Skip to main content

Improving Understanding of Health-Relevant Numerical Information

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Psychological Perspectives on Risk and Risk Analysis

Abstract

In this chapter, we discuss why risks are often not communicated in a transparent and understandable way and why this is problematic. At the core of the chapter are four examples that illustrate how risk communication can be improved. These examples are (a) the use of natural frequencies in the context of diagnostic reasoning, (b) the use of visual aids to support the beneficial effect of natural frequency representations, (c) the use of natural frequencies to clarify the distinction between relative and absolute risk reduction, and (d) a clarification of the meaning and pitfalls of survival rates that are often used to quantify the benefit of screening programs. In each of these topics, we describe original empirical studies illuminating a specific problem as well as how these problems can be overcome, and we discuss practical implications of the results and the proposed solutions. Subsequently, we illustrate, using an example from mammography screening, what transparent risk communication could look like. The chapter concludes with a discussion of training programs designed to enhance health-related, high-stakes decision making.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ahmed, H., Naik, G., Willoughby, H., & Edwards, A. G. (2012). Communicating risk. British Medical Journal, 344, e3996.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albert, U., Alt, D., Kreienberg, R., Naβ-Griegoleit, I., Schulte, H., & Wöckel, A. (2010). Früherkennung von Brustkrebs—Eine Entscheidungshilfe für Frauen. In Leitlinienprogramm Onkologie. der Arbeitsgemeinschaft der Wissenschaftlichen Medizinischen Fachgesellschaften e.V., der Deutschen Krebsgesellschaft e.V. und der Deutschen Krebshilfe e.V. (Hrsg.). Retrieved June 19, 2018, from https://www.leitlinienprogramm-onkologie.de/fileadmin/user_upload/Downloads/Patientenleitlinien/Patientenleitlinie_Brustkrebs_Frueherkennung.pdf

  • Anderson, B. L., & Schulkin, J. (2014). Numerical reasoning in judgments and decision making about health. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Arkes, H. R. (1991). Costs and benefits of judgment errors: Implications for debiasing. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 486–498. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.110.3.486

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B. (2006). Mathematical expertise. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 553–568). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cokely, E. T., Feltz, A., Ghazal, S., Allan, J. N., Petrova, D., & Garcia-Retamero, R. (2018). Decision making skill: From intelligence to numeracy and expertise. In K. A. Ericsson, R. R. Hoffman, A. Kozbelt, & A. M. Williams (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (2nd Ed.) (pp. 476–505). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cokely, E. T., Galesic, M., Schulz, E., Ghazal, S., & Garcia-Retamero, R. (2012). Measuring risk literacy: The Berlin Numeracy Test. Judgment and Decision Making, 7(1), 25–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cokely, E. T., Ghazal, S., & Garcia-Retamero, R. (2014). Measuring numeracy. In B. L. Anderson & J. Schulkin (Eds.), Numerical reasoning in judgments and decision making about health (pp. 11–38). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Eddy, D. M. (1982). Probabilistic reasoning in clinical medicine: Problems and opportunities. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 249–267). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fagerlin, A., Ubel, P. A., Smith, D. M., & Zikmund-Fisher, B. J. (2007). Making numbers matter: Present and future research in risk communication. American Journal of Health Behavior, 31, 47–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagerlin, A., Wang, C., & Ubel, P. A. (2005). Reducing the influence of anecdotal reasoning on people’s health care decisions: Is a picture worth a thousand statistics? Medical Decision Making, 25(4), 398–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fong, G. T., Krantz, D. H., & Nisbett, R. E. (1986). The effects of statistical training on thinking about everyday problems. Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 253–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90001-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, W. T. (1817). The private correspondence of Benjamin Franklin (Vol. 1, 2nd ed.). London: Henry Colborn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaissmaier, W., Wegwarth, O., Skopec, D., Müller, A., Broschinski, S., & Politi, M. C. (2012). Numbers can be worth a thousand pictures: Individual differences in understanding graphical and numerical representations of health-related information. Health Psychology, 31, 286–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Retamero, R., Cokely, E., & Hoffrage, U. (2015). Visual aids improve diagnostic inferences and metacognitive judgment calibration. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 932 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00932

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Retamero, R., & Cokely, E. T. (2011). Effective communication of risks to young adults: Using message framing and visual aids to increase condom use and STD screening. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17(3), 270–287.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Retamero, R., & Cokely, E. T. (2013). Communicating health risks with visual aids. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(5), 392–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Retamero, R., & Hoffrage, U. (2013). Visual representation of statistical information improves diagnostic inferences in doctors and their patients. Social Science & Medicine, 83, 27–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghosh, A. K., & Ghosh, K. (2005). Translating evidence-based information into effective risk communication: Current challenges and opportunities. Journal of Laboratory and Clinical Medicine, 145(4), 171–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Calculated risks: How to know when numbers deceive you. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Edwards, A. (2003). Simple tools for understanding risks: From innumeracy to insight. British Medical Journal, 327, 741–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Gaissmaier, W., Kurz-Milcke, E., Schwartz, L. M., & Woloshin, S. (2007). Helping doctors and patients make sense of health statistics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8(2), 53–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instruction: Frequency formats. Psychological Review, 102(4), 684–704.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Muir Gray, J. A. (Eds.). (2011). Better doctors, better patients, better decisions: Envisioning health care 2020. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gøtzsche P. C., Hartling O. J., Nielsen M., & Brodersen J. (2012). Screening for breast cancer with mammography. The Nordic Cochrane Centre. Retrieved June 19, 2018, from http://nordic.cochrane.org/screening-breast-cancer-mammography

  • Griebenow, B. (2008). Beratung zur Krebsfrüherkennung: Vor- und Nachteile darstellen. Deutsches Ärzteblatt, 105(33), A-1724/B-1488/C-1456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haas, G. P., Delongchamps, N., Brawley, O. W., Wang, C. Y., & de la Roza, G. (2008). The worldwide epidemiology of prostate cancer: Perspectives from autopsy studies. The Canadian Journal of Urology, 15(1), 3866–3871.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, J. L. (2008). Shared decision making: Have we missed the obvious? Archives of Internal Medicine, 168, 1368–1370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harding Center for Risk Literacy. (2018). Retrieved June 19, 2018, from https://www.harding-center.mpg.de/en/fact-boxes

  • Heijnsdijk, E. A., Wever, E. M., Auvinen, A., Hugosson, J., Ciatto, S., Nelen, V., … Zappa, M. (2012). Quality-of-life effects of prostate-specific antigen screening. New England Journal of Medicine, 367(7), 595–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffrage, U. (2016). Overconfidence. In R. F. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive illusions: Intriguing phenomena in thinking, judgement, and memory (2nd ed., pp. 291–314). Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffrage, U., & Gigerenzer, G. (1998). Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences. Academic Medicine, 73(5), 538–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffrage, U., Gigerenzer, G., Krauss, S., & Martignon, L. (2002). Representation facilitates reasoning: What natural frequencies are and what they are not. Cognition, 84(3), 343–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffrage, U., & Koller, M. (2015). Chances und risks in medical risk communication. German Medical Science, 13, Doc07 (pp. 1–7). https://doi.org/10.3205/000211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffrage, U., Kurzenhäuser, S., & Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Wie kann man die Bedeutung medizinischer Testbefunde besser verstehen und kommunizieren? [How to improve the communication and understanding of medical test results?]. Zeitschrift für Ärztliche Fortbildung und Qualitätssicherung, 94, 713–719.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffrage, U., Lindsey, S., Hertwig, R., & Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Communicating statistical information. Science, 290, 2261–2262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jørgensen, K. J., Brodersen, J., Hartling, O. J., Nielsen, M., & Gøtzsche, P. C. (2009). Informed choice requires information about both benefits and harms. Journal of Medical Ethics, 35(4), 268–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jørgensen, K. J., Gøtzsche, P. C., Kalager, M., & Zahl, P. (2017). Screening in Denmark: A cohort study of tumor size and overdiagnosis. Annals of Internal Medicine, 166(5), 313–323. https://doi.org/10.7326/M16-0270 (see also the summary for patients. Retrieved June19, 2018, from, http://annals.org/aim/article/2597576/breast-cancer-tumor-size-overdiagnosis).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, K., & Mühlhauser, I. (2008). Kriterien zur Erstellung von Patienteninformationen zu Krebsfrüherkennungsuntersuchungen: Stellungnahme des Fachbereichs Patienteninformation des Deutschen Netzwerkes für Evidenzbasierte Medizin (DNEbM). Retrieved June 19, 2018, from http://www.ebm-netzwerk.de/pdf/stellungnahmen/dnebm-080630.pdf

  • Kurzenhäuser, S. (2003). Welche Informationen vermitteln deutsche Gesundheitsbroschüren über die Screening-Mammographie? [What information is provided in German health information pamphlets on mammography screening?]. In Zeitschrift für ärztliche Fortbildung und Qualitätssicherung (Vol. 97, pp. 53–57).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurzenhäuser, S., & Hoffrage, U. (2002). Teaching Bayesian reasoning: An evaluation of a classroom tutorial for medical students. Medical Teacher, 24(5), 516–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzenhäuser, S., & Hoffrage, U. (2012). Designing risk communication in health. In P. M. Todd, G. Gigerenzer, & the ABC Research Group (Eds.), Ecological rationality: Intelligence in the world (pp. 428–453). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larrick, R. P. (2004). Debiasing. In D. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 316–338). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470752937.ch16

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lipkus, I. M. (2007). Numeric, verbal, and visual formats of conveying health risks: Suggested best practices and future recommendations. Medical Decision Making, 27, 696–713. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X07307271

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malek, D., & Kääb-Sanyal, V. (2016). Jahresbericht Evaluation 2014. Deutsches Mammographie-Screening-Programm. In: Kooperationsgemeinschaft Mammographie, Berlin. (Hrsg.) Retrieved June 19, 2018, from http://newsroom.mammo-programm.de/download/fachpublikation/KOOPMAMMO_Jahresbericht_EVAL2014_20161206_klein.pdf

  • Matter-Walstra, K., & Hoffrage, U. (2001). Individuelle Entscheidungsfindung am Beispiel der Brustkrebs-Früherkennung: Erfahrungen aus Fokusgruppen in der Schweiz [Individual decision making concerning breast cancer screening: Observations with focus groups in Switzerland]. Schweizer Zeitschrift für Managed Care und Care Management, 3(5), 26–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, M., Rebitschek, F. G., Gigerenzer, G., & Wegwarth, O. (2016). A simple tool for communicating the benefits and harms of health interventions: A guide for creating a fact box. Medical Decision Making Policy & Practise, 1, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mechanic, D., & Meyer, S. (2000). Concepts of trust among patients with serious illness. Social Science and Medicine, 51, 657–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michaels, D. (2006). Regarding “Phenylpropanolamine and hemorrhagic stroke in the hemorrhagic stroke project”: Mercenary epidemiology—Data reanalysis and reinterpretation for sponsors with financial interest in the outcome. Annals of Epidemiology, 16, 49–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morewedge, C. K., Yoon, H., Scopelliti, I., Symborski, C. W., Korris, J. H., & Kassam, K. S. (2015). Debiasing decisions: Improved decision making with a single training intervention. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2(1), 129–140. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215600886

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, A. S., Stevenson, E., Kerr, F., & Burns, C. (2010). “A heartbeat moment:” Qualitative study of GP views of patients bringing health information from the internet to a consultation. British Journal of General Practice, 60, 88–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E., Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Mazzocco, K., & Dickert, S. (2006). Numeracy and decision making. Psychological Science, 17, 407–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, L. M., & Woloshin, S. (2007). Participation in mammography screening: Women should be encouraged to decide what is right for them, rather than being told what to do. British Medical Journal, 335, 731–732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, L. M., Woloshin, S., & Welch, H. G. (2007). The drug facts box: Providing consumers with simple tabular data on drug benefit and harm. Medical Decision Making, 27, 655–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, L. M., Woloshin, S., & Welch, H. G. (2009). Using a drug facts box to communicate drug benefits and harms: Two randomized trials. Annals of Internal Medicine, 150(8), 516–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skolbekken, J. A. (1998). Communicating the risk reduction achieved by cholesterol reducing drugs. British Medical Journal, 316, 1956–1958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soll, J. B., Milkman, K. L., & Payne, J. W. (2015). A user’s guide to debiasing. In G. Keren & G. Wu (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 924–951). Chichester: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118468333.ch33

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Spiegelhalter, D., Pearson, M., & Short, I. (2011). Visualizing uncertainty about the future. Science, 333(6048), 1393–1400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starr, P. (1949). The social transformation of American medicine. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Studdert, D. M., Mello, M. M., Sage, W. M., DesRoches, C. M., Peugh, J., Zapert, K., et al. (2005). Defensive medicine among high-risk specialist physicians in a volatile malpractice environment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, 2660–2662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tait, A. R., Voepel-Lewis, T., Zikmund-Fisher, B. J., & Fagerlin, A. (2010). The effect of format on parents’ understanding of the risks and benefits of clinical research: A comparison between text, tables, and graphics. Journal of Health Communication, 15(5), 487–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgerson, C., Porthouse, J., & Brooks, G. (2005). A systematic review of controlled trials evaluating interventions in adult literacy and numeracy. Journal of Research in Reading, 28(2), 87–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, E. A., Weinstein, N. D., Colditz, G. A., & Emmons, K. M. (2007). Reducing aversion to side effects in preventive medical treatment decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13(1), 11–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegwarth, O., Gaissmaier, W., & Gigerenzer, G. (2011). Deceiving numbers: Survival rates and their impact on doctors’ risk communication. Medical Decision Making, 31(3), 386–394. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X10391469

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegwarth, O., & Gigerenzer, G. (2011). Risikokommunikation: Risiken und Unsicherheiten richtig verstehen lernen. Deutsches Ärzteblatt, 108(9), A448–A451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegwarth, O., & Gigerenzer, G. (2013). Overdiagnosis and overtreatment: Evaluation of what physicians tell their patients about screening harms. JAMA Internal Medicine, 173(22), 2086–2087.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welch, H. G., & Frankel, B. A. (2011). Likelihood that a woman with screen-detected breast cancer has had her “life saved” by that screening. Archives of Internal Medicine, 171(22), 2043–2046.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weymayr, C. (2010). Kennzahlen Mammographie-Screening. Dokumentation 2010, im Auftrag der Kooperationsgemeinschaft Mammographie, Version 1.2. Retrieved June 19, 2018, from http://fachservice.mammo-programm.de/download/Kennzahlen_Mammographie-Screening_in_Deutschland_2010.pdf

  • Woloshin, S., & Schwartz, L. M. (2009). Numbers needed to decide. Journal of the National Cancer Institute., 101(17), 1163–1165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xin, Y. P., & Jitendra, A. K. (1999). The effects of instruction in solving mathematical word problems for students with learning problems: A meta-analysis. The Journal of Special Education, 32(4), 207–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the editors of this book for helpful comments on an earlier version and the Swiss National Science Foundation (100014–140503/1) and the Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (Spain) (PSI2011-22954 and PSI2014-51842-R) for financial support. Parts of this chapter are adopted from Hoffrage and Koller (2015).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ulrich Hoffrage .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Hoffrage, U., Garcia-Retamero, R. (2018). Improving Understanding of Health-Relevant Numerical Information. In: Raue, M., Lermer, E., Streicher, B. (eds) Psychological Perspectives on Risk and Risk Analysis. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-92478-6_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics