Skip to main content
Log in

Relationships between time of day, day of the week, and positive mood: Exploring the role of the mood measure

  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the relationship between time of day, day of the week, and two measures of positive affect (PA). According to previous research and the circumplex model of affect, one scale was designed to assess the activation component of PA, and the other one measured the pleasantness aspect. Subjects rated their mood three times a day for 7 consecutive days. Consistent with our hypotheses, PA-Pleasantness showed a peak on the weekend, whereas PA-Activation remained stable throughout the week. Regarding time of day, maximum PA-Activation was reached in the afternoon. In contrast, the Pleasantness component of PA increased from morning to evening. Implications of these results as well as other findings concerning the differential content of “PA” measures are discussed regarding the fact that a certain scale is most appropriate and maximally valid for representing certain aspects of affective experience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baker, J. G., Zevon, M. A., & Rounds, J. B. (1994). Differences in positive and negative affect dimensions: Latent trait analysis.Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 161–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolger, N., DeLongis, A., Kessler, R. C., & Schilling, E. A. (1989). Effects of daily stress on negative mood.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 808–818.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1973).Personality and mood by questionnaire. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1988). Mood and the mundane: Relations between daily life events and self-reported mood.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 296–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Leeka, J. (1989). Diurnal variation in the positive affects.Motivation and Emotion, 13, 205–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1105–1117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 464–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fröberg, J. (1977). Twenty-four hour patterns in human performance, subjective and physiological variables and differences between morning and evening active subjects.Biological Psychology, 5, 119–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M. D., & Strube, M. J. (1994). Independence revisited: The relation between positive and negative affect in a naturalistic setting.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 57–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedges, S. M., Jandorf, L., & Stone, A. A. (1985). The meaning of daily mood assessments.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 428–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller, W. (1990). The neuropsychology of emotion: Developmental patterns and implications for psychopathology. In N. L. Stein, B. Leventhal, & T. Trabasso (Eds.),Psychological and biological approaches to emotion (pp. 167–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howell, A., & Conway, M. (1992). Mood and the suppression of positive and negative self-referent thoughts.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 535–555.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy-Moore, E., Greenberg, M. A., Newman, M. G., & Stone, A. A. (1992). The relationship between daily events and mood: The mood measure may matter.Motivation and Emotion, 16, 143–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krohne, H. W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C.-W., & Tausch, A. (in press). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Form der Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) [Investigations with a German version of the PANAS].Diagnostica.

  • Larsen, R. J. (1989, August).Personality as an affect dispositional system. In L. A. Clark & D. Watson (Chair),Emotional bases of personality. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, New Orleans.

  • Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion. In M. S. Clark (Ed.),Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 25–59). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J., & Kasimatis, M. (1991). Day-to-day physical symptoms: Individual differences in the occurrence, duration, and emotional concomitants of minor daily illnesses.Journal of Personality, 59, 387–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R. W., Richards, M. H., & Perry-Jenkins, M. (1994). Divergent worlds: The daily emotional experience of mothers and fathers in the domestic and public spheres.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1034–1046.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, G. J., & Shack, J. R. (19889). The structural convergence of mood and personality: Evidence for old and new directions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 691–706.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michela, J. L. (1990). Within-person correlational design and analysis. In C. Hendrick & M. S. Clark (Eds.),Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 279–311). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowlis, V. (1965). Research with the Mood Adjective Check List. In S. S. Tomkins & C. E. Izard (Eds.),Affect, cognition, and personality (pp. 352–389). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Brien, R. G., & Kaiser, M. K. (1985). MANOVA method for analyzing repeated measures designs: An extensive primer.Psychological Bulletin, 97, 316–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W. (1982).The psychology of physical symptoms. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossi, A. S., & Rossi, P. E. (1977). Body time and social time.Social Science Review, 6, 273–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O'Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–1086.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix.Psychological Bulletin, 87, 245–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, A. A. (1981). The association between perceptions of daily experiences and self- and spouse-rated mood.Journal of Research in Personality, 15, 510–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, A. A. (1987). Event content in a daily survey is differentially associated with concurrent mood.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 56–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, A. A., Hedges, S. M., Neale, J. M., & Satin, M. S. (1985). Prospective and cross-sectional mood reports offer no evidence of a “Blue Monday” phenomenon.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 129–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, A. A., & Neale, J. M. (1984). The effects of severe daily events on mood.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 137–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thayer, R. E. (1987). Problem perception, optimism, and related states as a function of time of day (diurnal rhythm) and moderate exercise: Two arousal systems in interaction.Motivation and Emotion, 11, 19–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thayer, R. E., Takahashi, P. J., & Pauli, J. A. (1988). Multidimensional arousal states, diurnal rhythms, cognitive and social processes, and extraversion.Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 15–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasey, M. W., & Thayer, J. F. (1987). The continuing problem of false positives in repeated measures ANOVA in psychophysiology: A multivariate solution.Psychophysiology, 24, 479–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 6, 473–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D. (1988a). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of positive and negative affect: Their relation to health complaints, perceived stress, and daily activities.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1020–1030.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D. (1988b). The vicissitudes of mood measurement: Effects of varying descriptors, time frames, and response formats on measures of positive and negative affect.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 128–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood.Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers and the associate editor, Robert A. Baron, for their constructive comments on an earlier version of this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Egloff, B., Tausch, A., Kohlmann, CW. et al. Relationships between time of day, day of the week, and positive mood: Exploring the role of the mood measure. Motiv Emot 19, 99–110 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02250565

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02250565

Keywords

Navigation