susceptible to framing effects. Theoretical arguments lay out compelling reasons for either directional assumption. For instance, the politically unsophisticated may be more susceptible to persuasion by frames because they have fewer established opinions about political issues—these individuals, then, should be more likely to accept a frame’s perspective at face value and not counter-argue the frame’s assertion (e.g., Kinder and Sanders 1990; Haider-Markel and Joslyn 2001). Alternatively, others argue that sophisticates have the capacity to meaningfully incorporate a new frame into their existing knowledge and beliefs about political matters (e.g., Nelson et al. 1997; Miller and Krosnick 2000; Druckman and Nelson 2003). The bulk of the empirical evidence is coalescing around the latter reasoning: that political sophisticates will respond to frames more so than political non-sophisticates.
 
14
Because the subjects were randomly assigned, we can be confident that any differences between conditions can be attributed to the experimental treatments (or lack thereof) (Druckman et al. 2011). As such, it is unnecessary to administer pre- and post-treatment questionnaires to the subjects. We also address the slim possibility of a break-down in the random assignment procedure by further analyzing the data with multiple regression controls. For further explanation of experimental methods and the trade-offs between these two types of designs, see Druckman et al. (2011).
 
15
We chose 80 percent as our reported level of agreement for two reasons. First, we wanted the agreement levels to be high enough to account for the possibility, in respondents’ minds, that Congress is not always acting to contravene personal liberties. Given the institutional propensity of Congress, however, with an agreement rate this high, we surmised that respondents would begin to suspect that the Court was not opposing congressional action that harmed personal liberties. At the same time, to preserve credibility amongst the sample subset who might, off the top of their heads, come up with several recent instances of disagreement between the Court and Congress, we did not want to make the agreement rate much higher.
 
16
We are uncertain about how the Control group will compare to the other conditions because any expectations would theoretically depend on respondents' beliefs about the actual level of policy agreement between the Court and Congress. If, for example, a respondent thinks the Court and Congress do actually agree a large percentage of the time, her responses may mimic those in the Agreement condition. However, we could not easily ask a question about this without the risk that doing so would prime the separation of powers context, thus compromising the idea that the Control group is not subjected to an experimental treatment. Instead, we included the Control condition to explore the extent to which respondents may possess a shared baseline perception of these inter-branch dynamics that would be revealed by a comparison with the treatment groups. Our primary focus, though, is to test for differences between the alternative characterizations of the separation of powers context.
 
17
Respondents are well-distributed across conditions. There are 91, 91, 81, and 90 respondents, respectively, in the Agreement, Congress Fault, Court Fault, and Control groups.
 
18
In constructing the framing manipulation, a key decision was whether to present the (dis)agreement to respondents as relating to one specific case, or to an aggregation of cases. Ultimately, we chose to focus on an aggregation of cases. We contend that doing so more accurately reflects how individuals assess the Supreme Court in a real world setting. Infrequently, a case is salient enough to draw the attention of the broad public (e.g., Bush v. Gore or Roe v. Wade). Past work, however, has shown that individual cases may temporarily shift institutional support for the Court, but that it relatively quickly returns to pre-decision equilibrium levels (e.g., Durr et al. 2000; Yates and Whitford 2002). This suggests that there is a strong, established basis for opinions about the Supreme Court which are only temporarily influenced by major cases. It is this foundation of support that we hope to tap and test in a separation of powers context.
In addition, we argue that by aggregating cases, we minimize the potential distraction of ideology amongst our respondents. Our goal is to test McCloskey’s framework by examining whether institutional support for the Court is influenced by a consideration of how the Court's actions relate to actions taken by Congress. It would have been possible to present respondents with an aggregation of cases in the frame which also persistently pointed in one ideological direction—varying, for instance, not just the levels of (dis)agreement, but also the ideological direction of that disagreement. This would be an interesting venue for further investigation but is beyond the scope of this initial experiment. McCloskey’s framework focuses on the separation between fundamental law and popular will, which does not directly correspond to an ideological dimension. What is more, over the course of most respondents’ lifetimes, they will have seen both “liberal” and “conservative” courts, balancing out the differences when it comes to aggregate assessments of institutional legitimacy. Furthermore, we control for respondents’ ideological predispositions in the models to guard against any potential influence along these lines.
Finally, actual Supreme Court news stories suggest this aggregate focus is externally valid. For instance, in a New York Times report during the confirmation of Chief Justice John Roberts, Greenhouse (2005) notes that, “In a series of decisions…the Court declared unconstitutional acts of Congress that had been passed by broad bipartisan majorities.” Later, Greenhouse continues outlining how the Court's decisions relate to actions taken by Congress in the aggregate, writing, “From 1995 to 2003, the Supreme Court overturned all or parts of 33 federal statutes…Until then, the modern court gave Congress a wide berth to define its own role.” These excerpts suggest the treatment articles used here, which also discuss Court’s actions as they relate to Congress in the aggregate, are appropriately similar to real news stories.
 
19
In the debate of whether political sophisticates will be more or less susceptible to framing effects, Druckman and Nelson (2003) find NE to be a key control variable. This construct measures a subject’s propensity to thoroughly examine a new argument or, perhaps, a new frame. Respondents who record high levels of NE tend to have many prior opinions and are chronic evaluators (Jarvis and Petty 1996). Once the authors control for NE, sophisticates are more susceptible to the media frames, while high-NE individuals are typically less susceptible to the frames (Druckman and Nelson 2003, p. 740). We include the measure here to control for NE’s potential to be conflated with sophistication.
 
20
We generally follow Gibson et al.'s (2003) scale with only minor adjustments. Our Cronbach’s α indicates it is appropriate to scale these items (α = 0.80). See Appendix for the questions included.
 
21
Political sophistication is relatively well-distributed across the range of the variable. Specifically, it ranges from 0 to 10 with a mean of 6.96 and a standard deviation of 2.67. Additionally, there are respondents at each of the 11 levels of the scale.
 
22
We include those who scored at the median with sophisticates since we believe answering 7 out of 10 political knowledge questions correctly (the median value), including some fill in the blank questions, signals substantial awareness and understanding of politics. Additionally, we explored other possible operationalizations of sophistication including a three-way split (i.e., low, mid, and high sophistication based on one standard deviation above and below the median) and a continuous measure of sophistication and found substantively similar or slightly weaker results, which is consistent with Druckman (2004) and Miller and Krosnick (2000). See Druckman (2004) for a discussion of the advantages of a median split.
 
23
Subsequent to pre-testing for this survey, Gibson and Caldeira (2009b) noted the positive bivariate relationship between courts-based knowledge and institutional support. While our measure relies on broader political knowledge questions, we include three Court-specific knowledge questions. The correlation between the number of correct answers on these three questions and the more general political knowledge scale is high: 0.85. What is more, when we run our final model, basing the sophistication split on the Court-only questions, the results largely correspond to the final model used here.
 
24
The questionnaire included three need to evaluate questions (see Appendix). However the Cronbach’s α for the three-item scale is 0.64 but increases to 0.74 without the third item. Thus, we proceed using the two-item measure, which asks whether the respondent tends to have an opinion about a few things or many things and whether the respondent has fewer or more opinions than others. It ranges from 0 to 1, has a mean of 0.61 and a standard deviation of 0.21. For more on NE, see footnote 19.
 
25
Predicted values are calculated with all independent variables at their median or modal values unless otherwise noted.
 
26
Specifically, we also predicted values by including those who scored at the median on political knowledge with non-sophisticates and by contrasting those who scored below the median with those who scored above. When we included those at the median in the group of non-sophisticates, there were 168 (as opposed to 129) non-sophisticate respondents. (See footnote 22 for an explanation as to why we include those who score at the median in the sophisticate group.) Differences among all groups remained insignificant at the p < 0.1 (two-tailed) level.
 
27
This lack of statistical significance could again be related to the relatively small number of non-sophisticate respondents.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Baird, V., & Gangl, A. (2006). Shattering the myth of legality: The impact of the media’s framing of Supreme Court procedures on perceptions of fairness. Political Psychology, 27, 597–613.CrossRef Baird, V., & Gangl, A. (2006). Shattering the myth of legality: The impact of the media’s framing of Supreme Court procedures on perceptions of fairness. Political Psychology, 27, 597–613.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brambor, T., Clark, W. R., & Golder, M. (2006). Understanding interaction models: Improving empirical analyses. Political Analysis, 14, 63–82.CrossRef Brambor, T., Clark, W. R., & Golder, M. (2006). Understanding interaction models: Improving empirical analyses. Political Analysis, 14, 63–82.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Caldeira, G. (1986). Neither the purse nor the sword: Dynamics of public confidence in the Supreme Court. American Political Science Review, 80, 1209–1226.CrossRef Caldeira, G. (1986). Neither the purse nor the sword: Dynamics of public confidence in the Supreme Court. American Political Science Review, 80, 1209–1226.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Caldeira, G., & Gibson, J. (1992). The etiology of public support for the Supreme Court. American Journal of Political Science, 36, 635–664.CrossRef Caldeira, G., & Gibson, J. (1992). The etiology of public support for the Supreme Court. American Journal of Political Science, 36, 635–664.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Caldeira, G., & Gibson, J. (1995). The legitimacy of the Court of Justice in the European Union: Models of institutional support. American Political Science Review, 89, 356–376.CrossRef Caldeira, G., & Gibson, J. (1995). The legitimacy of the Court of Justice in the European Union: Models of institutional support. American Political Science Review, 89, 356–376.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Choper, J. (1980). Judicial review and the national political process. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Choper, J. (1980). Judicial review and the national political process. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Clark, T. (2009). The separation of powers, court-curbing and judicial legitimacy. American Journal of Political Science, 53(4), 971–989.CrossRef Clark, T. (2009). The separation of powers, court-curbing and judicial legitimacy. American Journal of Political Science, 53(4), 971–989.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Delli Carpini, M., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. Cambridge: Yale University Press. Delli Carpini, M., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. Cambridge: Yale University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J. (2001). The implications of framing effects for citizen competence. Political Behavior, 23, 225–256.CrossRef Druckman, J. (2001). The implications of framing effects for citizen competence. Political Behavior, 23, 225–256.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (ir)relevance of framing effects. American Political Science Review, 98, 671–686.CrossRef Druckman, J. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (ir)relevance of framing effects. American Political Science Review, 98, 671–686.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J., Green, D., Kuklinski, J., & Lupia, A. (2011). Experiments: An introduction to core concepts. In J. Druckman, D. Green, J. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Druckman, J., Green, D., Kuklinski, J., & Lupia, A. (2011). Experiments: An introduction to core concepts. In J. Druckman, D. Green, J. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Druckman, J., & Nelson, K. (2003). Framing deliberation: How citizens’ conversations limit elite influence. American Journal of Political Science, 47, 729–745.CrossRef Druckman, J., & Nelson, K. (2003). Framing deliberation: How citizens’ conversations limit elite influence. American Journal of Political Science, 47, 729–745.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Durr, R., Martin, A., & Wolbrecht, C. (2000). Ideological divergence and public support for the Supreme Court. American Journal of Political Science, 44(4), 768–776.CrossRef Durr, R., Martin, A., & Wolbrecht, C. (2000). Ideological divergence and public support for the Supreme Court. American Journal of Political Science, 44(4), 768–776.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Easton, D. (1965). A systems analysis of political life. New York: Wiley. Easton, D. (1965). A systems analysis of political life. New York: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Friedman, B. (2009). The will of the people. New York: Farrar, Staus and Giroux. Friedman, B. (2009). The will of the people. New York: Farrar, Staus and Giroux.
Zurück zum Zitat Gelman, A., & Stern, H. (2006). The difference between “significant” and “not significant” is not itself significant. The American Statistician, 60(4), 328–331.CrossRef Gelman, A., & Stern, H. (2006). The difference between “significant” and “not significant” is not itself significant. The American Statistician, 60(4), 328–331.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J. (2007). The legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court in a polarized polity. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 4(3), 507–538.CrossRef Gibson, J. (2007). The legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court in a polarized polity. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 4(3), 507–538.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J., & Caldeira, G. (2009a). Citizens, courts, and confirmations: Positivity theory and the judgments of the American people. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Gibson, J., & Caldeira, G. (2009a). Citizens, courts, and confirmations: Positivity theory and the judgments of the American people. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J., & Caldeira, G. (2009b). Knowing the Supreme Court? A reconsideration of public ignorance of the high court. Journal of Politics, 71, 429–441.CrossRef Gibson, J., & Caldeira, G. (2009b). Knowing the Supreme Court? A reconsideration of public ignorance of the high court. Journal of Politics, 71, 429–441.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J., Caldeira, G., & Baird, V. (1998). On the legitimacy of national high courts. American Political Science Review, 92, 343–358.CrossRef Gibson, J., Caldeira, G., & Baird, V. (1998). On the legitimacy of national high courts. American Political Science Review, 92, 343–358.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J., Caldeira, G., & Spence, L. (2003). Measuring attitudes toward the United States Supreme Court. American Journal of Political Science, 47, 354–367.CrossRef Gibson, J., Caldeira, G., & Spence, L. (2003). Measuring attitudes toward the United States Supreme Court. American Journal of Political Science, 47, 354–367.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Haider-Markel, D., & Joslyn, M. (2001). Gun policy, opinion, tragedy, and blame attribution. Journal of Politics, 63(2), 520–543. Haider-Markel, D., & Joslyn, M. (2001). Gun policy, opinion, tragedy, and blame attribution. Journal of Politics, 63(2), 520–543.
Zurück zum Zitat Hibbing, J., & Theiss-Morse, E. (1995). Congress as public enemy. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Hibbing, J., & Theiss-Morse, E. (1995). Congress as public enemy. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hoekstra, V. (2003). Public reaction to Supreme Court decisions. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Hoekstra, V. (2003). Public reaction to Supreme Court decisions. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jarvis, W., & Petty, R. (1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(1), 172–194.CrossRef Jarvis, W., & Petty, R. (1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(1), 172–194.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kinder, D., & Sanders, L. (1990). Mimicking political debate with survey questions. Social Cognition, 8(1), 73–103.CrossRef Kinder, D., & Sanders, L. (1990). Mimicking political debate with survey questions. Social Cognition, 8(1), 73–103.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McCloskey, R. (1994). The American Supreme Court. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. McCloskey, R. (1994). The American Supreme Court. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller, J., & Krosnick, J. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of presidential evaluations. American Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 295–309.CrossRef Miller, J., & Krosnick, J. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of presidential evaluations. American Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 295–309.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mondak, J. (1991). Substantive and procedural aspects of Supreme Court decisions as determinants of institutional approval. American Politics Quarterly, 19, 174–188.CrossRef Mondak, J. (1991). Substantive and procedural aspects of Supreme Court decisions as determinants of institutional approval. American Politics Quarterly, 19, 174–188.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mondak, J. (1992). Institutional legitimacy, policy legitimacy, and the Supreme Court. American Politics Quarterly, 20, 457–477.CrossRef Mondak, J. (1992). Institutional legitimacy, policy legitimacy, and the Supreme Court. American Politics Quarterly, 20, 457–477.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mondak, J. (1994). Policy legitimacy and the Supreme Court: The sources and contexts of legitimation. Political Research Quarterly, 47, 675–692. Mondak, J. (1994). Policy legitimacy and the Supreme Court: The sources and contexts of legitimation. Political Research Quarterly, 47, 675–692.
Zurück zum Zitat Murphy, W., Tanenhaus, J., & Kastner, D. (1973). Public evaluations of constitutional courts: Alternative explanations. Beverly Hills: Sage. Murphy, W., Tanenhaus, J., & Kastner, D. (1973). Public evaluations of constitutional courts: Alternative explanations. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson, T., Oxley, Z., & Clawson, R. (1997). Toward a psychology of framing effects. Political Behavior, 19(3), 221–246.CrossRef Nelson, T., Oxley, Z., & Clawson, R. (1997). Toward a psychology of framing effects. Political Behavior, 19(3), 221–246.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sears, D. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(3), 515–530.CrossRef Sears, D. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(3), 515–530.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Yates, J., & Whitford, A. (2002). The presidency and the Supreme Court after Bush v. Gore: Implications for institutional legitimacy and effectiveness. Stanford Law & Policy Review, 13(1), 101–118. Yates, J., & Whitford, A. (2002). The presidency and the Supreme Court after Bush v. Gore: Implications for institutional legitimacy and effectiveness. Stanford Law & Policy Review, 13(1), 101–118.
Metadaten
Titel
Inter-Institutional Dynamics: Assessments of the Supreme Court in a Separation of Powers Context
verfasst von
Kjersten R. Nelson
Eve M. Ringsmuth
Publikationsdatum
01.06.2013
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 2/2013
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-011-9190-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2013

Political Behavior 2/2013 Zur Ausgabe