Skip to main content
Log in

What makes a good story

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Three different ways have been selected for analyzing a story's structure, each focusing on different types of information. (a) Episodic or story grammar approaches stories as problem-solving episodes, emphasizing goals and activities to achieve them. (b) Labov's high point structure emphasizes affective information and sees stories as organized around emotional high points or crisis events. (c) Deese's dependency analysis emphasizes linguistic complexity and, in particular, the way propositions are related to each other through a relationship of either coordination or subordination. Stories were scored according to how well they realized good structure in each system, and the three scoring systems were relatively independent of each other. Adults were asked to rate 3-to 9-year old children's personal narratives in terms of how good a story each was, and their ratings were compared to how the stories were scored in terms of all three systems. None of the systems completely explained the subjective quality ratings; rather, all three seemed to contribute in different ways to subjects' ratings, with the best narratives generally deemed sophisticated and complex in at least two of the three systems.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Black, J. B., & Wilensky, R. (1979). An evaluation of story grammars.Cognitive Science, 3, 213–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973).A first language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiensen, F., & Christiensen, B. (1976).A new rhetoric. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deese, J. (1983).Thought into speech: Psychology of a language. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, C. (1978). The role of episodic structure and of story length on children's recall of single stories.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 229–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, C., & Stein, N. (1980). Syntactic structures and real world themes in stories generated by children (Technical report). Urbana: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. J. (1974). Remember that old theory of memory? Well, forget it.American Psychologist, 785–795.

  • Kintsch, W., & Greene, E. (1978). The role of culture-specific schemata in the comprehension and recall of stories.Discourse Processes, 1, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W. (1972)Language in the inner city. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W., Cohen, P., Robins, C., & Lewis, J. (1968).A study of the non-standard English of Negro and Puerto Rican speakers in New York City (Vol. 2) (Cooperative Research Project No. 3288). Washington, D.C.: Office of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In J. Helm (Ed.)Essays on the verbal and visual arts. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M. (1978). A code in the node: The use of a story schema in retrieval.Discourse Process, 1, 14–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall.Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M., Scribner, S., Cole, M., & DeForest, M. (1980). Cross-cultural invariance in story recall.Child Development, 51, 19–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983)Three ways of looking at a child's narrative: A psycholinguistic analysis. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollard-Gott, L., McCloskey, M., & Todres, A. (1979). Subjective story structure.Discourse Processes, 2, 251–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories In D. G. Bobros & A. Collins (Eds.),Representation and understanding: Studies in cognitive science. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Understanding and summarizing brief stories. In D. LaBerge & S. J. Samuels (Eds.),Basic processes in reading: Perception and comprehension. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1977).Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. Freedle (Ed.),New directions in discourse processing. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndyke, P. W. (1977). Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse.Cognitive Psychology, 9, 77–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, T. A., & Petoefi, J. (Eds.) (1977).Grammars and description: Studies in text theory and text analysis. Netherlands: DeGruytier.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The authors wish to acknowledge their great debt to James Deese and Christine Glenn. This research was supported by the National Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Grant A-7911 to Carole Peterson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McCabe, A., Peterson, C. What makes a good story. J Psycholinguist Res 13, 457–480 (1984). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01068179

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01068179

Keywords

Navigation