Skip to main content
Log in

The continuous end-state comfort effect: weighted integration of multiple biases

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The grasp orientation when grasping an object is frequently aligned in anticipation of the intended rotation of the object (end-state comfort effect). We analyzed grasp orientation selection in a continuous task to determine the mechanisms underlying the end-state comfort effect. Participants had to grasp a box by a circular handle—which allowed for arbitrary grasp orientations—and then had to rotate the box by various angles. Experiments 1 and 2 revealed both that the rotation’s direction considerably determined grasp orientations and that end-postures varied considerably. Experiments 3 and 4 further showed that visual stimuli and initial arm postures biased grasp orientations if the intended rotation could be easily achieved. The data show that end-state comfort but also other factors determine grasp orientation selection. A simple mechanism that integrates multiple weighted biases can account for the data.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. With the term “anticipatory grasp orientations”, we refer to grasp orientations that have an anticipatory component.

  2. The rotation angle can assume both positive and negative values

  3. The effect of the context is unlikely to be cancelled out by the different ratios of women and men participating in Experiments 1 and 2 (c.f. Fischman 1998), because similar results are obtained if only women are included in the analysis (for all interactions p > 0.23).

  4. Grasp selection may be biased by optical illusions (Crajé et al. 2008). However, here we did not want to create an illusion that distorted the perceived orientation of the object that was to be grasped, but wanted to suggest a certain way of grasping and turning the box to the participants.

  5. Note that we only included the arrow start position left and right because they are the conditions that can be interpreted most easily.

  6. We thank an anonymous reviewer for suggesting this explanation.

  7. We used the matlab function fminsearchbnd (http://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/8277-fminsearchbnd) to determine values that result in the smallest root mean square error for each participant. We constrained the values of p anti,cw, p anti,ccw, and p const to [−200°,200°]. As each participant provided only three data points in Experiment 2, the model, which has four free parameters in the case of Experiment 2, fits the data trivially (R 2 = 1.0).

References

  • Adalbjornsson, C. F., Fischman, M. G., & Rudisill, M. E. (2008). The end-state comfort effect in young children. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79, 36–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bubic, A., von Cramon, D. Y., & Schubotz, R. I. (2010). Prediction, cognition and the brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butz, M. V., Herbort, O., & Hoffmann, J. (2007). Exploiting redundancy for flexible behavior: Unsupervised learning in a modular sensorimotor control architecture. Psychological Review, 114(4), 1015–1046.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, K. M., Weiss, D. J., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2010). Evolutionary roots of motor planning: The end-state comfort effect in lemurs. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 124(2), 229–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cisek, P. (2006). Integrated neural processes for defining potential actions and deciding between them: A computational model. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(38), 9761–9770.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. G., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2004). Where grasps are made reveals how grasps are planned: Generation and recall of motor plans. Experimental Brain Research, 157, 486–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coren, S. (1993). The lateral preference inventory for measurement of handedness, footedness, eyedness, and earedness: Norms for young adults. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31(1), 1–3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crajé, C., van der Kamp, J., & Steenbergen, B. (2008). The effect of the “rod-and-frame” illusion on grip planning in a sequential object manipulation task. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 53–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engelbrecht, S. E. (2001). Minimum principles in motor control. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 45, 497–542.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erlhagen, W., & Schöner, G. (2002). Dynamic field theory of movement preparation. Psychological Review, 109(3), 545–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., Rosenbaum, D. A., & Vaughan, J. (1997). Speed and sequential effects in reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(2), 404–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischman, M. G. (1998). Constraints on grip-selection: Minimizing awkwardness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 328–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flash, T., & Hogan, N. (1985). The coordination of arm movements: An experimentally confirmed mathematical model. The Journal of Neuroscience, 5(7), 1688–1703.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haggard, P. (1998). Planning of action sequences. Acta Psychologica, 99(2), 201–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. M., & Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature, 394, 780–784.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herbort, O., & Butz, M. V. (2007). Encoding complete body models enables task dependent optimal control. Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Neural Networks 20, 1639–1644.

  • Herbort, O., & Butz, M. (2010). Planning and control of hand orientation in grasping movements. Experimental Brain Research, 202(4), 867–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C. (1996). Brief report: Planning problems in autism at the level of motor control. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(1), 99–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. H. (2000). Thinking ahead: The case for motor imagery in prospective judgements of prehension. Cognition, 74(1), 33–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S., Buchanan, J. J., & Murata, T. (1994). Multifunctionality and switching in the coordination dynamics of reaching and grasping. Human Movement Science, 13(1), 63–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knill, D. C., & Pouget, A. (2004). The bayesian brain: The role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(12), 712–719. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marotta, J. J., Medendorp, W. P., & Crawford, J. D. (2003). Kinematic rules for upper and lower arm contributions to grasp orientation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90, 3816–3820.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuoka, J., Berger, R. A., Berglund, L. J., & An, K.-N. (2006). An analysis of symmetry of torque strength of the forearm under resisted forearm rotation in normal subjects. The Journal of Hand Surgery, 31(5), 801–805.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Sullivan, L., & Gallwey, T. (2005). Forearm torque strengths and discomfort profiles in pronation and supination. Ergonomics, 48(6), 703–721.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pezzulo, G., Butz, M. V., Gastelfranchi, C., & Falcone, R. (2008). The challenge of anticipation: A unifying framework for the analysis and design of artificial cognitive systems. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robert, T., Beurier, G., & Wang, X. (2009). Arm postural anticipation for rotating a spherical object. Computer Methods in Biomechanics and Biomedical Engineering, 12(1), 217–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Cohen, R. G., Jax, S. A., Weiss, D. J., & van der Wel, R. (2007). The problem of serial order in behavior: Lashley’s legacy. Human Movement Science, 26, 525–554.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Halloran, E. S., & Cohen, R. G. (2006). Grasping movement plans. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(5), 918–922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1992). Planning macroscopic aspects of manual control. Human Movement Science, 11(1–2), 61–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Loukopoulos, L. D., Meulenbroek, R. G. J., Vaughan, J., & Engelbrecht, S. E. (1995). Planning reaches by evaluating stored postures. Psychological Review, 102(1), 28–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Siotta, J. D., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1990). Constraints for action selection: Overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and Performance, Vol. XIII (pp. 321–345). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. G. J., Vaughan, J., & Jansen, C. (2001). Posture-based motion planning: Applications to grasping. Psychological Review, 108(4), 709–734.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., van Heugten, C. M., & Caldwell, G. E. (1996). From cognition to biomechanics and back: The end-state comfort effect and the middle-is-faster effect. Acta Psychologica, 94, 59–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Vaughan, J., Barnes, H. J., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1992). Time course of movement planning: Selection of handgrips for object manipulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(5), 1058–1073.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Vaughan, J., Jorgensen, M. J., Barnes, H. J., & Stewart, E. (1993). Plans for object manipulation. In D. E. Meyer & S. Kornblum (Ed.), Attention and performance XIV—a silver jubilee: Synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence and cognitive neuroscience (pp. 803–820). Cambridge: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

  • Schütz, C., Weigelt, M., Odekerken, D., Klein-Soetebier, & Schack, T. (in press). Motor control strategies in a continuous task space. Motor Control.

  • Short, M. W., & Cauraugh, J. H. (1999). Precision hypothesis and the end-state comfort effect. Acta Psychologica, 100(3), 243–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soechting, J. F., Buneo, C. A., Herrmann, U., & Flanders, M. (1995). Moving effortlessly in three dimensions: Does donders’ law apply to arm movement? Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 6271–6280.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steenbergen, B., Hulstijn, W., & Dortmans, S. (2000). Constraints on grip selection in cerebral palsy: Minimising discomfort. Experimental Brain Research, 134, 385–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thibaut, J.-P., & Toussaint, L. (2010). Developing motor planning over ages. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(1–2), 116–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todorov, E. (2004). Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nature Review Neuroscience, 7(9), 907–915.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Vaart, A. J. M., (1995). Arm movements in operating rotary control (Doctoral thesis, Technische Universiteit Delft, Delft, The Netherlands). http://repository.tudelft.nl/assets/uuid:20033739-71d9-42a4-bc3e-058d78ab8cec/ide_vaart_19950616.PDF.

  • van Swieten, L. M., van Bergen, E., Williams, J. H., Wilson, A. D., Plumb, M. S., Kent, S. W., et al. (2010). A test of motor (not executive) planning in developmental coordination disorder and autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(2), 493–499.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weigelt, M., Rosenbaum, D. A., Huelshorst, S., & Schack, T. (2009). Moving and memorizing: Motor planning modulates the recency effect in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica, 132(1), 68–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, D. J., Wark, J. D., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2007). Monkey see, monkey plan, monkey do: The end-state comfort effect in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Psychological Science, 18(12), 1063–1068.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert, D. M., & Ghahramani, Z. (2000). Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1212–1217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, W., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2008). Planning for manual positioning: The end-state comfort effect for manual abduction-adduction. Experimental Brain Research, 184, 383–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge funding from the Emmy Noether program of the German Research Foundation (grant BU1335/3-1), thank Georg Schüssler for technical support, and thank David Rosenbaum and John Buchanan for comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Oliver Herbort.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (PDF 71 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Herbort, O., Butz, M.V. The continuous end-state comfort effect: weighted integration of multiple biases. Psychological Research 76, 345–363 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0334-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0334-7

Keywords

Navigation