Skip to main content
Log in

On rhythmic and discrete movements: reflections, definitions and implications for motor control

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

At present, rhythmic and discrete movements are investigated by largely distinct research communities using different experimental paradigms and theoretical constructs. As these two classes of movements are tightly interlinked in everyday behavior, a common theoretical foundation spanning across these two types of movements would be valuable. Furthermore, it has been argued that these two movement types may constitute primitives for more complex behavior. The goal of this paper is to develop a rigorous taxonomic foundation that not only permits better communication between different research communities, but also helps in defining movement types in experimental design and thereby clarifies fundamental questions about primitives in motor control. We propose formal definitions for discrete and rhythmic movements, analyze some of their variants, and discuss the application of a smoothness measure to both types that enables quantification of discreteness and rhythmicity. Central to the definition of discrete movement is their separation by postures. Based on this intuitive definition, certain variants of rhythmic movement are indistinguishable from a sequence of discrete movements, reflecting an ongoing debate in the motor neuroscience literature. Conversely, there exist rhythmic movements that cannot be composed of a sequence of discrete movements. As such, this taxonomy may provide a language for studying more complex behaviors in a principled fashion.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This notation means that both t i and t j are contained within the interval between t p  − δ p and t p .

  2. A familiar example is the tangent to a curve, a line which touches the curve at a single point with the same slope as the curve at that point. Of course, the slope of a point is meaningless but the slope of a curve at a point may be defined by considering a line through two suitably close points and noting the limiting value to which its slope converges as the separation of the points diminishes. This avoids the “divide-by-zero” problem which would otherwise confound attempts at precision.

  3. Subjects gave informed consent as approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology and the Pennsylvania State University.

  4. It should be noted that because a strictly periodic function has infinite duration, periodicity can never be proven conclusively from experimental observation, and certainly in biology deviations from strict periodicity should be anticipated.

  5. A function y(t) is continuous if at every point a in its domain, there exists a constant δ > 0 corresponding to every constant ɛ > 0 such that |y(t) − y(a)| < ɛ for all t in the neighborhood |ta| < δ. To be continuous at a specific point a the function must be defined at ta and \({{\mathop {\lim}\limits_{t \to a}}\;y{\left(t \right)} = y{\left(a \right)};}\) a continuous function is continuous at all points in its domain.

  6. This is similar to the definition of familiar concepts such as the shortest line. The shortest line has zero length (a trivial answer), so we need boundary conditions, i.e., two points. The shortest line between two points may be found by defining a measure of length, which assigns a scalar to each possible path. This scalar is defined by adding all of the infinitesimal displacements along the path. Variational calculus may then be applied to find the path with minimal length (a straight line in Euclidean space, a Great Circle on the surface of a sphere and so on). Note that length, like smoothness, is not a categorical distinction but a matter of degree.

References

  • Adam JJ, Paas FGWC (1996) Dwell time in reciprocal aiming tasks. Hum Mov Sci 15:1–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adamovich SV, Levin MF, Feldman AG (1994) Merging different motor patterns: coordination between rhythmical and discrete single-joint movements. Exp Brain Res 99(2):325–337

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Amazeen PG, Amazeen EL, Turvey MT (1998) Dynamics of human intersegmental coordination: theory and research. In: Rosenbaum DA, Collyer CE (eds) Timing of behavior: neural, computational, and psychological perspectives. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 237–259

    Google Scholar 

  • Bizzi E, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Giszter S (1991) Computations underlying the execution of movement: a biological perspective. Science 253:287–291

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Borowski EJ, Borwein JM (1991) The Harper-Collins dictionary of mathematics. Harper-Collins Publishers

  • Buchanan JJ, Park JH, Ryu YU, Shea CH (2003) Discrete and cyclical units of action in a mixed target pair aiming task. Exp Brain Res 150(4):473–489

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Park JH, Shea CH (2004) Systematic scaling of target width: dynamics, planning, and feedback. Neurosci Lett 367(3):317–322

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Park JH, Shea CH (2006) Target width scaling in a repetitive aiming task: switching between cyclical and discrete units of action. Exp Brain Res 175(4):710–25

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casdagli MC (1997) Recurrence plots revisited. Physica D 108(1–2):12–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • d’Avella A, Bizzi E (2005) Shared and specific muscle synergies in natural motor behaviors. Proc Natl Acad Sci 102(8):3076–3081

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dean WJ (2001) Rhythmical and discrete movements patterns in the upper extremity. Pennsylvania State University, University Park

    Google Scholar 

  • Delcomyn F (1980) Neural basis of rhythmic behavior in animals. Science 210:492–498

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP, Cavanagh PR, Sternad D (2001) Local dynamic stability versus kinematic variability of continuous overground and treadmill walking. J Biomed Eng 123(1):27–32

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dipietro L, Krebs HI, Volpe BT, Hogan N (2004) Combinations of elementary units underlying human arm movements at different speeds. Abstr Soc Neurosci 872

  • Doeringer JA, Hogan N (1998) Intermittency in preplanned elbow movements persists in the absence of visual feedback. J Neurophysiol 80:1787–1799

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eckmann JP, Olifsson, Kamphorst S, Ruelle D (1987) Recurrence plots of dynamical systems. Europhys Lett 4:973–977

    Google Scholar 

  • Elble RJ, Higgins C and Hughes L (1994) Essential tremor entrains rapid voluntary movements. Exp Neurol 126:138–143

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flash T, Hogan N (1985) The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. J Neurosci 5(7):1688–1703

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flash T, Hogan N, Richardson MJE (2003) Optimization principles in motor control. In: Arbib MA (ed) The handbook of brain theory and neural networks, 2nd edn. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 827–831

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse P (1963) The psychology of time. Harper and Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Giszter SF, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Bizzi E (1993) Convergent force fields organized in the frog’s spinal cord. J Neurosci 13(2):467–491

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano MSA, Taylor CSR, Moore T (2002) Complex movements evoked by microstimulation of precentral cortex. Neuron 34:841–851

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grillner S (1975) Locomotion in vertebrates: central mechanisms and reflex interaction. Physiol Rev 55:247–304

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guiard Y (1993) On Fitts and Hooke’s law: simple harmonic movements in upper-limb cyclical aiming. Acta Psychol 82:139–159

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guiard Y (1997) Fitt’s law in the discrete vs. cyclical paradigm. Hum Mov Sci 16:97–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haken H, Kelso JAS, Bunz H (1985) A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biol Cybern 51:347–356

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hart CB, Giszter SF (2004) Modular premotor drives and unit bursts as primitives for frog motor behaviors. J Neurosci 24(22):5269–5282

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hausdorff JM, Ashkenazy Y, Peng CK, Ivanov PC, Stanley HE, Goldberger A (2001) When human walking becomes random walking: fractal analysis and modeling of gait rhythm fluctuations. Physica A 302(1–4):138–147

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hogan N (1982) Control and coordination of voluntary arm movement. Paper presented at the IEEE American Control Conference 2:522–527.

  • Hogan N (1984) An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements. J Neurosci 4(11):2745–2754

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hollerbach JM (1981) An oscillation theory of handwriting. Biol Cybern 39:139–156

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ivry RB, Spencer RM (2004) The neural representation of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:225–232

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ivry RB, Spencer RM, Zelaznik HN, Diedrichsen J (2002) The cerebellum and event timing. Ann N Y Acad Sci 978:302–317

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jirsa V, Kelso JAS (2005) The excitator as a minimal model for the coordination dynamics of discrete and rhythmic movement generation. J Mot Behav 37(1):35–51

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kantz H, Schreiber T (1997) Nonlinear time series analysis. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs HI, Aisen ML, Volpe BT, Hogan N (1999) Quantization of continuous arm movements in humans with brain injury. Proc Natl Acad Sci 96(8):4645–4649

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kurtzer IL, Herter TM, Scott SH (2006) Nonuniform distribution of reach-related and torque-related activity in upper arm muscles and neurons of primary motor cortex. J Neurophysiol 96(6):3220–3230

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML (2005) Postural synergies and their development. Neural Plast 12(2–3):119–139

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis PA, Miall RC (2003) Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: evidence from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:250–255

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marder E, Calabrese RL (1996) Principles of rhythmic motor pattern generation. Physiol Rev 76(3):687–717

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mink JW, Thach WT (1991) Basal ganglia motor control. I: Nonexclusive relation of pallidal discharge to five movement modes. J Neurophysiol 65(2):273–300

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van Mourik A, Beek PJ (2004) Discrete and cyclical movements: unified dynamics or separate control? Acta Psychol 117(2):121–138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagasaki H (1991) Asymmetrical trajectory formation in cyclic forearm movements in man. Exp Brain Res 87:653–661

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Naselaris T, Merchant H, Amirikian B, Georgopoulos AP (2006a) Large-scale organization of preferred directions in the motor cortex I: motor cortical hyperacuity for forward reaching. J Neurophysiol 96(6):3231–36

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naselaris T, Merchant H, Amirikian B, Georgopoulos AP (2006b) Large-scale organization of preferred directions in the motor cortex II: analysis of local distributions. J Neurophysiol 96(6):3237–3247

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson W (1983) Physical principles for economies of skilled movements. Biol Cybern 46:135–147

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Prablanc C, Desmurget M, Grea H (2003) Neural control of on-line guidance of hand reaching movements. Prog Brain Res 142:155–170

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson MJE, Flash T (2002) Comparing smooth arm movement with the two-thirds power law and the related segmented-control hypothesis. J Neurosci 22(18):8201–8211

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • de Rugy A, Sternad D (2003) Interaction between discrete and rhythmic movements: reaction time and phase of discrete movement initiation against oscillatory movements. Brain Res 994:160–174

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • de Rugy A, Taga G, Montagne G, Buekers MJ, Laurent M (2002) Perception–action coupling model for human locomotor pointing. Biol Cybern 87(2):141–150

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schaal S, Sternad D, Osu R, Kawato M (2004) Rhythmic arm movement is not discrete. Nat Neurosci 7(10):1136–1143

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA, Lee TD (2005) Motor control and learning: a behavioral emphasis, 4th edn. Human Kinetics

  • Schöner G (1990) A dynamic theory of coordination of discrete movements. Biol Cybern 63:257–270

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shadmehr R, Wise SP (2005) Computational neurobiology of reaching and pointing: a foundation for motor learning. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Smits-Engelman BCM, Van Galen GP, Duysens J (2002) The breakdown of Fitts’ law in rapid, reciprocal aiming movements. Exp Brain Res 145:222–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer RM, Zelaznik HN, Diedrichsen J, Ivry RB (2003) Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science 300:1437–1439

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D (2007) Towards a unified framework for rhythmic and discrete movements Ð behavioral, modeling and imaging results. In: Fuchs A, Jirsa V (eds) Coordination: Neural, behavioral and social dynamics. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Dean WJ (2003) Rhythmic and discrete elements in multijoint coordination. Brain Res 989:151–172

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Turvey MT, Schmidt RC (1992) Average phase difference theory and 1:1 phase entrainment in interlimb coordination. Biol Cybern 67:223–231

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Saltzman EL, Turvey MT (1998) Interlimb coordination in a simple serial behavior: a task dynamic approach. Hum Mov Sci 17:393–433

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Turvey MT, Saltzman EL (1999) Dynamics of 1:2 coordination in rhythmic interlimb movement: I. Generalizing relative phase. J Mot Behav 31(3):207–223

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Dean WJ, Schaal S (2000a) Interaction of rhythmic and discrete pattern generators in single-joint movements. Hum Mov Sci 19:627–665

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Duarte M, Katsumata H, Schaal S (2000b) Dynamics of a bouncing ball in human performance. Phys Rev E 63:011902–011901–011902–011908

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Duarte M, Katsumata H, Schaal S (2001) Bouncing a ball: tuning into dynamic stability. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27(5):1163–1184

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, de Rugy A, Pataky T, Dean WJ (2002) Interactions of discrete and rhythmic movements over a wide range of periods. Exp Brain Res 147:162–174

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Wei K, Diedrichsen J, Ivry RB (2007) Intermanual interactions during initiation and production of rhythmic and discrete movements in individuals lacking a corpus callosum. Exp Brain Res 176(4):559–574

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strogatz SH (1994) Nonlinear dynamics and chaos. Addison–Wesley, Reading, USA

    Google Scholar 

  • Teeken JC, Adam JJ, Paas FGWC, van Boxtel MP, Houx PJ, Jolles J (1996) Effects of age and gender on discrete and reciprocal aiming movements. Psychol Aging 11(2):195–198

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ting LH, Macpherson JM (2005) A limited set of muscle synergies for force control during a postural task. J Neurophysiol 93(1):609–613

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vindras P, Desmurget M, Viviani P (2005) Error parsing in visuomotor pointing reveals independent processing of amplitude and direction. J Neurophysiol 94(2):1212–1224

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wei K, Wertman G, Sternad D (2003) Discrete and rhythmic components in bimanual actions. Motor Control 7(2):134–155

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wierzbicka MM, Staude G, Wolf W, Dengler R (1993) Relationship between tremor and the onset of rapid voluntary contraction in Parkinson disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 56:782–787

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wing AM, Kristofferson AB (1973) The timing of interresponse intervals. Percept Psychophys 13(3):455–460

    Google Scholar 

  • Winstein CJ, Pohl PS (1995) Effects of unilateral brain damage on the control of goal-directed hand movements. Exp Brain Res 105(1):163–174

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Winter DA (1990) Biomechanics and motor control of human movement. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Yu H, Sternad D, Corcos DM, Vaillancourt DE (2007) Role of hyperactive cerebellum and motor cortex in Parkinson’s Disease. NeuroImage 35:222–233

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zadeh LA (1965) Fuzzy sets. Inf Control 8:338–353

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation, BCS-0096543 and PAC-0450218, and the National Institutes of Health R01HD045639, awarded to Dagmar Sternad. Neville Hogan was supported by a grant from the New York State Spinal Cord Injury Center of Research Excellence. We would like to thank Robert Sainburg for helpful discussions of an earlier version of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dagmar Sternad.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Definition of recurrent

To precisely define “approximately equal” and “significantly different”, consider a particular value in the range of the function and denote the corresponding argument by t i . Let t i+ denote the closest larger value of the argument at which the function differs by a small constant ε from that value,

$$t_{i+}=t_{i}+\delta_{i+}$$

where δ i+ is the maximum positive constant such that

$$\Big|y(t)-y(t_{i})\Big| < \varepsilon \text{ for all }t\text{ in the interval }t_{i}\leq t\leq t_{i}+\delta_{i+}$$

and let t i denote the closest smaller value,

$$t_{i-}=t_{i}-\delta_{i-}$$

where δ i is the maximum positive constant such that

$$\Big|y(t)-y(t_{i})\Big| < \varepsilon \text{ for all }t\text{ in the interval }t_{i}-\delta_{i-}\leq t\leq t_{i}.$$

The function is recurrent if there exists a set of arguments {t j } for which

$$\Big|y(t_{i})-y(t_{j})\Big| < \varepsilon \text{ and }t_{j} < t_{i-}\text{ or }t_{j} > t_{i+}.$$

This might be termed minimally recurrent as it may be satisfied by as few as two values of the argument. To precisely define “a large number” of recurrences, we require the number to grow without bound if the observation interval grows without bound. For any candidate t j identified above, find t j+ and t j as above to identify the interval in which the function remains approximately equal and assign all t in the interval t jt ≤ t j+ as one occurrence. Let N denote the number of members of the set {t j } and D denote the interval of observation, 0 ≤ t ≤ D. Then the function is indefinitely recurrent if \({{\mathop {\lim}\limits_{D \to \infty}}N = \infty.}\) White noise is a theoretical extreme case which might be termed infinitely recurrent; in a finite interval of observation, all values in the amplitude distribution recur an infinite number of times with probability approaching unity.

Appendix 2: Smoothest discrete and cyclic movements

Using mean-squared-jerk as a measure, the problem of identifying the smoothest movement may be formulated using optimization theory as that of finding the function y(t) that minimizes the scalar

$${msj = \frac{1}{{t_{2} - t_{1}}}{\int\limits_{t_{1}}^{t_{2}} {\frac{1}{2}u^{2} {\rm d}t}}}$$

subject to the dynamic constraints

$${\dot{y} = v, \dot{v} = a, \dot{a} = u}$$

where y, v, a and u are position, velocity, acceleration and jerk, respectively. Using the method of Lagrange multipliers, the constraints may be added to the scalar to be minimized as

$${msj = \frac{1}{{t_{2} - t_{1}}}{\int\limits_{t_{1}}^{t_{2}} {{\left\{{\frac{1}{2}u^{2} + \lambda_{y} {\left({v - \dot{y}} \right)} + \lambda_{v} {\left({a - \dot{v}} \right)} + \lambda_{a} {\left({u - \dot{a}} \right)}} \right\}}{\rm d}t}}}$$

where λ y , λ v and λ a are functions to be determined. For convenience, form the Hamiltonian

$${H = \frac{1}{2}u^{2} + \lambda_{y} v + \lambda_{v} a + \lambda_{a} u}$$

so that

$${msj = \frac{1}{{t_{2} - t_{1}}}{\int\limits_{t_{1}}^{t_{2}} {{\left({H - \lambda_{y} \dot{y} - \lambda_{v} \dot{v} - \lambda_{a} \dot{a}} \right)}{\rm d}t}}}.$$

For simplicity, assume a time scale such that t 1= 0 and t 2 = 1. Application of variational calculus shows that within the interval 0 ≤ t ≤ 1 the partial derivative of H with respect to each of its arguments must be zero. The dynamic constraint equations (or state equations) are recovered from

$${\left. {{\partial H} \mathord{\left/ {\vphantom {{\partial H} {\partial \lambda_{y}}}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} {\partial \lambda_{y}}} \right|_{o} = v = \dot{y}, \left. {{\partial H} \mathord{\left/ {\vphantom {{\partial H} {\partial \lambda_{v}}}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} {\partial \lambda_{v}}} \right|_{o} = a = \dot{v}}\text{ and }{\left. {{\partial H} \mathord{\left/{\vphantom {{\partial H} {\partial \lambda_{a}}}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} {\partial \lambda_{a}}} \right|_{o} = u = \dot{a}.}$$

The Lagrange multipliers are determined by the co-state equations

$${\left. {{\partial H} \mathord{\left/ {\vphantom {{\partial H} {\partial y}}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} {\partial y}} \right|_{o} = 0 = - \dot{\lambda}_{y}, \left. {{\partial H} \mathord{\left/ {\vphantom {{\partial H} {\partial v}}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} {\partial v}} \right|_{o} = \lambda_{y} = - \dot{\lambda}_{v} }\text{ and }{\left. {{\partial H} \mathord{\left/ {\vphantom {{\partial H} {\partial a}}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} {\partial a}} \right|_{o} = \lambda_{v} = - \dot{\lambda}_{a}.}$$

The optimal solution is defined by

$${\left. {{\partial H} \mathord{\left/ {\vphantom {{\partial H} {\partial u}}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} {\partial u}} \right|_{o} = 0 = u^{o} + \lambda_{a}.}$$

Combining equations, −λ a is jerk, λ v is snap (the fourth time derivative of position), −λ y is crackle (the fifth time derivative of position) and the minimal mean-squared jerk movement is defined by

$${{{\rm d}^{6} y} \mathord{\left/ {\vphantom {{{\rm d}^{6} y} {{\rm d}t^{6}=0}}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} {{\rm d}t^{6} = 0}.}$$

Integrating yields a fifth-order polynomial

$$y^{0}(t)=a_{0}+a_{1}t+a_{2}t^{2}+a_{3}t^{3}+a_{4}t^{4}+a_{5}t^{5}$$

with six coefficients to be determined by the boundary conditions at the ends of the time interval. A discrete movement starting from rest at y(0) = 0 and ending at rest at y(1) = 1 yields

$$a_{0}=a_{1}=a_{2}=0,\;a_{3}=10,\;a_{4}=-15\;\text{and }a_{5}=6$$

so that the general solution is

$${y^{0} {\left(t \right)} = y{\left(0 \right)} + A{\left[ {10{\left({\frac{t}{d}} \right)}^{3} - 15{\left({\frac{t}{d}} \right)}^{4} + 6{\left({\frac{t}{d}} \right)}^{5}} \right]}}$$

for 0 ≤ t ≤ 1 where y(0) is initial position, A is movement amplitude and d is movement duration. The minimal value of the msj measure for this movement is

$${msj^{0} = 360{A^{2}} \mathord{\left/ {\vphantom {{A^{2}} {d^{6}}}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} {d^{6}}.}$$

To identify the smoothest cyclic movement between two positions, consider two adjacent intervals, i.e., a “forth” movement from the first to the second position in the interval 0 ≤ t ≤ m, and a “back” movement from the second to the first position in the interval mt ≤ p, where p is the period of the cycle and 0 < mp is the passage time (to be determined) at which the second position, y 2, is passed. The details of movement between these positions may be found by solving an optimization problem with an “interior-point constraint” N on position at the passage time m such that

$$N=y(m)-y_{2}=0.$$

Again using the method of Lagrange multipliers, this constraint may be appended to the measure to be minimized to form

$${msj_{c} = \pi N + \frac{1}{p}{\int\limits_0^p {\frac{1}{2}u^{2} {\rm d}t}}.}$$

The integral has two components, the first with a variable end time, the second with a variable start time,

$${msj_{c} = \pi N + \frac{1}{p}{\left[ {{\int\limits_0^m {\frac{1}{2}u^{2} {\rm d}t}} + {\int\limits_m^p {\frac{1}{2}u^{2} {\rm d}t}}} \right]}.}$$

Applying variational calculus, within each interval the movement is described by a quintic polynomial as above. Thirteen coefficients (six for each interval plus the passage time, m) have to be determined from the boundary conditions at the end of each interval. For simplicity, assume p = 2. The position, velocity and acceleration (indeed, all derivatives) at the beginning of the “forth” interval and end of the “back” interval are identical:

$$y(2)=y(0)=0,\;v(2)=v(0),\;a(2)=a(0)$$

and so on; this is a basic requirement for a cyclic movement. At the passage time, position is known but none of its time derivatives are. In general, the Hamiltonian and each of the Lagrange multipliers λ y , λ v and λ a may be discontinuous at time m. Using self-evident subscripts for the first and second intervals, a general boundary condition at time m is

$$ {\rm d}y{\left({- \lambda_{{y1}} + \pi + \lambda_{{y2}}} \right)} + {\rm d}v{\left({- \lambda_{{v1}} + \lambda_{{v2}}} \right)} + {\rm d}a{\left({- \lambda_{{a1}} + \lambda_{{a2}}} \right)} + {\rm d}m{\left({H_{1} - H_{2}} \right)} = 0. $$

As each of the infinitesimal differentials dy, dv, da and dm are unspecified, this reduces to

$${H_{1}=H_{2},\; \lambda_{a_1}=\lambda_{a_2},\; \lambda_{v_1}=\lambda_{v_2},\;}\text{ and }{\lambda_{y_1}=\pi + \lambda_{y_2}.}$$

Thus crackle may be discontinuous at the passage time but the Hamiltonian, jerk and snap are continuous and (by integration) acceleration, velocity and position are also continuous. If crackle is discontinuous and the Hamiltonian is continuous then velocity must be zero at the passage time.

While this analysis yields sufficient equations to determine the unknown coefficients, evaluating them requires solving a large number of simultaneous, nonlinear, algebraic equations. A simpler approach is to assume that the “forth” and “back” movements have the same (unknown) shape, though perhaps different durations. If so, the jerk measure for each interval is proportional to the square of movement amplitude and inversely proportional to the sixth power of interval duration so that

$${msj_{c} = \frac{1}{p}{\left[ {\frac{{CA^{2}}}{{m^{5}}} + \frac{{CA^{2}}}{{{\left({p - m} \right)}^{5}}}} \right]},}$$

where C is a constant that depends on the details of the shape. Setting p = 2 and minimizing with respect to the passage time, m, yields a sixth-order polynomial with only one real-valued root at m = 1. Thus the smoothest cyclic movement has equal-duration “forth” and “back” segments, each a mirror-image of the other. Boundary conditions for the “forth” movement are

$$y(0)=0,\;y(1)=1,\;v(0)=-v(1),\;a(0)=-a(1),\;u(0)=-u(1)\text{ and }s(0)=-s(1),$$

where s is snap. They yield

$${a_{0}=a_{1}=0,\; a_{2}=5 \mathord{\left/{\vphantom {5 2}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} 2,\; a_{3}=0,\; a_{4}=-5 \mathord{\left/{\vphantom {5 2}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} 2}\text{ and }a_{5}=1$$

so that the general solution is

$${y^{0} {\left(t \right)} = y{\left(0 \right)} + A{\left[ {\frac{5}{2}{\left({{\left({\frac{t}{d}} \right)}^{2} - {\left({\frac{t}{d}} \right)}^{4}} \right)} + {\left({\frac{t}{d}} \right)}^{5}} \right]}}$$

for 0 ≤ t ≤ d where y(0) is initial position, A is movement amplitude and d is the duration of the outbound movement, so that p = 2d. The minimal value of the msj measure for this movement is

$${msj^{0} = 60{A^{2}} \mathord{\left/ {\vphantom {{A^{2}} {d^{6}}}} \right. \kern-\nulldelimiterspace} {d^{6}}.}$$

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hogan, N., Sternad, D. On rhythmic and discrete movements: reflections, definitions and implications for motor control. Exp Brain Res 181, 13–30 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-0899-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-0899-y

Keywords

Navigation