Skip to main content

Ecological Aspects of Neophobia and Neophilia in Birds

  • Chapter
Current Ornithology

Part of the book series: Current Ornithology ((CUOR,volume 16))

Abstract

Nearly fifty years ago D. F. Berlyne (1950) wrote

Psychology has so far had surprisingly little to say about stimuli which influence behavior simply because they are new. Stimuli which owe their potency to the fact that they are not new ... have given rise to the vast corpus of observations and generalization that go to make up learning theory. But the everyday activity of both men and animals seem [sic] to attest to the importance of novelty as well as familiarity in features of the environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, J. A., 1988, Frequency-dependent selection by predatorsPhil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. Ser. B. 319:485–503.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Andrew, R. J., 1972, Changes in search behaviour in male and female chicks following different doses of testosteroneAnim. Behan 20:741–750.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Andrew, R. J., and Rogers, L. J., 1972, Testosterone, search behaviour and persistenceNature 237:343–346.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Atwood, J. L., 1980, Social interactions in the Santa Cruz Island Scrub JayCondor 82:440–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker-Gabb, D. J., 1986, Ecological release and behavioural and ecological flexibility in Marsh Harriers on islandsEmu 86:71–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barraud, E. M., 1961, The development of behaviour in some young passerinesBird Study 8:111–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrows, E. M., 1995Animal Behaviour Desk ReferenceCRC Press, Boca Raton, FL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrows, E. M., Acquavella, A. P., Weinstein, P. J. S., and Nosal, R. E., 1980, Response to novel food in captive, juvenile MockingbirdsWilson Bull. 92:399–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, P. G., 1971, Imprinting, in:Ontogeny of Vertebrate Behavior(H. Moltz, ed.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 369–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beesher, J., Jensen, H. C., and Bevins, R. A., 1999, Dopamine antagonism in a novel-object preference and a novel-object place conditioning preparation with ratsBehay. Brain Res. 103:35–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beissinger, S. R., Donnay, T. J., and Walton, R., 1994, Experimental analysis of diet specialization in the Snail Kite: The role of behavioral conservatismOecologia 100:54–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, J L, Li, C., Patterson, B. D., Greenberg, D. L., Murphy, D., and Hammer, H., 1996, Population and familial association between the D4 dopamine receptor gene and measures of novelty seekingNature Genet. 12:81–84.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E., 1950, Novelty and curiosity as determinants of wild and laboratory ratsBr. J. Psych. 41:68–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berthold, P., 1976, Animalische and vegetabilische Ernährung omnivorer Singvogelarten: Nahrungsbevorzugung, Jahresperiodik der Nahrungswahl, physiologische and ökologische BedeutungJ. Ornith. 117:145–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bevins, R. A., and Bardo, M. T., 1999, Conditioned increase in place preference by access to novel objects: Antagonism by MK-801Behay. Brain Res. 99:53–60.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Birke, L. I. A., and Archer, J., 1983, Some issues and problems in the study of animal exploration, in:Exploration in Animals and Humans(J. Archer and L. Birke, eds.), Workingham Press, Berkshire, England, pp. 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blazquez, M. C., Rodriguez-Estrella, R., and Delibes, M., 1997, Escape behaviour and predation risk of mainland and island Spiny-tailed Iguanas(Ctenosaura hemilopha) Ethology 103:990–998.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blondel, J., Chessel, D., and Frochot, B., 1988, Bird species impoverishment, niche expansion, and density inflation in Mediterranean island habitatsEcology 69:1899–1917.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boarman, W. I., and Barry, K., 1995, Common Ravens in the southwestern United States, 1968–92, in:Our Living Resources(E. T. LaRoe, ed.), National Biological Service. Washington, D.C., pp. 73–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boisy, A., 1995, Fear and fearfulness in animalsQuart. Rev. Biol. 70:165–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Budaev, S. V., 1997, Personality in the Guppy(Poecilia reticulata):A correlational study of exploratory behavior and social tendencyJ. Comp. Psych. 111:399–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cadieu, J. C., Cadieu, N., and Medioni, J., 1995, Parental influence on food familiarization in the fledgling Canary(Serinus canarius)and its long-term effectEthol. Ecol. Evol. 7:265–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, G. S., and Rubinoff, R., 1983, Avoidance of venomous sea snakes by naïve herons and egretsAuk 100:195–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capretta, P. J., 1969, The establishment of food preferences in chicksGallus gallus Anim. Behay. 17:229–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chai, P., 1996, Butterfly visual characteristics and ontogeny of responses to butterflies by a specialized tropical birdBiol. J. Linn. Soc. 59:37–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chamove, A. S., 1983, Role of dominance on macaque response to novel objectsMotivation and Emotion 7:213–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chance, M. R. A., and Meade, A. P., 1955, Competition between feeding and exploration in the ratBehaviour 8:174–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, A. S., and Boinski, S., 1995, Temperament in nonhuman primatesAm. J. Primat. 37:103–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, A. S., and Linburg, D. G., 1993, Behavioral contrasts between male cynomlgus and Lion-tailed MacaquesAm. J. Primat. 29:49–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, A. S., Mason, W. A., and Moberg, G. P., 1988, Differential behavioral and adreno-cortical responses to stress among three macaque speciesAm. J. Primat. 14:37–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, K., and Wilson, D. S., 1998, Shyness and boldness in Pumpkinseed Sunfish:Individual differences are context-specificAnim. Behay. 56:927–936.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, S. L., and Mellgren, R. L., 1994, Neophobia when feeding alone and in flocks in Zebra FinchesTaeniopygia guttata Anim. Behay. 48:903–907.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coppinger, R. P., 1969, The effect of experience and novelty on avian feeding behaviour with reference to the evolution of warning colouration in butterflies. Part I. Reactions of wild caught Blue Jays to novel insects.Behaviour 35:45–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coppinger, R. P., 1970, The effect of experience and novelty on avian feeding with reference to the warning coloration of butterflies. II. Reactions of naïve birds to novel insectsAm. Nat. 194:323–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corey, D. T., 1977, The determinants of exploration and neophobiaNeurosci. Biobehay. Rev. 2:235–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, P. E., 1976, New object reaction of Battus rattus:the relative importance of different cuesBehay. Biol. 16:31–44.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, P. E., and Barnett, S. A., 1975, The new object and new place reactions ofBattus rattus(L.)Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 56:219–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowell, K. L., 1983, Islands—insight or artifact?: Population dynamics and habitat utilization in insular rodentsOikos 41:442–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daugherty, C. H., Gibbs, G. W., and Hitchmough, R. A., 1993, Mega-island or micro-continent? New Zealand and its faunaTrends Ecol. Evol. 8:437–442.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Deckert, G., and Deckert, K., 1982, Spielverhalten and Komfortbewegungen beim GrünflügelaraAra chloroptera Bonn. Zool. Beitr. 33:269–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • DesForges, M. F., and Wood-Gush, D. G. M., 1975, A behavioural comparison of domestic and Mallard ducksAnim. Behay. 23: 692–697.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J. M., 1970, Ecological consequences of island colonization by southwest pacific birds, I. Types of niche shiftsProc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 67:529–536.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Doherty, S., and Cowie, R. J., 1994, The effects of early feeding experience on long-term seed choice by Canaries(Serinus canaria) Ethology 97:177–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drent, P. J., 1997, Realised heritability of a behavioural syndrome in the Great TitProgress Report of the Netherlands Institute of Ecology44–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drent, P. J., and Marchetti, C., 1999, Individuality, exploration and foraging in hand raised juvenile Great tits.Proc. 22nd Intern. Ornith. Congr.(N. Adams, and R. Slowtow, eds.), University Natal, Durban, pp. 896–914.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duvall II, F., 1980, Social and object interactions of juvenile Rooks(Corvus frugilegus)and Hooded Crows(C. corone cornix) Verh. Dtsch. Zool. Ges.332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebstein, R. P., Novick, O., Umansky, R., Priel, B., Osher, Y., Blaine, D., Bennett, E. R., Nemanov, L., Katz, M., and Belmaker, R. H., 1996, Dopamine receptor (D4DR) exon III polymorphism associated with the human personality trait of novelty seekingNature Genet. 12:78–80.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Elner, R. W., and Hughes, R. N., 1978, Energy maximisation in the diet of the Shore CrabCarcinus maenas J. Anim. Ecol. 47:103–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franchina,J. J., Johnson, L. J., and Leynes, P. A., 1994, Roles of visual and taste cues in ingestional neophobia: Response latency effects in chicks(Gallus domesticus) Anim. Learn. Behay. 22:341–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galef, Jr., B. J., 1993, Functions of social learning about food: a causal analysis of effects of diet novelty on preference transmissionAnim. Behay. 46:257–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galef, Jr., B. J., and Clark, M. M., 1971, Social factors in the poison avoidance and feeding behavior of wild and domesticated rat pupsJ. Comp. Phys. Psych. 75:341–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glickman, S. E., and Sroges, R. W., 1966, Curiosity in zoo animalsBehaviour24:151–188. Glück, E., 1984, Habitat selection and the role of early experienceZ. Tierpsychol. 66:45–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glück, E., and Leisler, B.,1994, Ontogenetic aspects of habitat selectionJ. Ornith. 135 :480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Götmark, F., 1994, Does a novel bright color patch increase or decrease predation: red wings reduce predation risk in European BlackbirdsNature 256:83–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, J. P., 1974, Risk, stochastic preferences, and the value of informationJ. Econ.Theory 8:64–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, P. R., 1998, Patterns on islands and microevolution, in:Evolution on Islands(P. R. Grant, ed.), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1979, Body size, breeding habitat, and winter exploitation systems inDendroica Auk 96:756–766.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1983, The role of neophobia in foraging specialization of some migrant warblersAm. Natur.123:444–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1984a, The winter exploitation systems of Bay-breasted and Chestnut-sided Warblers in PanamaUniv. Calif. Pub]. Zool. 117:1–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1984b, Differences in feeding neophobia between two species of tropical migrant warblers(Dendroica castaneaandD. pensylvanica) J. Comp. Psych. 98: 131–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1984c, Neophobia in the foraging site selection of a neotropical migrant bird: an experimental study.Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 81:3778–3780.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1987a, Social facilitation does not reduce neophobia in Chestnut-sided Warblers (Parulinae:Dendroica) J. Ethol. 5:7–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1987b, The development of dead leaf foraging in a Neotropical migrant warblerEcology 68:130–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1989, Neophobia, aversion to open space, and ecological plasticity in Song and Swamp SparrowsCan. J. Zool. 67:1194–1199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1990a, Feeding neophobia and ecological plasticity: a test of the hypothesis with captive sparrowsAnim. Behay. 39:375–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1990b, Ecological plasticity, neophobia, and resource use in birdsStud. Avian Biol. 13:431–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1992, Differences in neophobia between naïve Song and Swamp SparrowsEthology 91:17–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., 1995, Novelty responses: the bridge between psychology, behavioral ecology, and community ecologyTrends Ecol. Evol. 10:165–166.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., and Droege, S., 1999, On the decline of the Rusty Blackbird and the use of ornithological literature to document long-term population trendsConserv. Biol. 13:553–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greig-Smith, P. W., 1987a, Persistence in foraging: when do Bullfinches abandon unprofitable seeds?Behaviour 103:203–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greig-Smith, P. W., 1987b, Aversion of starlings and sparrows to unfamiliar, unexpected or unusual flavours and colours in foodEthology 4:155–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grünberger, S., and Leisler, B., 1990, Angeborene and erfahrungsbedingte Komponenten der Habitatwahl der Tannenmeise(Parus ater) J. Ornith. 131:460–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grünberger, S., and Leisler, B., 1993, Auswirkung der Umwelterfahrung auf die Neophobie der Tannenmeise(Parus ater). J. Ornith. 134:352–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gwinner, E., 1996, Circannual clocks in avian reproduction and migrationIbis138:47–63. Gwinner, E., Biebach, H., and Kries, I. V., 1985, Food availability affects migratory restless-ness in caged Garden Warbler(Sylvia borin) Naturwissenschaften 72:51–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haemig, P. D., 1989, A comparative experimental study of exploratory behavior in Santa Cruz Island and mainland Scrub JaysAphelocoma coerulescens Bird Behay. 8:38–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegner, R. E., 1985, Dominance and anti-predator behaviour in Blue Tits(Parus cae-ruleus) Anim. Behay. 33:762–768.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heinrich, B., 1988, Why do ravens fear their food?Condor 90:950–952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heinrich, B., 1995, Neophilia and exploration in juvenile Common RavensCorvus corax Anim. Behay. 50:675–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heinrich, B., Marzluff, J. M., and Adams, W., 1995, Fear and food recognition in naïve Common RavensAuk 112:499–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, J. A., 1965, An experimental study of conflict and fear: An analysis of behavior of young chicks towards a mealworm. I. The behavior of chicks which do not eat the wormBehaviour 25:45–97.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, R. N., 1997, Intrinsic exploration in animals• motives and measurementBehay. Proc. 41:213–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Immelmann, K., and Beer, C., 1989A Dictionary of EthologyHarvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inglis, I. R., and Shepherd, D. S., 1994, Rats work for food they then reject: support for the information-primacy approach to learned industriousnessEthology 98:154–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jämmrich, S., 1989, Quantitative Analyse des Verhaltens gegenüber unbekannten Objekten. Untersuchungen an Insectivore, Rodentia and PrimatesZool. Beitr. N.F. 32: 195–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, T. D., 1981, Contrasting approaches to a theory of learningBrain Behay. Sci. 4:125–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R. B., 1982, Effects of early environmental enrichment upon open-field behaviour and timidity in the domestic chickDevel. Psychobiol 15:105–111.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R. B., 1986, Responses of domestic chicks to novel food as a function of sex, strain and previous experienceBehay. Proc. 12:261–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R. B., and Andrew, R. J., 1992, Responses of adult domestic cocks and capons to novel and alarming stimuliBehay. Proc. 26:189–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R. B., Larkins, C., and Hughes, B. 0., 1996, Approach/avoidance responses of domestic chicks to familiar and unfamiliar video images of biologically neutral stimuliAppl. Anim. Behay. Sci. 48:81–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., and Snidman, N, 1988, Biological basis of childhood shynessScience 240:167–171.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J., Snidman, N., and Arcus, B., 1998, Childhood derivatives of low levels of reactivity in infancyChild Development 69:1483–1493.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kamil, A. C., and Yoerg, S., 1982, Learning and foraging behavior, in:Perspectives in Ethology Volume5 (P. Bateson and P. Klopfer, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 325–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, G., 1982, The relationship between social structure and the response to novel spaceBehaviour 81:231–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, G., 1983, Relationship between social structure and response to noveltyBehaviour 87:183–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kilham, L., 1974, Play in Hairy, Downy, and other woodpeckersWilson Bull. 86:35–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klopfer, P. H., 1963, Behavioral aspects of habitat selection: the role of early experienceWilson Bull. 75:15–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klopfer, P. H., 1967, Behavioral stereotypy in birdsWilson Bull. 79:290–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kothbauer-Hellmann, R., and Winkler, H., 1991, Aufenthaltsdauern explorierender Meisen: Habitat-und Artspezifität.J. Ornith. 132:329–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubat, S., 1992, Die Rolle von Neuigkeit, Andersartigkeit und sozialer Struktur für die Exploration von Objekten beim Kea (Nestor notabilis), Ph.D. dissertation, Universität Wien, Vienna, Austria.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z., 1967The Dynamics of Behaviour Development: An Epigenetic ViewRandom House, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahti, K., Koivula, K., and Orell, M., 1997, Dominance, daily activity, and winter survival in Willow Tits: Detrimental cost of long working hours?Behaviour 134:921–939.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefebvre, L., Whittle, P., Lascaris, E., and Finkelstein, A., 1997, Feeding innovations and forebrain in birdsAnim. Behay.53:549–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levins, R., and Lewontin, R., 1985The Dialectical BiologistHarvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lima, S. L., and Dill, L. M., 1990, Behavioural decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectusCan. J. Zool. 68:619–640.

    Google Scholar 

  • Löfgren, O., 1995, Niche expansion and increased maturation rate ofClethrionomys glareolusin the absence of competitorsJ. Mamm. 76:1100–1112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K., 1956, Plays and vacuum activities, in:L’Instinct dans le comportement de animaux et de L’hommeFondation Singer Polignac, Masson et Cie Editerus, Paris, pp. 633–638.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowndes, M., and Davies, M. C., 1995, The effect of archistrial lesions on open field and fear avoidance behavior in the domestic chickBehay. Brain Res. 72:25–32.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lowndes, M., Davies, M. C., and Johnson, M. H., 1997, Archistrial lesions impair the acquisition of filial preferencesEur. J. Neurosci. 6:1143–1148.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacArthur, R. H., Diamond, J. M., and Karr, J. R., 1972, Density compensation in island faunasEcology 53:330–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mappes, J., and Alatalo, R. V., 1997, Effects of novelty and gregariousness in survival of aposematic preyBehay. Ecol. 8:174–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchetti, K., and Price, T., 1989, Differences in the foraging of juvenile and adult birds: the importance of developmental constraintsBiol. Rev. Camb. Phil. Soc. 64:51–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marler, P., 1956, Behavior of the ChaffinchBehaviour(suppl.) 5:1–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marples, N. M., and Brakefield, P. M., 1995, Genetic variation for the rate of recruitment of novel insect prey into the diet of a birdBiol. J. Linn. Soc. 55:17–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marples, N. M., Roper, T., and Harper, D., 1998, Responses of wild birds to novel prey: evidence for dietary conservatismOikos 83:161–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marzluff, J. M., and Heinrich, B., 1991, Foraging by Common Ravens in the presence and absence of territory holders: an experimental analysis of social foragingAnim. Behay. 42:755–770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mastota, F. N., and Mench, J. A., 1994, Avoidance of dyed foods by Northern BobwhiteAppl. Anim. Behay. Sci. 12:109–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayeaux, D. J., and Mason, W. A., 1998, Development of responsiveness to novel objects in the Titi MonkeyCallicebus moloch Primates39:419–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, J. F., and Roughgarden, J., 1989, Avian predation onAnolislizards in thenortheastern Caribbean: An inter-island contrastEcology 70:617–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, J., and Houston, A., 1985, A simple model of information use in the exploita-tion of patchily distributed foodAnim. Behay. 33:553–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menzel, E. W., Davenport Jr., R. K., and Rogers, C. M., 1961, Some aspects of behavior toward novelty in young chimpanzeesJ. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 54:16–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettke, C., 1993, Auftreten und Ausprägung von Explorationsverhalten bei Papageien im Ökologischen Kontext, Ph.D. dissertation, Freie Universität, Berlin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettke, C., 1995, Explorationsverhalten von Papageien—Adaptation an die Umwelt?J. Ornith. 136:468–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettke-Hofmann, C., 1999, Niche expansion and exploratory behavior on islands: are they linked?Proc. 22nd Intern. Ornith. Congr.(N.Adams, and R. Slowtow, eds.), University Natal, Durban.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettke-Hofmann, C., 2000a, Reactions of nomadic and resident parrot speciesPsittacidaeto environmental enrichment at the Max Planck InstitutInt. Zoo Yearbook 37: 244–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettke-Hofmann, C., 2000b, Changes in exploration from courtship to the breeding state in Red-rumped Parrots(Psephotus haematonotus) Behay. Proc. 49:139–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. H., 1942, Habitat selection among higher vertebrates and its relation to intra-specific variationAm. Natur. 76:25–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milton, K., 1981, Distribution pattern of tropical plant foods as an evolutionary stimulus to mental developmentAmerican Anthropologist 83:534–548.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, D., 1976, Experiments on neophobia in wild and laboratory rats: a reevaluationJ. Comp. Phys. Psych. 90:190–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, K. C., 1955, The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory behaviorComp. Phys. Psych. 48:254–260.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morse, D. H., 1971, The foraging of warblers isolated on small islandsEcology 52:216–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse, D. H., 1977, The occupation of small islands by passerine birdsCondor 79: 399–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse, D. H., 1980Behavioral Mechanisms in EcologyHarvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, E. S., 1980, Adaptations to seasonal changes by migrant land birds in the Panama Canal Zone, in:Migrant Birds in the Neotropics: Ecology Behavior Distribution and Conservation(A. Keast and E. S. Morton, eds.), Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, D.C., pp. 437–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, E. S., 1998, Pairing in Mallards and American Black Ducks: a new view on population decline in American Black DucksAnim. Cons. 1:239–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, M. L., Wakamati, M. L., Pereya, M. E., and Morton, G. A., 1991, Post-fledging dispersal, habitat imprinting, and philopatry in a migratory sparrowOmis Scand. 22:98–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, L. B., 1977, Responses of domestic fowl to novel food and objectsAppl. Anim. Ethol. 3:335–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Negro, J. J., Bustamante, J., Milward, J., and Bird, D. M., 1996, Captive fledgling American Kestrels prefer to play with objects resembling natural preyAnim. Behay. 52: 707–714.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nietsch, A., and Todt, D.,1985, Monopolisierung tragbarer Objekte durch juvenile Individuen bei halbfrei lebenden Berberaffen(Macaca sylvanus) Verh. Dtsch. Zool. Ges. 78:213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortega, J. C., and Bekoff, M., 1987, Avian play: evolutionary and developmental trendsAuk 104:338–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellis, S. M., 1981, Exploration and play in the behavioural development of the Australian MagpieGymnorhina tibicen Bird Behay.3:37–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, R. E., and Youngren, O. M.,1986, Unilateral kainic acid lesions reveal dominance of right archistriatum in avian fear behaviorBrain Res. 377:216–220.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Poucet, B., Durup, M., and Thinus-Blanc, C., 1988, Short-term and long-term habituation of exploration in rats, hamsters, and gerbilsBehay. Proc. 16:203–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raach, A., and Leisler, B., 1989, Auswirkung der Jugenderfahrung auf die Wahl von Habitatstrukturen und auf das Erkundungsverhalten des Mariskensängers (Acrocephalus melanopogon) J. Ornith. 130:256–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabinowitch, V., 1968, The role of experience in the development of food preferences in gull ChicksAnim. Behay. 16:425–428.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rabinowitch, V., 1969, The role of experience in the development and retention of seed preferences in Zebra FinchesBehaviour 33:222–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rana, B. D., 1989, Some observations on neophobic behaviour among House SparrowsPasser domesticus Pavo 27:35–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, D. R., and Rasmussen, K. L., 1979, Social ecology of adult males in a confined troop of Japanese Macaques(Macaca fuscata) Anim. Behay. 27:434–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raudensush, B., and Frank, R. A., 1999, Assessing food neophobia: The role of stimulus familiarityAppetite 32:261–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, J. M., Boulinier, T., Danchin, E., and Oring, L. W., 1999, Informed dispersal: prospecting by birds for breeding sites, in:Current Ornithologyvol.15(V. Nolan Jr., E. D.Ketterson, and C. F. Thompson, eds.), Kluwer Academic/Plenum, New York, pp. 189–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renner, M. J., 1987, Experience-dependent changes in exploratory behaviour in the adult rat (Rattus norvegicus): overall activity level and interactions with objectsJ. Comp. Psychol. 101 :94-100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renner, M. J., 1988, Learning during exploration: The role of behavioural topography during exploration in determining subsequent adaptive behaviourInt. J. Comp. Psych. 2:43–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renner, M. J., and Seltzer, C. P., 1991, Molar characteristics of exploratory and investiga-tory behaviour in therat (Rattus norvegicus) J. Comp. Psychol. 105:326–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roper, T. J., 1990, Responses of domestic chicks to artificially coloured insect prey: effects of previous experience and background colorAnim. Behay. 39:466–473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, M. L., 1985, Some theoretical aspects of habitat selection, in:Habitat Selec-tion in Birds(M. L. Cody, ed.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 517–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russel, E. M., and Pearce, G. A., 1971, Exploration of novel objects by marsupialsBehav-iour 40:312–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, P. A., 1973, Relationship between exploratory behaviour and fear: a reviewBr. J. Psychology 64:417–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saegert, S., and Rajecki, D. W., 1973, Effects of prior exposure to animate object on approach tendency in chicksBehay. Biol. 8:749–754.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schaden, G., 1993, Exploration and neophobia in captive Barn Owls(Tyto alba guttata) Egretta 36:67–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schleidt, W. M.,1961, Reaktionen von T uthühnern auf fliegende Raubvögel and Versuche zur Analyse ihrer AAM’sZ. Tierpsychol. 18:534–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuler, W., 1982, Zur Funktion von Warnfarben: Die Reaktion junger Stare auf wespenähnlich schwarz-gelbeAttrappe Z. Tierpsychol. 58:66–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuler, W., and Hesse, E., 1985, On the function of warning coloration: A black and yellow pattern inhibits prey-attack by naive domestic chicksBehay. Ecol. Sociobiol. 16:249–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuler, C., and Roper, T. J., 1992, Responses to warning coloration in avian predatorsAdvances in the Study of Behaviour 21:111–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seferta, A., 1998The Role of Intervening Variables in Learning Differences between Group foraging and Territorial ColumbidsMS thesis, McGill University, Montreal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J., 1972, The role of novelty in learned avoidance of unpalatable “prey” by domestic chicksAnim. Behay. 20:29–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J., 1993, Where is the comparison in comparative cognition? Alternative research programsPsych. Sci. 4:179–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S., 1973, Factors directing prey-attack behavior by the young of three passerine speciesLiving Bird 12:57–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S., 1975, Innate recognition of coral snake pattern by a possible avian predatorScience 187:759–760.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stamps, J. A., 1987, The effect of familiarity with a neighborhood on territory acquisitionBehay. Ecol. Sociobiol. 21:273–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, D. W., and Krebs, J. R., 1986Foraging TheoryPrinceton University Press, Princeton, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stolba, A., and Wood-Gush, D. G. M., 1980, Arousal and exploration in growing pigs in different environmentsAppl. Anim. Ethol. 6:382–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, P. F., Fifield, W., Kennedy, M., Mulder, R. T., Sellman, J. D., and Joyce, P. R., 1998, No association between novelty seeking and type 4 dopamine receptor gene (DRD4) in two New Zealand samplesAm. J. Psychiatry 155:98–101.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tardiff, S., 1994, SensitiveSaguinusand courageousCallithrix:differences in adaptability and neophobia in two callitrichid generaProc. Am. Zoo Ass. Reg. Conf. 1994:408–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, W. H., 1945, The evolutionary significance of habitat selectionJ. Anim. Ecol. 14: 67–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, W. H., 1956Learning and Instincts in Animals.Methuen and Co., London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todt, D., Hammerschmidt, K., and Hultsch, H., 1992, The behaviour of Barbary Macaques(Macaca sylvanus):perspective and projects of a long-term studyPrimate Report 32:19–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torigoe, T., 1985, Comparison of object manipulation among 74 species of nonhuman primatesPrimates 26:182–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turro-Vincent, I., Launey, F., Mills, A. D., Picard, M., and Faure, J. M., 1995, Experimental and genetic influence on learnt food aversions in Japanese Quail selected for high or low levels of fearfulnessBehay. Proc. 34:23–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaughn, F. A., 1983, Startle responses of Blue Jays to visual stimuli presented during feedingAnim. Behay. 31:385–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verbeek, M. E. M., Drent, P. J., and Wiepkema, P. R., 1994, Consistent individual differences in early exploratory behaviour of male Great TitsAnim. Behay. 48:1113–1121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verbeek, M. E. M., Boon, A., and Drent, P. J., 1996, Exploration, aggressive behaviour, and dominance in pair-wise confrontations of juvenile male Great TitsBehaviour 133: 945–963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verbeek, M. E. M., De Geode, P., Drent, P. J., and Wiepkema, P. R., 1999, Individual behavioural characteristics and dominance in aviary groups of Great Tits.Behaviour 136:23–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vince, M. A., 1960, Developmental changes in responsiveness in the Great TitParus major Behaviour 15:219–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Visalberghi, E., and Fragaszy, D., 1995, The behaviour of Capuchin MonkeysCebus apellawith novel foods: the role of social contextAnim. Behay. 49:1081–1097.

    Google Scholar 

  • Visalberghi, E., Valente, M., and Fragaszy, D., 1998, Social context and consumption of unfamiliar foods by Capuchin Monkeys(Cebus apella)over repeated encountersAm. J. Primat. 45:367–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster, S. J., and Lefebvre, L., 2000, Neophobia by the Lesser-Antillean Bullfinch, a foraging generalist, and the Bananaquit, a nectar specialistWilsonBu11.112:424–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welker, W. I., 1956, Some determinants of play and exploration in ChimpanzeesJ. Comp. Physiol. Psycho). 49:84–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, T. K., and Sherry, T. W., 1987, Behavioral feeding specialization inPinaroloxias inornatathe “Darwin’s Finch” of Cocos Island, Costa RicaProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 84:5506–5520.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, M., 1977, Exploration and play with objects in domestic kittensDev. Psychobiol. 10: 53–57.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., 1998, Adaptive individual differences within a single populationPhil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 353:199–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., Clark, A. B., Colman, K., and Dearstyne, T., 1994, Shyness and boldness in humans and other animalsTrends Ecol. Evol. 9:442–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood-Gush, D. G. M., Vestergaard, K., and Petersen, D. V., 1990, The significance of motivation and environment in the development of exploration in pigsBiol. Behay. 15:39–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wunschmann, A,1963, Quantitative Untersuchungen zum Neugierverhalten von WirbeltierenZ. Tierpsychol. 20:80–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyles, S. J., Kunkel, J. G., and Wilson, A. C., 1983, Birds, behavior, and anatomical evolutionProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 80:4394–4397.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yeaton, R., and Cody, M., 1973, Competitive release in island Song Sparrow populationsTheor. Pop. Biol. 4:42–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zach, R., and Falls, J. B., 1976, Foraging behaviour, learning, and exploration by captive Ovenbirds(Aves: Parulidae) Can. J. Zool. 54:1880–1893.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2001 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Greenberg, R., Mettke-hofmann, C. (2001). Ecological Aspects of Neophobia and Neophilia in Birds. In: Nolan, V., Thompson, C.F. (eds) Current Ornithology. Current Ornithology, vol 16. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-1211-0_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-1211-0_3

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-5443-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-1211-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics