Skip to main content
Log in

Nonverbal behaviors as indices of arousal: Extension to the psychotherapy context

  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In an investigation guided by the premise that overt nonverbal indices may often be practical and valid indicators of arousal, Burgoon, Kelley, Newton, and Keeley-Dyreson (1989) found different nonverbal behaviors associated with arousal intensity and with positively and negatively valenced arousal. The current investigation extends that research to a new context, examining (1) the ability of a global measure of observed arousal and specific nonverbal indices of arousal intensity and valence to detect presumed nonlinear arousal changes during psychotherapy, (2) the differential impact of nonverbal indicators across phases of a therapy session, and (3) the relationship of nonverbal indicators to therapy outcome. Raters judged all-channel global arousal or specific kinesic and vocalic nonverbal behaviors during Focused Expressive Psychotherapy (FEP) sessions, during which high levels of negative arousal are evoked. Manifest global arousal, random movement/self-adaptors, and kinesic pleasantness conformed to hypothesized nonlinear patterns across time within therapy sessions. Nonverbal correlates of manifest global arousal varied across therapy phases. Moderate manifest arousal, especially random movement/self-adaptors and vocal tension, and high vocal expressiveness during the middle of the therapy work session were most predictive of patients' self-reported resolution at the end of therapy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andersen, P. A. (1985). Nonverbal immediacy in interpersonal communication. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.),Multichannel integrations of nonverbal behavior (pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1978).Depression inventory. Philadelphia: Center for Cognitive Therapy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beutler, L. E., & Clarkin, J. F. (1990).Systematic treatment selection: Toward targeted therapeutic intervention. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beutler, L. E., Engle, D., Oro'-Beutler, M. E., Daldrup, R., & Meredith, K. (1986). Inability to express intense affect: A common link between depression and pain?Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54 752–759.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beutler, L. E., Engle, D., Shoham-Salomon, V., Mohr, D. C., Dean, J. C., & Bernat, E. M. (1991). University of Arizona: Searching for differential treatments. In L. E. Beutler & M. Crago (Eds.),Psychotherapy research: An international review of programmatic studies (pp. 90–97). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beutler, L. E., Frank, M., Schieber, S. C., Calvert, S., & Gaines, J. (1984). Comparative effects of group psychotherapies in a short-term inpatient setting: An experience with deterioration effects.Psychiatry, 47 66–76.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blascovich, J., & Katkin, E. S. (1982). Arousal-based social behaviors: Do they reflect differences in visceral perception? In L. Wheeler (Ed.),Review of personality and social psychology, Vol. 3 (pp. 73–95). Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, B. L., Warner, C. A., & Williams, R. N. (1985). Vocal paralanguage without unconscious processes. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.),Multichannel integrations of nonverbal behavior (pp. 149–193). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1979). Individual differences in nonverbal sending accuracy and electrodermal responding: The externalizing-internalizing dimension. In R. Rosenthal (Ed.),Skill in nonverbal communication: Individual differences (p. 140–170). Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn & Hain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. (1978). A communication model of personal space violations: Explication and an initial test.Human Communication Research, 4 129–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. (1983). Nonverbal violations of expectations. In J. M. Wiemann & R. P. Harrison (Eds.),Nonverbal interaction (pp. 77–111). Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Beutler, L. E., Le Poire, B. A., Engle, D., Bergan, J., Salvio, M-A., & Mohr, D. C. (1992).Nonverbal indices of arousal in group psychotherapy. Manuscript under review for publication.

  • Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1988). Nonverbal expectancy violations: Model elaboration and application to immediacy behaviors.Communication Monographs, 55 58–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Kelley, D. L., Newton, D. A., & Keeley-Dyreson, M. P. (1989). The nature of arousal and nonverbal indices.Human Communication Research, 16 217–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., & Le Poire, B. A. (1992). A reply from the heart: Who are Sparks and Greene and why are they saying all these horrible things?Human Communication Research, 18 472–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Newton, D. A., Walther, J. B., & Baesler, E. J. (1989). Nonverbal expectancy violations and conversational involvement.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13 97–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N., & Greene, J. O. (1982). A discrepancy-arousal explanation of mutual influence in expressive behavior for adult and infant-adult interaction.Communication Monographs, 49 89–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N., & Greene, J. O. (1984). The effects of distance and individual differences in arousability on nonverbal involvement: A test of discrepancy-arousal theory.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8 259–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christ, W. G. (1985). The construct of arousal in communication research.Human Communication Research, 11 575–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daldrup, R. J., Beutler, L. E., Engle, D., & Greenberg, L. S. (1988).Focused expressive psychotherapy: Freeing the overcontrolled patient. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, E. (1962).Activation and behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Nonverbal leakage and clues to deception.Psychiatry, 1 88–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43 522–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, M. (1967). Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness.British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6 278–296.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harré, R. (1972).The philosophies of science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the C. N. S.Psychological Review, 62 243–254.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Katkin, E. S., Blascovich, J., & Goldband, S. (1981). Empirical assessment of visceral self-perception: Individual and sex differences in the acquisition of heartbeat discrimination.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40 1095–1101.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Katkin, E. S., Morell, M. A., Goldband, S., Bernstein, G. L., & Wise, J. A. (1982). Individual differences in heartbeat discrimination.Psychophysiology, 19 160–166.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, J. I. (1950). Individual differences in somatic response patterns.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43 338–350.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, J. I. (1967). Somatic response patterning and stress: Some revisions of activation theory. In M. H. Appley & R. Trumball (Eds.),Psychological stress: Issues in research. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Poire, B. A. (1991).Two contrasting explanations of involvement violations: Orientation response or affective reaction? Unpublished dissertation, University of Arizona, Tucson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Poire, B. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (1992, May).Usefulness of the ‘orientation response’ for theories of nonverbal involvement violations: The orienting versus arousal debate. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Communication Association.

  • Lynn, R. (1966).Attention, arousal, and the orientation reaction. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1975).Mind and emotion. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manstead, A. S. R. (1991). Expressiveness as an individual difference. In R. S. Feldman & B. Rimé (Eds.),Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 285–328). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1977). Individual differences in stimulus screening and arousability.Journal of Personality, 45 237–250.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1973). A measure of arousal seeking tendency.Environment and Behavior, 5 315–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L. C., Murphy, R., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Consciousness of body: Private and public.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41 397–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L. (1976). An arousal model of interpersonal intimacy.Psychological Review, 83 235–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L. (1983).Nonverbal behavior: A functional perspective. New-York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pedhazur, E. (1982).Multiple regression in behavioral research (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W. (1981). Accuracy of symptom and perception. In J. E. Singer & A. Baum (Eds.),Handbook of medical psychology (Vol. 5). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, L. E., Helzer, J. E., Croughan, J., & Radcliffe, K. S. (1981). National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: History, characteristics, and validity.Archives of General Psychology, 38 381–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seta, J. J., Paulus, P. B., & Schkade, J. K. (1976). Effects of group size and proximity under cooperative and competitive conditions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34 47–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E. N.Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Pergamon Press.

  • Sparks, G. G., & Greene, J. O. (1992). On the validity of nonverbal indicators as measures of physiological arousal: A response to Burgoon, Kelley, Newton, and Keeley-Dyreson.Human Communication Research, 18 445–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tecce, J. J. (1972). Contingent negative variation (CNV) and psychological processes in man.Psychological Bulletin, 77 73–108.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, R. M. & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation.Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18 459–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zillmann, D. (1978). Attribution and misattribution of excitatory reactions. In J. H. Harvey, W. J. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.),New directions in attribution research, Vol. 2 (pp. 335–368). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zillmann, D. (1982). Television viewing and arousal. In D. Pearl, L. Bouthilet, & J. Lazar (Eds.),Television and behavior: Ten years of scientific progress and implications for the eighties. Vol. 2: Technical reviews. U.S. Public Health Service Publication No. ADM 82-1196 (pp. 53–67). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zillmann, D. (1984).Connections between sex and aggression. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Portions of this research were supported through grant no. MH-R01-39859 from the National Institute of Mental Health and were presented in an earlier version to the annual meeting of the Speech Communication Association, Chicago, November 1990.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Burgoon, J.K., Le Poire, B.A., Beutler, L.E. et al. Nonverbal behaviors as indices of arousal: Extension to the psychotherapy context. J Nonverbal Behav 16, 159–178 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00988032

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00988032

Keywords

Navigation