Skip to main content
Log in

Making IBM’s computer, Watson, human

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This essay uses the recent victory of an IBM computer (Watson) in the TV game, Jeopardy, to speculate on the abilities Watson would need, in addition to those it has, to be human. The essay’s basic premise is that to be human is to behave as humans behave and to function in society as humans function. Alternatives to this premise are considered and rejected. The viewpoint of the essay is that of teleological behaviorism. Mental states are defined as temporally extended patterns of overt behavior. From this viewpoint (although Watson does not currently have them), essential human attributes such as consciousness, the ability to love, to feel pain, to sense, to perceive, and to imagine may all be possessed by a computer. Most crucially, a computer may possess self-control and may act altruistically. However, the computer’s appearance, its ability to make specific movements, its possession of particular internal structures (e.g., whether those structures are organic or inorganic), and the presence of any nonmaterial “self,” are all incidental to its humanity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ainslie, G. (1992). Picoeconomics: The strategic interaction of successive motivational states within the person. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (2005). Understanding behaviorism: Science, behavior and culture. New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1981). Psychologism and behaviorism. Philosophical Review, 90, 5–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1978). Why you can’t make a computer that feels pain. Synthese, 38, 415–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grunow, A., & Neuringer, A. (2002). Learning to vary and varying to learn. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 250–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locey, M. L., & Rachlin, H. (2011). A behavioral analysis of altruism. Behavioural Processes, 87, 25–33. NIHMS 260711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Madden, G. J., & Bickel, W. K. (Eds.). (2009). Impulsivity: Theory, science, and neuroscience of discounting. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, M. (2011). Watson looks for work. Scientific American, 304, 19.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? The Philosophical Review, 83, 435–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noë, A. (2009). Out of our heads: Why you are not your brain, and other lessons from the biology of consciousness. New York: Hill and Wang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parfit, D. (1984). Reasons and persons. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (1985). Pain and behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 43–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (1994). Behavior and mind: The roots of modern psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (1995). The value of temporal patterns in behavior. Current Directions, 4, 188–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (2000). The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (2002). Altruism and selfishness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 239–296.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S. (2011). The tell-tale brain: A neuroscientist’s quest for what makes us human. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1980). Minds, brains and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 417–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. (1995). Interpersonal allocation continuous with intertemporal allocation. Rationality and Society, 7, 367–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R., & Simmelhag, V. L. (1971). The “superstition” experiment: A reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review, 78, 3–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G., & Edelman, G. M. (1998). Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282, 1846–1851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2008). Evolution “for the good of the group.” American Scientist, 96, 380–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Howard Rachlin.

Additional information

Preparation of this article was supported by Grant DA02652021 from the National Institute of Drug Abuse. The content is solely the responsibility of the author and does not necessarily represent the views of the National Institutes of Health.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rachlin, H. Making IBM’s computer, Watson, human. BEHAV ANALYST 35, 1–16 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392260

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392260

Key words

Navigation