Introduction

Severe and age-inappropriate levels of hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention present in more than one area of life, now referred to as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), is one of the most frequently observed psychiatric disorders [1]. Although the theoretical conception and diagnostic criteria of ADHD have undergone several modifications, the core symptoms have long been known.

In this selective review, we illustrate a part of the history of ADHD in the context of research on sustained attention, preparation, and response control using the continuous performance test (CPT). The following section gives a brief overview of the history of the ADHD concept, followed by a short clinical characterization. The second section of the manuscript introduces the CPT, which was used early on as a test of sustained attention and vigilance for the diagnosis of patients with brain damage. We will illustrate that important aspects of ADHD core symptoms such as impulsivity and inattention can be captured with the CPT on levels of task performance data and brain activity measures. This renders the CPT a valuable paradigm for testing current theories of ADHD, identifying genetic and environmental risk factors, and further elucidating the pathophysiology of the disorder.

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

ADHD: History of the Concept in a Nutshell

Various descriptions of children with behavioral problems, seemingly characterized by hyperkinetic symptoms, date from the mid-nineteenth century [2], and CPT demands such as sustained attention and response control were considered very early on. Heinrich Hoffmann’s popular children’s book Struwwelpeter (Shockheaded Peter), first published in 1845, gives an early differentiation of two subtypes, predominant hyperactivity (the “Fussy-Philip”) and attention problems (the “Hans Stare-in-the-Air”). As such, ADHD symptoms made their way into literature and paintings as early as the middle of the nineteenth century. The first clear systematic scientific study of hyperactivity was carried out in 1902 by the pediatrician George Frederick Still (1868–1941), who attributed the observed behavioral problems to a "defect of moral control" as the result of an organic injury [3].

Also early in the twentieth century, ADHD-like symptoms were ascribed to brain dysfunctions. Observations of patients with frontal lobe lesions revealed that such injuries resulted in extensive deficits of "executive functions". "Cognitive conflicts" could no longer be resolved, and the patients had a tendency to give in to their primary action impulses, potentially leading to great difficulties in solving everyday life problems. These observations suggested that ADHD could be a milder form of dysfunction in frontal cortical structures [2]. The concepts of "minimal brain damage" (MBD) or "organic restlessness" were introduced, suggesting that milder forms of brain damage might lead to less severe behavioral problems such as ADHD. In 1908, the physician Alfred F. Tredgold postulated that modest hypoxia or other brain damage during birth was the possible cause of hyperkinetic behavioral problems that often became clear with the requirements of the first year of school. Tredgold also believed that the affected children had an inclination toward criminal behavior, even if they grew up in a beneficial environment.

After an epidemic of encephalitis in 1917 and 1918, some of the cured children suffered from behavioral and cognitive problems similar to ADHD core symptoms. These observations in the first half of the twentieth century led to the assumption that hyperactivity disorder may be caused by an organic brain damage, although the validity of this view was later criticized by Schachar, who analyzed case reports from an outbreak of encephalitis from 1918 to 1924 in England, and found clear ADHD symptoms only rarely amongst affected children [4].

In 1932, Kramer and Pollnow published a work on hyperkinetic disorder in children, Hyperkinetischen Erkrankung des Kindesalters, in which they described a syndrome characterized by restlessness, distractibility, and speech developmental disorders. In 1934, Lederer and Ederer described an independent hyperkinetic syndrome, Hypermotilitätsneurose im Kindesalter, as well. The use of terminology such as "postencephalitic behavior disorder" or "minimal brain damage" is evidence that an organic brain deficit was the assumed cause of all of these behavioral problems. The terms were suggested, but neither described nor assessed uniformly, and were never precisely defined [5]. Since the 1950s, the assumption that an organic brain injury such as "minimal brain damage" should be considered the central factor in hyperactivity disorders has been called into question as an impediment to rather than incentive for further research [6].

From the 1960s onward, views regarding ADHD differed between Europe and the United States. In Europe, the condition was considered as rare and related to a brain injury, accompanied by considerable restlessness, while the phenomenon in the United States was considered common, and not necessarily associated with brain injury. In the 1970s, the focus shifted towards the attention aspects of the disorder. Studies reported that children with hyperkinetic symptoms often had great difficulty sustaining attention in task-related activities. Because of the lack of continuous, macroscopically visible neuroanatomical damage, the term MBD was replaced by "minimal cerebral dysfunction (MCD)". However, the unconditional correlation between MCD and behavior was rejected in epidemiological studies, and MCD was henceforth regarded as an organic risk factor for ADHD symptoms [5]. In the 1970s, MCD was thought to be the cause of several cognitive symptoms, including learning disabilities, attention deficit, and developmental problems, as well as hyperactivity.

More recent theoretical approaches have integrated clinical symptoms and neuropsychological difficulties in a framework of more or less specific brain dysfunctions. A number of cognitive theories suggest that ADHD is associated with deficits in executive functions following on from dysfunction in frontostriatal or mesocortical brain networks responsible for attention and response organization [7, 8]. A classical approach proposed by Barkley suggested behavioral inhibition as the core deficit leading to problems with working memory, self-regulation, internalization of speech, and reconstitution, which are characteristic of ADHD [9]. Numerous studies have reported deficits in tasks tapping executive functions. Although there is much highly supportive evidence of differences between groups of patients with ADHD and controls, many patients do not show significant impairment in executive functions, while impairments have been observed in several controls that show no ADHD symptoms [10].

In contrast, earlier motivational theories like Sonuga-Barke’s delay aversion theory [11] and Sagvolden’s theory of impaired reward processing due to a shorter and steeper delay-of-reinforcement gradient [12] highlight that performance problems in ADHD may also origin from motivation problems. Such deficits were later associated with dysfunctions in the mesolimbic dopaminergic system, including the ventral tegmentum and nucleus accumbens, which is in good agreement with fMRI studies showing lower activity in the ventral striatum, including nucleus caudatus, nucleus accumbens, and putamen, in ADHD during reward anticipation [13••].

Cognitive and motivational theories are not necessarily contradictory, but rather may supplement each other, as suggested in the syntheses described in later “multiple pathway” theories associating cognitive and motivational deficits in ADHD with dysfunction in distinct neural networks [14, 15]. This has recently been put forward in an approach considering connectivity between brain regions, suggesting that in ADHD in particular, default mode network activity may interfere with activity in a number of neural networks responsible for visual attention, orientation, executive control, or motor activity [16, 17••].

Clinical Characteristics

In the DSM-III (1980), attention problems and hyperactivity were classified as “attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity (ADDH), which requires the core symptoms of inappropriate levels of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity beginning before the age of 7 years and present for at least six months in more than one area of life; and ADD, a subtype diagnosed without hyperactivity. The revised DSM-III-R (1987) introduced a list of 14 symptoms to clarify and improve diagnostic criteria. The DSM-IV released in 1994 distinguishes ADHD subtypes characterized predominantly by attention problems from individuals with hyperactivity and impulsivity, as well as a combined type fulfilling criteria for both inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity [18]. The recently released DSM-V, which considers ADHD as a neurodevelopmental disorder, eased the age of onset criterion towards 12 years and clarified diagnostic criteria for ADHD in adults [1]. In any case, the symptoms must be manifest from childhood for more than six months in at least two areas of life [19].

With a prevalence of approximately 5 % in children and adolescents, and high persistence leading to 3 % prevalence in adults, ADHD is one of the most frequently diagnosed disorders in psychiatry [20, 21]. It is associated with lower educational, social, and clinical outcomes in adulthood, rendering ADHD a lifelong problem, even in the case of early remission [22••]. ADHD is much more prevalent among family members of patients, which must be considered for diagnosis and treatment [23, 24]. Twin studies in childhood showed high heritability estimates of more than 70 % explained symptom variance due to genetic factors [25, 26], suggesting a strong genetic background for the development and persistence of the disorder.

The Continuous Performance Test

The history of the CPT originates in the middle of the twentieth century with the work of Rosvold et al. (1956). In search of a paradigm sensitive to problems with lapses of sustained attention in individuals with brain damage, the authors suggested a task requiring sustained attention for up to 10 minutes of assessment [27]. This was in stark contrast, for example, to digit span or digit symbol substitution tests frequently used to measure attention or alertness, which usually cover shorter periods of time and may not detect shorter lapses of attention, such as those expected in patients with brain damage.

Figure 1 provides a general overview of this approach. In brief, a sequence of letters is presented, and participants are instructed to press a button if the target letter (e.g., the letter “X”) is presented, but to withhold the response in all other instances. The simplest version requires a response to each target letter, and to withhold the response to all other stimuli (see Fig. 1, left side). This “CPT-X” is similar to a go/no-go task, but incorporates a lower target rate, and consequently does not help in maintaining activation or inducing a strong “prepotency” to respond. A more difficult extension of this paradigm as initially presented by Rosvold (1956) requires a response only if the target letter is preceded by a cue (e.g., the letter “A”) (see Fig. 1, right side). This cued “CPT-AX” can also be used to assess response preparation, which may be a smart move, particularly in research on ADHD.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Description of the Continuous Performance Test. The continuous performance test comprises a sequence of letters, usually presented in the central visual field. In the case of the CPT-X (left), a response is required to all targets (letter “X”), while the CPT-AX requires a response to cued targets only (right)

In the original study, a moderate time pressure was introduced by a presentation rate of one stimulus per second, and the procedure was able to differentiate groups of children and adults with brain damage from healthy controls [27]. A subsequent study, which in addition to patients with brain damage in the Rosvold et al. study included individuals with other psychiatric conditions, revealed that the CPT may also be sensitive for attention problems in psychotic patients [28].

The CPT in Research on ADHD

Since problems with sustained attention were expected in ADHD, the CPT paradigm was adopted early on for research and diagnosis. In the following sections, we describe some historical trends in ADHD research using the CPT.

Early studies dwell particularly on the neuropsychological characteristics of CPT performance, and address important moderating characteristics of the task in research on ADHD. Later, brain activity associated with task performance came into focus, and was assessed with increasing spatial and temporal resolution using event-related potential (ERP) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) paradigms, unravelling the time course and implicated structures during CPT performance. This parallels advances in the theoretical consideration of ADHD as a (heterogeneous) disorder associated with specific brain dysfunctions. The task was also used for the evaluation of treatment effects, beginning with pharmacological interventions, but later also psychotherapeutic interventions such as attention training and neurofeedback were assessed. Finally, the CPT was used in genetic studies, suggesting that it may uncover specific genetic effects on impulsivity, as well as preparation and response inhibition, which may be relevant for ADHD and other psychiatric disorders sharing similar underlying deficits.

Neuropsychological Characteristics

Performance in the CPT at the behavioral level is often characterized by reaction time to targets (indexing some general processing speed), errors of omission (e.g., no response to targets, often regarded as inattention), and errors of commission (false alarms to non-targets, often regarded as indicators of impulsive responses). Errors of omission in the cued CPT-AX were later further subdivided by Halperin et al. (1989) into (a) false alarms to cued non-targets (A-not-X, often faster than correct responses, possibly a better indicator of impulsivity), (b) false alarms to uncued targets (X-only, often with longer latency, potentially associated with inattention as a result of the preceding stimulus), (c) responses to the cue (in the case of longer latency, potentially premature responses indexing the inability to wait, thus possibly also related to impulsivity), and (d) random errors (present particularly in ADHD, but of unclear origin). In non-referred children, omission and X-only errors were associated with higher teacher-rated inattention, whereas A-not-X commission errors were associated with elevated hyperactivity and impulsivity ratings [29]. Thus the CPT, particularly in the AX variant, has shown striking face validity for assessing symptoms of inattention and impulsivity under controlled conditions.

Early studies, however, have raised doubts as to whether the CPT can be used for the diagnosis of ADHD. Although a high percentage of children with ADHD may exhibit performance problems, control children may also show impairment without the presence of ADHD symptoms, and conversely, some children with ADHD may not show impairment in the CPT [10, 30]. Because sensitivity and specificity for ADHD may both be below 70 %, CPT performance parameters are not recommended for individual diagnosis [31]. Moreover, despite replication of substantial group differences between patients with ADHD and controls, the key CPT performance parameters may not be specifically related to core symptoms of ADHD in patients [32].

An initial narrative review based on studies from the 1970s to 1990 was critical of the CPT. The authors showed that the task was far away from an ideal instrument for ADHD research; it lacked a sound theoretical basis and thorough investigation of mediating factors such as developmental effects (the authors suggested a strong improvement in sustained attention between the ages of 4 and 6 years), task variables (stimulus onset time, event rate, task duration), and situational factors (time of day during assessment, medication, or noise) for the interpretation of performance data [33]. For example, the target detection rate may be related to the event rate in an inverted U-shaped manner: a higher event rate (more than one event per second) may lead to over-activation and induce time pressure, leading to a lower target detection rate, with a performance maximum of one event in two seconds and lower detection rate if an event occurs only every four seconds, which is particularly pronounced in children with ADHD [34]. Moreover, the target rate (often ranging from 10 % to 25 % in a CPT) may influence the prepotency to respond, and deficits in vigilance or sustained attention may be particularly present with an extended time on task, all of which may influence group differences between ADHD and controls. In lieu of heterogeneous ADHD effects on key CPT performance parameters discussed thus far (which may, in addition to all of these uncontrolled moderators, reflect poor statistical power), Corkum and Siegel have suggested a cognitive energetic model, including psychological resource arousal, activation, and effort that may play a role in ADHD [33, 35]. Thus, patients with ADHD may be particularly likely to show deficit when their energetic or cognitive and motivational resources are stressed, which may be manipulated in the CPT implementation, e.g., by event rate, target frequency, or presence of incentives, a concept that should be considered in further studies.

Potential vigilance deficits in ADHD were revisited in a recent meta-analysis by Hunag-Pollock (2012). The authors aggregated studies published from 1995 to 2010 that had not been covered by earlier studies [33, 36]. Inclusion criteria were limited to studies assessing samples of children with ADHD and normally developing controls, predominantly aged 6 to 12 years, with a CPT of less than 50 % target probability (separating the CPT from go/no-go tasks specifically tapping response control), which controlled a number of moderators identified in the earlier review by Corkum et al. (1993), suggesting that the aggregated studies are particularly sensitive for ADHD difficulties. In total, more than 30 studies comprising more than 3,000 participants reported errors of commission and omission, 26 studies including more than 1,300 subjects assessed mean reaction time, and 16 studies reported intra-individual standard deviation of reaction time. All of these parameters indicated CPT performance problems in ADHD of large effect size, but also confirmed a number of potential moderators. It was determined that ADHD problems with mean response speed were more pronounced in CPTs with more frequent targets, effects on omission errors were larger in older children, and ADHD effects on RT-SD were more frequent in studies with a larger proportion of males [37•]. However, only a few studies assessed time-on-task effects, which may provide further insights into ADHD. In sum, the children with ADHD may show moderate to significant difficulty with sustained attention and impulsivity in CPTs with moderate event rates and rare targets.

Another early application of the CPT in ADHD was the evaluation of treatment using MPH. A meta-analysis on CPT error rates by Losier et al. (1996) applied rigorous selection criteria (limited to high-quality studies published between 1973 and 1995 on children aged 6 to 12 years, addressing case–control differences and effects of MPH in ADHD in thoroughly diagnosed samples) and aggregated effect size across studies. The authors confirmed higher omission and commission error rates in children with ADHD, which were considerably improved following administration of MPH [36]. Beneficial MPH effects on error rates as well as response speed were confirmed in a later narrative review, further suggesting that administration of dextroamphetamine may also yield improvements [38]. In addition, the CPT was used for the evaluation of improved pharmaceutical dosage forms of MPH, e.g., extended controlled-release capsules [39]. It was recently demonstrated that atomoxetine (ATX, a non-stimulant selective norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor used for the treatment of ADHD in children and adults) may lead to improved CPT performance, albeit of a smaller magnitude than with MPH [40, 41].

In addition to the evaluation of pharmacological treatments in ADHD, the CPT was recently used for identifying drug response. This was demonstrated for a response criterion of at least 25 % improvement in the ADHD Rating Scale-IV after six weeks of treatment, where the response rate was significantly diminished in children older than 10 years with extremely low reaction times (below the fifth percentile) in the CPT [42], although the validity of such effects needs to be replicated in further studies. While MPH effects may be visible to the naked eye within 30 minutes after administration, the prognosis of response to treatment could be particularly important for agents like ATX, which may reach full effectiveness in ADHD after several weeks of treatment [43], although studies are currently lacking.

The CPT has also been used for the evaluation of non-pharmacological interventions in ADHD. One key example is in the area of neurofeedback training, which has been shown to be effective on symptom ratings and neuropsychological task performance, including the CPT [44•, 45].

In sum, the CPT may uncover difficulties in ADHD under controlled assessments, and may thus be used for treatment evaluation.

The majority of the above-mentioned neuropsychological findings were obtained from laboratory experiments under well-controlled—albeit often not very naturalistic—conditions. Assessment of CPT performance outside the laboratory was undertaken by Bart et al. (2014), who administered the CPT online, with good reliability, although the performance was generally lower than that during laboratory testing [46]. It remains to be seen whether the advantages of assessing potentially large samples in situations where ADHD problems are eminent can outweigh the pitfalls of lower experimental control over the variables described above in moderating CPT performance.

Another promising approach was demonstrated with the implementation of the CPT in a virtual reality environment, transferring CPT demands into standardized everyday life situations. This technique was utilized by Pollak et al. (2009) in children with ADHD, in which a CPT-AX was presented within a virtual classroom with audio-visual distractors (classroom noise, a flying airplane, or a car rumbling outside the classroom window). The virtual environment CPT yielded similar case–control effect sizes as the classical CPT from the Test of Variables of Attention (T.O.V.A.) [47], and was also sensitive to MPH treatment effects [48]. This approach may afford the opportunity to conduct assessments with higher ecological validity without losing experimental rigor. Further experiments should assess the influence of naturalistic distractors on performance in healthy controls as well as patients with ADHD.

Psychophysiological Characteristics

Assessment of brain activity parameters has been used to identify impairment in CPT performance in ADHD on the basis of neural inhibition. In the last two decades, studies in ADHD have focused on the assessment of brain electrical activity evoked by the CPT (for an overview, see [49]) as well as assessment via functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).

Brain activity corresponding to changes in the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) contrast during CPT performance can be detected with fMRI assessment utilizing high spatial resolution [50]. These studies suggest that the CPT leads to activations in basal ganglia, prefrontal, and parietal attentional systems including middle frontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex [ACC], and precuneus, which are implicated in several “dopaminergic” psychiatric disorders [51, 52].

Children with ADHD showed performance and activation deficits in fronto-striato-parieto-cerebellar brain networks in the go/no-go contrast during an uncued rewarded CPT, which was normalised in temporal and parietal regions after administration of MPH [53]. Adults with ADHD were characterized by reduced fronto-striatal and left parietal activation during no-go trials of a relatively fast-paced CPT-AX; in addition, impulsivity was related to lower activity in the caudatum and ACC but increased activity in the left insular cortex [54].

In electrophysiological recordings during task performance, neural activity can be tracked as event-related potential (ERP) with high temporal resolution [49, 55], uncovering the activity of information processing during CPT performance (see Fig. 2). Early ERP components such as P1/N1, generated within the first 200 ms after stimulus presentation in the primary sensory cortices, are sensitive to physical characteristics of the stimuli but may be further modulated by early attention, while later components may reflect higher cognitive processing of particular interest for CPT performance in ADHD.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Event-Related Potentials Evoked by the CPT-AX. Electrophysiological characteristics of CPT-AX performance at midline electrodes from typically developing children. Stimulus processing is characterized by early P1/N1 components generated in primary and secondary visual areas, which are particularly elevated for the occipital N1 (with a positive amplitude at Fz after 200 ms) during cue processing (red). The N2 associated with cognitive control (with a fronto-central maximum after approx. 300 ms latency) is larger for cued No-Go (green) than Go (blue) trials, while the parietal P3 to task-relevant cues and cued targets is maximal over parietal sites. Cues elicit a slow wave contingent negative variation associated with preparation (consequently also present preceding cued No-Go and Go trials), which is not evoked by distractors (black). Further details are provided in [96]

Cues elicit after 300–1,000 ms a broad positive deflection, which is maximal over parietal scalp areas. This Cue-P3 may be generated in posterior attentional networks associated with attentional orienting and resource allocation, and it may be reduced in amplitude in children and adults with ADHD [5658]. The Cue-P3 is followed by a negative slow cortical potential maximal over the vertex that terminates with the presentation of the next stimulus (contingent negative variation, CNV) [59]. The CNV has been linked to timing and temporal memory [60, 61], and may be crucial for preparation in the CPT. Neurophysiological studies have implicated an ensemble of thalamocortical structures involved in CNV generation including the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, frontal cortex, thalamus, and midbrain dopaminergic nuclei involved in CNV generation [6264]. Numerous studies have demonstrated that CNV amplitude may be reduced in ADHD [56, 6568] and may represent a persistent deficit in patients with ADHD [69••], although there have also been some negative findings [58, 70, 71].

In trials following such cues, response control is required, and the difference in task demand between go and no-go trials is paralleled in the ERP. Following enhanced N2 amplitude in no-go trials indexing action monitoring processes (which may be less challenging in the CPT compared to stop signal or flanker tasks, and often do not reveal deficits in ADHD) [72], the subsequent P3 is also modulated: cued distractors that do not require execution of a prepared response evoke a no-go P3 maximal at fronto-central sites. Studies with more response-inhibition-demanding Go/No-Go tasks suggest that it may reflect response inhibition [7375], but since its latencies often exceed typical go reaction times, it may rather indicate termination of motor activation through a lapse of cortical excitation in motor areas when nothing more is left to do [74, 76, 77]. Clearly, these aspects are interrelated, and thus No-Go-P3 may be generated by several processes of terminal response control in the medial or anterior cingulate cortex, premotor areas, and frontal areas, likely following dopaminergic input from basal ganglia [7882]. Clinical studies have shown that no-go P3 was altered in amplitude or anteriorization in patients with ADHD and may, like CNV, represent a persistent neurophysiological deficit [69••, 83, 84], although similar effects are present in a wide range of other disorders such as Parkinson’s [85] and Huntington’s [82] diseases.

The more parietally distributed Go-P3 has been associated with several attentional functions such as evaluation of stimuli, closure, and resource allocation [86, 87], and may thus reflect executive response control. Explicit analyses of Go-P3 evoked by the CPT in ADHD are rare, but some instances of diminished amplitude in patients have been observed [70, 72, 88, 89].

In addition to clarification of neural dysfunctions in ADHD, early studies on brain electrical activity during CPT performance were conducted to evaluate the impact of methylphenidate medication. Studies showed that MPH ameliorated CPT-X performance and led to increased N2 and P3 amplitudes to targets and non-targets in children with ADHD [90, 91]. However, these earlier investigations were often limited by relatively small sample size (often less than 15 participants per sample) and a relatively poor spatial resolution of the recorded signals due to a low number of recorded sites (often less than 10). Limitations of sample size were overcome by the use of multicentre studies [58], and advances in amplifier technology allowed high-density recordings with 32 channels, as well as detailed assessment of topography during stable micro states in brain mapping approaches [92].

CPT was later used to differentiate ADHD subtypes or frequent comorbidities. For example, a study from our lab revealed that children with comorbid ADHD and oppositional defiant or conduct disorder (ODD/CD) were characterized by less abnormal performance, Cue-P3, and -CNV than suggested by an additive model of impairments found in children with pure ADHD and ODD/CD symptoms [56].

Genetics

Although heritability estimates of 70 % explained symptom variance in twins suggest a strong genetic impact on ADHD, association with candidate genes were rather moderate, and genome-wide association scans (GWAS) thus far have revealed no significant effects. This may suggest that ADHD is a genetically heterogeneous disorder on which frequent variants have a very small effect (potentially less than 0.5 % explained variance), while variants with larger effects are likely rare, and the situation may be complicated by gene–gene and gene–environment interactions and may be further moderated by epigenetic influences [25, 93••, 94].

Endophenotypes, which are characterized by more fundamental biological properties between genetic or environmental risk factors and the phenotype, may help to clarify these associations through potentially larger and more specific genetic effects, and may thus offer advantages for molecular genetic studies [95]. In many cases, risk factors are expected to be shared within families, suggesting that non-affected relatives show some (milder) impairments in these parameters. This was demonstrated with the CPT in studies during childhood and adolescence, where boys with ADHD as well as their non-affected siblings showed diminished brain activity related to preparation (Cue-P3 and -CNV) and response inhibition (no-go P3) compared to controls without a family history of ADHD [96]. Cue-P3 and No-Go-P3 remain familial-driven during adulthood [97], and a familial transmission of CPT commission errors between parents and children has also been demonstrated [98], suggesting that the CPT may uncover endophenotypes for ADHD.

Studies on the impact of certain candidate genes specifically related to the dopaminergic system on CPT performance or associated brain activity have spanned nearly the last decade, and have elucidated the impact of various candidate genes. This has been illustrated for the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) and dopamine transporter (DAT1) genes.

The DRD4 gene is located on chromosome 11p15.5. It modulates the D2-like receptor, which is particularly present in the frontal lobe and the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortex [99, 100]. It has a variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) between 2 and 10, of which 2, 4, and 7 are most common in humans. The 7-repeat (7R) polymorphism in particular is less sensitive to dopaminergic stimulation [101, 102]. Recent meta-analyses suggest that DRD4 7R is also moderately associated with ADHD [25, 99, 103, 104] and novelty-seeking [105], which highlights the role it may play in attention and motivation.

In an early study using an uncued CPT implemented in T.O.V.A. in children with ADHD, increased impulsivity as measured by commission errors was significantly associated with the shorter (shorter than 7-, and particularly 2-repeat) variants of the DRD4 gene (as well as with the long polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase gene A [MAO-A]), which was ameliorated by methylphenidate [106, 107]. Importantly, the studies by Manor et al. used the uncued CPT as implemented in T.O.V.A., which did not tap preparation processes. Moreover, the authors found (and replicated) the short DRD4 alleles associated with ADHD in their sample from Israel, which is in contrast to findings from other samples. Thus further research is needed in order to interpret these “paradoxical” findings that an ADHD risk allele was associated with fewer impulsivity errors in the CPT.

A larger study in patients with ADHD carrying the DRD4 7R allele revealed faster and more error-prone responses in the Matching Familiar Figures Test and lower response speed in the stop task, but no DRD4 effects were detected in the go/no-go task or the CPT [108]. Unfortunately, details regarding the CPT procedure used were not provided, but it was likely an implementation without cues that require a response if a stimulus is repeated.

DRD4 polymorphisms also had an impact on error rates in a simplified CPT requiring a response to a target from a sequence of numbers. Children with ADHD carrying at least one 7R allele showed more commission errors, while children homozygous for the 4R allele showed protective effects and made fewer errors of commission and omission [109].

A more recent study with a cued CPT found evidence that the DRD4 7R variant may be associated with inattention as reflected in omission errors, which is dissociated from the impact of the DAT1 gene (see below) [110]. A recent study from our lab with a cued CPT was consistent with Gizer and Waldman’s findings regarding performance (children with DRD4 7R showed slower and more heterogeneous reaction times), as well as preparation problems as indicated by lower Cue-P3 and -CNV amplitude [111]. Importantly, these DRD4 effects were similarly present in children with ADHD and controls, and explained approximately a 4–5 % variance.

In sum, the impact of DRD4 polymorphisms on the CPT appears to be heterogeneous, depending on the task variant applied. While studies with the more difficult cued CPT-AX detected attention and preparation problems in carriers of the 7R allele, the simpler uncued CPT-X revealed results that are less clear. This is consistent with the well-known clinical impression that children with ADHD show more difficulties when the going gets tough.

The dopamine transporter gene (DAT1, SLC6A3) codes the carrier protein responsible for the reuptake of dopamine from the synaptic cleft, particularly in the mesolimbic (including ventral tegmentum and nucleus accumbens, and associated with reward processing) and mesocortical (also starting in the ventral tegmentum with projections to the prefrontal cortex, associated with arousal and various attention problems) dopamine systems [112, 14]. This may explain why methylphenidate, which blocks the dopamine transporter, resulting in higher levels of available dopamine in the synaptic cleft, has such a widespread and immediate impact on ADHD symptoms [113]. The DAT1 gene has two commonly studied VNTR polymorphisms of 9 or 10 repeats of a 40-basepair sequence in the 3’ untranslated region (UTR), but the effect of these polymorphisms on the dopamine transporter functioning, and associations with ADHD in particular, remain inconclusive [104], which may be explained by developmental or environmental effects [114]. As such, the 10-repeat (10R) allele may be a risk factor for childhood ADHD, while the 9-repeat polymorphism has been associated with persistent ADHD in adults [115].

There is some evidence linking DAT1 10R to impulsivity. An early study with an uncued CPT detected more heterogeneous response speed, more commission errors, and a more impulsive response style in children homozygous for the 10R [116]. This in in line with problems with selective attention and response inhibition detected in children homozygous for the 10R allele [117]. However, a Korean sample in ADHD children with DAT 9R showed more commission errors [118].

The aforementioned study by Gizer and Waldman with cued CPT also revealed elevated commission errors, indicating higher impulsivity in children with the DAT1 10R allele.

Findings from our study may complement these results, as children with the DAT1 10–6 haplotype did not show higher commission error rates; rather (potentially as a compensatory mechanism) the No-Go-P3 amplitude as an electrophysiological marker of response inhibition was higher [111]. This demonstrates that accessory analysis of brain activity may uncover potentially compensatory neural processes that may lead to normal overt performance.

Studies in adults appear to be less consistent. One study on CPT performance in healthy participants with the 10R allele showed faster response speed and more commission errors (and thus a rather impulsive response style) [119], while another EEG study revealed no clear performance differences, but less no-go P3 anteriorization specifically in patients with ADHD [120].

Taken together, studies on the impact of DRD4 and DAT1 polymorphisms on CPT performance and associated brain activity have yielded mixed results. This may be at least partially explained by differences in the CPT implementation, particularly the time pressure induced by shorter inter-stimulus intervals and whether the CPT includes cues. In the first instance, the DRD4 7R polymorphism may lead to preparation problems related to cue processing that may result in diminished responses to the adjacent targets. The impact of DAT1 variants may vary with age groups and the population assessed, but the 10R variant may be associated with impulsivity or compensatory response inhibition in children. Importantly, genetic effects in children explained about 4 % to 5 % variance in performance and electrophysiological brain activity parameters of preparation and response control, which is much greater than associations with ADHD detected in GWAS thus far, indicating that endophenotypes based on brain activity may be a promising approach for understanding the role of genetic and environmental factors on ADHD and other psychiatric disorders.

Conclusions

The understanding of ADHD has undergone a number of important changes. Brain dysfunctions were proposed as early as the beginning of the twentieth century. The CPT, introduced for the diagnosis of vigilance or sustained attention problems in patients with brain damage, was consequently applied early on in ADHD research, and has been a valuable tool for the assessment of cognitive and motivational deficits in ADHD. Future applications include treatment evaluation and differentiation of ADHD subtypes and associated or comorbid disorders. Early studies characterized CPT performance at the behavioral level with regard to omission and commission errors, response speed, and signal detection parameters. Subsequently, brain activity was assessed using fMRI and ERP to uncover activity in neural networks associated with attention, preparation, and response control. Further studies on genetic as well as epigenetic influences may lead to a deeper understanding of information processing in the brain and problems associated with ADHD.

Taken together, the CPT has helped to uncover important pieces of the ADHD puzzle, and is still useful for current research. The joint history of ADHD and CPT may explain the ongoing interest in a heritage task from the middle of the last century, suggesting that both concepts may outlast many researchers currently working on them.