Skip to main content
Log in

Happy but still focused: failures to find evidence for a mood-induced widening of visual attention

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In models of affect and cognition, it is held that positive affect broadens the scope of attention. Consistent with this claim, previous research has indeed suggested that positive affect is associated with impaired selective attention as evidenced by increased interference of spatially distant distractors. However, several recent findings cast doubt on the reliability of this observation. In the present study, we examined whether selective attention in a visual flanker task is influenced by positive mood induction. Across three experiments, positive affect consistently failed to exert any impact on selective attention. The implications of this null-finding for theoretical models of affect and cognition are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We also used the outlier analyses of Rowe et al. (2007) in which response times >1,000 ms were considered as outliers. Results were comparable.

References

  • Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779–806.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Burton, C. M., & King, L. A. (2009). The health benefits of writing about positive experiences: The role of broadened cognition. Psychology and Health, 24, 867–879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chajut, E., & Algom, D. (2003). Selective attention improves under stress: Implications for theories of social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 231–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Compton, R. J., Wirtz, D., Pajoumand, G., Claus, E., & Heller, W. (2004). Association between positive affect and attentional shifting. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 733–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In P. M. Niedenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 167–196). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon identification of a targetletter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estrada, C. A., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1994). Positive affect improves creative problem solving and influences reported source of practice satisfaction in physicians. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 285–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estrada, C. A., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1997). Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring in reasoning among physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72, 117–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finucane, A. M., Whiteman, M. C., & Power, M. J. (2010). The effect of happiness and sadness on alerting, orienting, and executive attention. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13, 629–639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology—The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Braningan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313–332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19, 476–482.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The blues broaden but the nasty narrows: Attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychological Science, 21, 211–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K. (2004). Do you see what I see? Affect and visual information processing. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 405–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, J. D., Sedikides, C., Saltzberg, J. A., Wood, J. V., & Forzano, L. A. (2003). Happy mood decreases self-focused attention. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 147–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., & Gable, P. A. (2009). Neural activity underlying the effect of approach-motivated positive affect on narrowed attention. Psychological Science, 20, 406–409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huntsinger, J. R., Clore, G. L., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2010). Mood and global-local focus: Priming a local focus reverses the link between mood and global-local processing. Emotion, 10, 722–726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (2001). An influence of positive affect on decision making in complex situations: Theoretical issues with practical implications. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11, 75–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, P. D., McGinnis, D., Lovejoy, S., & Stirling, J. (2000). Valence and arousal ratings for Velten mood induction statements. Motivation and Emotion, 24, 285–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. J., Waugh, C. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). Smile to see the forest: Facially expressed positive emotions broaden cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 299–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, E. A., & Kerns, J. G. (2011). The influence of positive mood on different aspects of cognitive control. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 265–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merritt, P., Hirshman, E., Wharton, W., Stangl, B., Devlin, J., & Lenz, A. (2007). Evidence for gender differences in visual selective attention. Personality and individual differences, 43, 597–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moriya, J., & Tanno, Y. (2009). Dysfunction of attentional networks for non-emotional processing in negative affect. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1090–1105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nummenmaa, L., & Niemi, P. (2004). Inducing affective states with success–failure manipulations: A meta-analysis. Emotion, 4, 207–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olivers, C. N. L., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2005). The beneficial effects of concurrent task-irrelevant mental activity on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 16, 265–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P., Acosta, A., Callejas, A., & Lupianez, J. (2010). Attention and anxiety: Different attentional functioning under state and trait anxiety. Psychological Science, 21, 298–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rottenberg, J., Ray, R. D., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion elicitation using films. In J. A. Coan & J. J. B. Allen (Eds.), The handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. J. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 383–388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., Weis, A., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1989). Affect grid: a single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valdez, P., Ramirez, C., Garcia, A., Talamantes, J., Armijo, P., & Borrani, J. (2005). Circadian rhythms in components of attention. Biological rhythm research, 36, 57–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Wouwe, N. C., Band, G. P. H., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2011). Positive affect modulates flexibility and evaluative control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 524–539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., & Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 557–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., & Goldsamt, L. A. (1990). Does affect induce self-focused attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 899–908.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this paper was partially supported by Grant BOF10/GOA/014 for a Concerted Research Action of Ghent University (awarded to Rudi De Raedt and Ernst Koster) and by a grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (awarded to Guido Band).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lynn Bruyneel.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bruyneel, L., van Steenbergen, H., Hommel, B. et al. Happy but still focused: failures to find evidence for a mood-induced widening of visual attention. Psychological Research 77, 320–332 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0432-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0432-1

Keywords

Navigation