Skip to main content
Log in

What animated illustrations conditions can improve technical document comprehension in young students? Format, signaling and control of the presentation

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We study the comprehension of a multimedia technical document about gear functioning by young pupils. The research is focused on the effect of three factors on the construction of a mental model: illustration format (animated versus static) signaling cues (presence versus absence) learner-control of information delivery (three rhythms of presentation: speed, slow and self-controlled). The experimental procedure, conducted with 123 children, follows three phases: pre-test, individual passation of the lesson, comprehension test, delayed post-test. The goal of the pre-test is the evaluation of prior knowledge about gears, but also the control of spatial and verbal working memory aptitude and reading performance. The results show an effect of the animated format, of signaling cues and of the rhythm on the immediate comprehension test and delayed test. For the immediate comprehension test, these effects are different according to the kind of comprehension question (recall, transfer, explanation). These effects are maintained at the delayed post-test, for the self-controlled condition and for the pupils with low prior knowledge. The factor information delivery rhythm shows an effect for the delayed post-test. Our observation device of the behaviour of the child during the lesson was specially designed to explore the reading strategies between the medias.

Résumé

Cet article porte sur la compréhension d’un document technique multimédia concernant le fonctionnement des engrenages pour de jeunes élèves. Nous étudions le rôle de trois facteurs: le format de présentation de l’illustration (dynamique ou statique) les signaux d’orientation attentionnelle (présence ou absence de guidages) et le contrôle par l’élève sur le déroulement de la leçon à travers trois rythmes de présentation (rapide, lent et auto-contrôlé). Une procédure expérimentale en quatre phases est conduite auprès d’une population de 123 élèves de primaire: pré-test, consultation individuelle de la leçon, épreuve de compréhension immédiate, post-test différé. Le pré-test comporte l’évaluation des connaissances préalables, des épreuves de mémoire de travail (verbales et spatiales) et une épreuve de lecture. Les résultats montrent un effet des animations, des guidages attentionnels ainsi que du rythme de présentation en compréhension immédiate et au post-test différé. En compréhension immédiate, ces effets sont différenciés selon le type de question (rappel, transfert, explications). Ils se maintiennent au post-test dans la condition d’auto-contrôle et surtout pour les élèves ayant de faibles connaissances préalables. La variable rythme de présentation à un effet au post-test différé. Le dispositif d’observation du comportement de l’élève permet d’explorer les stratégies de traitement des différents médias.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baccino, T. (2004).La lecture électronique.Grenoble: Presses Universitaires de Grenoble.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baeck, Y.K., & Layne, B.H. (1988). Color, graphics, and animation in computer-assisted learning tutorial lesson.Journal of computer-based instruction, 15, 131–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bétrancourt, M., & Dillenbourg, P. (2002). Learning from animation in individual and collaborative learning: Does the permanence of the previous frames on the screen improve learning? MeetingEARLI-SIG. “Comprehension of Verbal and Pictorial Information”. Poitiers, August, 29.

  • Bétrancourt, M., & Tversky, B. (2000). Effect of computer animation on user’s performance: A review.Le travail humain, 63(4), 311–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bétrancourt, M., & Tversky, B. (2001). Les animations sont-elles vraiment plus efficaces?Revue d’intelligence artificielle: Les interactions Homme-Système: Perspectives de recherches psycho-ergonomiques, 14(1–2), 149–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell, A.F., Jansen, A.R., & Marriott, K. (2000). Restricted focus viewer: A tool for tracking visual attention. In M. Anderson, P. Cheng, & V. Haarslev (Eds.),Theory and Application of Diagrams (pp. 162–177). Proceedings of the First International Conference, Diagrams. Berlin, New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Boucheix, J.M., & Noël, E. (2000). Influence des modalités de présentation d’illustrations chez de jeunes élèves.JETCSIC. Paris: Université Paris V.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P.A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duval, R. (1995). Sémiosis et pensée humaine. Registres sémiotiques et apprentissages intellectuels. Berne: Peter-Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M. (1992). Comprendre ce qu’on lit: De l’automatisme au contrôle. In M. Fayol, J.E. Gombert, P. Lecocq, L. Sprenger-Charolles, & D. Zagar (Eds.),Psychologie cognitive de la lecture (pp. 73–105). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M. (2002). Les documents techniques: Bilan et perspectives.Psychologie Française, 47(1), 9–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganier, F., Gombert, J.E., & Fayol, M. (2000). Effet du format de présentation des instructions sur l’apprentissage des procédures à l’aide de documents techniques.Le Travail Humain, 63(2), 121–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gombert, J.E., & Fayol, M. (1988). Auto-contrôle par l’enfant de ses réalisations dans des tâches cognitives.Revue Française de Pédagogie, 82, 47–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gyselinck, V., & Tardieu, H. (1999). The role of illustration in text comprehension: What, when, for whom and why? In, S.R. Goldman & H. van Oostendrop (Eds.),The construction of mental representation during reading (pp. 120–152). Mahwah, New Jersey, London: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gyselinck, V., Ehrlich, M.F., Cornoldi, R., De Beni, R., & Dubois, V. (2000). Visuo-spatial working memory in learning from multimedia systems.Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 16, 5–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hede, A. (2002). An integrated Model of Multimedia Effects on Learning.Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 11, 177–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heise, J., & Tversky, B. (2002). Descriptions and Depictions in Acquiring Complex Systems. In theproceedings of the 24th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.

  • Hegarty, M., & Just, M.A. (1993). Constructing mental models from texts and diagrams.Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 717–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, M., & Sims, V.K. (1994). Individual differences in mental animation during mechanical reasoning.Memory & Cognition, 22, 411–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, M., & Steinhoff, K. (1997). Individual differences in the use of diagrams as external memory in mechanical reasoning.Learning and Individual Differences, 9, 19–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, M., Narayanan, N.H., & Freitas, P. (2002). Understanding machines from multimedia and hypermedia presentations. In J. Otéro, J.A. Léon, & A.C. Graesser (Eds.),The psychology of Science Text Comprehension (pp. 357–384). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, M., Quilici, J., Narayanan, N.H., Holmquist, S., & Moreno, R. (1999). Designing multimedia manuals that explain how machines work: Lessons from evaluation theory-based design.Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 8, 119–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidrio, C., & Jamet, E. (2002a). Compréhension d’un dispositif technique: Apports d’une illustration dynamique et des traitements multiples.Psychologie Française, 47(1), 61–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidrio, C., & Jamet, E. (2002b). Comprehension of a spoken text: Effect of different types of illustrations.EARLI-SIG Meeting. Poitiers, August, 29.

  • Huck, J., & Floto E. (to appear). Signalling effects in multimedia learning.International Journal of Human-Computer Studies.

  • Jamet, E. (2002). L’apport de nouvelles technologies de l’information et de la communication dans la conception de documents techniques.Psychologie Française, 47(1), 33–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1983).Mental models. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, M.K., Proffitt, D.R., Whelan, S., & Hecht, H. (1992). Influence of animation on dynamical judgements.Journal of experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 669–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kieras, D. (1992). Diagrammatic display for ingineered systems: Effects on human performance interacting with malfunctioning systems.International Journal on Man-Machine studies, 36, 861–895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinze, C.K., Sherwood, R.D., & Loofbourrow, M. (1989). Simulation Software versus expository texts: A comparison of retention across two instructional tools.Reading research and Instruction, 28, 41–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarowitz, R., & Huppert, J. (1993). Science Process Skills of 10th grade biology students in a computer-assisted learning setting.Journal of Research in Computer in Education, 25, 366–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefavrais, P. (1967).Test de L’Alouette. Manuel et tests. Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée (ECPA), Paris.

  • Lowe, R.K. (1999). Extracting information from an animation during complex visual learning.European Journal of Psychology of Education, XIV(2), 225–244. [Special Issues: Visual Learning, W. Schnotz (Ed)]

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, R.K. (2003). Animation and learning: Selective processing of information in dynamic graphics.Learning and Instruction, 13, 157–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, R.K. (2004a). Interrogation of a dynamic visualization during learning.Learning and Instruction, 14, 257–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, R.K. (2004b). User-controllable animated diagrams: The solution for learning dynamic content. In A. Blackwell, K. Marriot, & A. Shimojima (Eds.),Diagrammatic representation and inference (pp. 355–359). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (1999). Instructional Technology. In F.T. Durso, R.S. Nickerson, R.W. Schvaneveldt, S.T. Dumais, D.S. Lindsay, & M.T.H. Chi (Eds.),Handbook of Applied Cognition (pp. 551–569). Chichester: John Wiley and Son’s.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E. (2001).Multimedia learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E. (2002). Using illustrations to promote constructivist learning from science text. In J. Otéro, J.A. Léon, & A.C. Graesser (Eds.),The psychology of Science Text Comprehension (pp. 333–356). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E., & Chandler, P. (2001). When learning is just a clik away: Does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of multimedia messages.Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 390–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer R.E., & Gallini, E. (1990). When is an illustration worth than thousand words?Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 715–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mautoné, P.D., & Mayer, R.E. (2001). Signaling as a cognitive guide in multimedia learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 377–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E., & Moreno, R. (1998). A split attention effect in multimedia learning: Evidence for dual processing systems in working memory.Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 312–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E., & Sims, K. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extensions of a dual-coding theory of multimedia learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 309–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mousavi, S., Lowe, R., & Sweller, J. (1995). Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes.Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 319–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narayanan, N.H., & Hegarty, M. (1998). On designing comprehensible interactive hypermedia manuals.International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 48, 267–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narayanan, N.H., & Hegarty, M. (2000). Communicating dynamic Behavior. Are interactive multimedia presentation better than static mixed-mode presentations? In M. Anderson, P. Cheng, & V. Haarslev (Eds.),Theory and applications of diagrams (pp. 178–193). Diagrams 2000, LNAI, 1889. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Narayanan, N.H. (2002). Cognitive Principles of Comprehensible Multimedia.EARLI-SIG, “Comprehension of Verbal and Pictorial Information”. Poitiers, August, 29.

  • Palmiter, S., & Elkerton, J. (1993). Animated demonstrations for learning procedural computer-based tasks.Human-Computer Interaction, 8, 193–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmiter, S., Elkerton, J., & Baggett, P. (1991). Animated demonstrations versus written instructions for learning procedural tasks: a preliminary investigation.International Journal of man machine studies, 34, 687–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pane, J.F., Corbett, A.T., & John, B.E. (1996). Assessing dynamics in computer-based instructions. InProceedings of the International Conference in computer-based human interaction (pp. 115–151), CHI’96. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ploetzner, R., & Lowe, R.K. (2004). Dynamic visualization and learning. Guest Editorial of the special Issue ofLearning and Instruction, 14, 235–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L. (1989). The effects of computer animated elaboration strategies and practice on factual and application learning in an elementary science lesson.Journal of Educational Computing Research, 5, 431–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L. (1990). Using computer animated graphics in science instructions with children.Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 135–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L. (1991). Animation, incidental learning and continuing motivation.Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 318–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rouet, J.F., Merlet, S., Ros, C., Richard, E., & Michaux, C. (2002). Effects of animated illustrations on the comprehension of an expository text. Poster Session ofEARLI-SIG, “Comprehension of Verbal and Pictorial Information”. Poitiers, August, 29.

  • Salomon, G. (1979).Interaction of Media, Cognition and learning: An exploration of how symbolic forms cultivate mental skills and affect knowledge acquisitions. San Francisco: Jossey-Blass Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G. (1981).Communication and Education: Social and Psychological Interactions. Berverly-Hills, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, E., Boucheix, J.M., & Lauw, S. (2003). A quelles conditions des animations peuvent-elles favoriser la l’intégration cognitive d’un système complexe?Neuvièmes Journées JETCSIC, 21 juin, Dijon.

  • Schnotz, W., & Grzondziel, H. (1999). Individual and co-operative learning with interactive animated pictures.European Journal of Psychology of Education, XIV(2), 245–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W., Bannert, M., & Seufert, T. (2002). Toward an integrative view of text and picture comprehension: Visualization effects on the construction of mental models. In J. Otéro, J.A. Léon, & A.C. Graesser (Eds.),The Psychology of Science Text Comprehension (pp. 385–416). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shah, P., Mayer, R.E., & Hegarty, M. (1999). Graphs as Aids to Knowledge Construction: Signaling Techniques for Guiding the Process of Graph Comprehension.Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 690–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W., & Lowe, R.K. (2003). External and internal representations in multimedia learning.Learning and Instruction, 13, 117–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwan, S., & Riempp, R. (2004). The cognitive benefits of interactive videos: Learning to tie nautical knots.Learning and Instruction, 14, 293–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, P., & Myaké, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach.Journal of experimental Psychology, 125, 4–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabbers, H. (2003).The modality of texts in multimedia instructions. Educational Technology Expertise Center. Open University of the Netherland, Heerlen.

  • Tabbers, H., Paas, F., Martens, R., & Van Merriënboer, J. (2002). Studying eye movement in multimedia learning.EARLI-SIG, “Comprehension of Verbal and Pictorial Information”. Poitiers, August, 29.

  • Tassini, S., & Bétrancourt, M. (2003). Le contrôle sur l’animation influence-t-elle le niveau d’efficacité cognitive de l’animation.Neuvièmes Journées JETCSIC, 21 juin, Dijon.

  • Thurstone L.L., & Thustone T.G. (1964).Batterie Factorielle P.M.A. (Aptitude Mentales Primaires, 11–17 ans). Paris: ECPA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., Bauer-Morrison, J., & Bétrancourt, M. (2002). Animation: Can it facilitate?International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 57, 247–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, P., Milroy, R., & Lickorish, A. (1999). Static and animated graphics in learning from interactive texts.European Journal of Psychology of Education, XIV(2), 203–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zacks, J.M., & Tversky, B. (2003). Structuring Information Interfaces for Procedural Learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 88–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jean-Michel Boucheix.

Additional information

This cognitive ergonomic work was supported by a collaboration between the University Institute for teacher, training of Burgundy at Dijon (IUFM), the LEAD-CNRS, and a project called “Makings of multimedia” whose partners (Town council of Dijon, Academic regional education authority, Hewlett-Packard and Microsoft) had the general goal to enhance “the daily use of computer in the classroom”).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Boucheix, JM., Guignard, H. What animated illustrations conditions can improve technical document comprehension in young students? Format, signaling and control of the presentation. Eur J Psychol Educ 20, 369–388 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173563

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173563

Key words

Navigation