Skip to main content
Log in

Dynamic visual support for story comprehension and mental model building by young, at-risk children

  • Research
  • Published:
Educational Technology Research and Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Multimedia technology allows precise coordination of linguistic and visual information and may provide teachers with tools for enhancing literacy foundations in children–especially those who might otherwise be at-risk for school failure. This research explores the hypothesis that a multimedia environment with dynamic visual support facilitates language comprehension when children listen to short stories. Kindergarten children heard stories in three conditions: Helpful video, in which dynamic, silent video accompanied the beginning of stories; No video, in which children only heard the stories; and Minimal video, in which static images of characters and places accompanied the beginning of stories. In all conditions, the ending of the story was presented without visual support. Overall, the pattern of results suggests that dynamic visual support can provide a framework for understanding and remembering linguistic information.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, A.B., & Stokes, S.J. (1984). Social and institutional influences on the development and practice of literacy. In H. Goelman, A. Oberg, & F. Smith (Eds.),Awakening to literacy (pp. 24–38). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beagles-Roos, J., & Gat, I. (1983). Specific impact of radio and television on children's story comprehension.Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(1), 128–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J.D. & Johnson, M.K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A.G., & van Ijzendoorn, M.H. (1988). Mother-child interactions, attachment, and emergent literacy: A cross-sectional study.Child Development, 59, 1262–1272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R.B., & Wagner, R.K. (1992).The psychology of reading: An introduction (2nd ed.), New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis (1980). Development of components of reading skill.Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(5), 656–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Digdon, N., Pressley, M., & Levin, J.R. (1985). Preschoolers' learning when pictures do not tell the whole story.Educational Communication and Technology Journal, 33(2), 139–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feitelson, D., Kita, B., & Goldstein, Z. (1986). Effects of listening to series stories on first graders' comprehension and use of language.Research in the Teaching of English, 20(4), 339–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M.A. (1990).Language comprehension as structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, J., Anderson, D.R., Smith, R., Field, D.E., & Fischer, C. (1986). Young children's recall and reconstruction of audio and audiovisual narratives.Child Development, 57, 1014–1023.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J.J. (1966).The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A.M., & Langston, W.E. (1992). Comprehension of illustrated text: Pictures help to build mental models.Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 129–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A.M., Meyer, M., & Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension.Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 69–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, J., Levin, J.R., & Pressley, M. (1977). Pictures, partial pictures, and young children's oral prose learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 69(5), 473–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, D.S., Kelly, S.B., & Mandel, M. (1986). Media differences in children's story synopses: Radio and television contrasted.Journal of Educational Psychology, 78(5), 341–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Health (1983).Ways with words (1st ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1983).Mental models. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1992). Longitudinal research on learning to read and write with at-risk students. In M. J. Dreher & W. H. Slater (Eds.),Elementary school literacy: Critical issues (pp. 73–99). Norwood, MA: Christopher-Gordon Publishers, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamil, M. (1991).A proposal for the National Reading Research Center. College of Education, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirsch, I., & Jungblut, A. (1986).Literacy: Profiles of America's young adults. (ETS No. 16-Pl-02). National Assessment of Educational Progress.

  • McNamara, T.P., Miller, D.L., & Bransford, J.D. (1991). Mental models and narrative comprehension. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.),Handbook of reading research (Vol. II). (pp. 490–511). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meringoff, L.K. (1980). Influence of the medium on children's story apprehension.Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(2), 240–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, D.G., Bower, G.H., & Greenspan, S. (1989). Updating situation models during narrative comprehension.Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 292–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, L.M. (1985). Retelling stories: A strategy for improving young children's comprehension, concept of story structure, and oral language complexity.The Elementary School Journal, 85, 647–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, L.M. (1986). Effects of structural guidance in story retelling of children's dictation of original stories.Journal of Reading Behavior, 18(2), 135–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, L.M. (1988). Young children's responses to one-to-one readings in school settings.Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 89–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, L.M. (1990). Small group story readings: The effects on children's comprehension and responses to literature.Reading Research and Instruction, 29(4), 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A. (1980). Picture-book reading in motherinfant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel.Child Development, 51, 587–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pezdek, K., Lehrer, A., & Simon, S. (1984). The relationship between reading and cognitive processing of television and radio.Child Development, 55, 2072–2082.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., Cariglia-Bull, T., Deane, S., & Schneider, W. (1987). Short-term memory, verbal competence, and age as predictors of imagery instructional effectiveness.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 194–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purcell-Gates, V. (1989). Written language knowledge held by low-SES, inner city children entering kindergarten. In S. McCormick & J. Zutrell (Eds.),Cognitive and social perspectives for literacy research and instruction: Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp. 95–105). Chicago, IL: National Reading Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, D.L.M., & McNamara, T.P. (1990).Spatial mental models in narrative comprehension: Now you see them, now you don't. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, LA.

  • Smiley, S.S., Oakley, D.D., Worthen, D., Campione, J.C., & Brown, A.L. (1977). Recall of thematically relevant material by adolescent good and poor readers as a function of written versus oral presentation.Journal of Educational Psychology, 69(4), 381–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C.E. (1983). Lieracy and language: Relationships during the preschool years.Harvard Educational Review, 53(2), 165–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C.E., & Goldfield, B.A. (1983). Turn the page, please: Situation-specific language acquisition.Journal of Child Language, 10, 535–549.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C.E., & Ninio, A. (1986). The contracts of literacy: What children learn from learning to read books. In W.H. Teale & E. Sulzby, (Eds.),Emergent literacy: Writing and reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teale, W.H. (1986). Home background and young children's literacy development. In W.H. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.),Emergent literacy: Writing and reading (pp. 173–206). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. (1985). Preschool literacy-related activities and success in school. In D.R. Olson, N. Torrence, & N. Hildyard (Eds.),Literacy, language, and learning (pp. 229–255). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N.A., & Oakhill, J. (1991).Children's problems in text comprehension: An experimental investigation. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was funded by the National Institute of Child Health and Development, grant P01 HD15051-12. The views expressed in this article do not necessarily reflect the views of that organization.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sharp, D.L.M., Bransford, J.D., Goldman, S.R. et al. Dynamic visual support for story comprehension and mental model building by young, at-risk children. ETR&D 43, 25–42 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02300489

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02300489

Keywords

Navigation