Skip to main content
Log in

Assigned Versus Random, Countermeasure-Like Responses in the P300 Based Complex Trial Protocol for Detection of Deception: Task Demand Effects

  • Published:
Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We recently introduced an accurate and countermeasure resistant P300-based deception detection test called the complex trial protocol (Rosenfeld et al. in Psychophysiology 45(6):906–919, 2008). When subjects use countermeasures to all irrelevant items in the test, the probe P300 is increased rather than reduced (as it was in previous P300-based deception protocols), allowing detection of countermeasure users. The current experiment examines the role of task demand on the complex trial protocol by forcing the subject to make countermeasure-like response to stimuli. Subjects made either a simple random button response to both probe and irrelevant stimuli (experiment 1) or a more complex, assigned, button response to probe and irrelevant stimuli (experiment 2). We found that an increase in task demand reduced the effectiveness of the test. Using random responses we found a simple guilty hit rate of 11/12 with no false positives, but only a 4/11 hit rate for countermeasure-users. Using assigned responses we found a simple guilty hit rate of 8/15 with no false positives, and a 7/16 hit rate for countermeasure-users. We herein suggest that the high level of task demand associated with these countermeasure-like responses causes reduced hit rates.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, J., Iacono, W. G., & Danielson, K. D. (1992). The identification of concealed memories using the event-related potential and implicit behavioral measures: A methodology for prediction in the face of individual differences. Psychophysiology, 29, 504–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Shakhar, G. (2002). A Critical Review of the Control Questions Test (CQT). In M. Kleiner (Ed.), Handbook of polygraph testing (pp. 103–126). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donchin, E., & Coles, M. G. H. (1988). Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 355–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donchin, E., Kramer, A., & Wickens, C. (1986). Applications of brain event related potentials to problems in engineering psychology. In M. Coles, S. Porges, & E. Donchin (Eds.), Psychophysiology: Systems, processes and applications (pp. 702–710). Guilford: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., Karis, D., & Donchin, E. (1987). The definition, identification and reliability of measurement of the P300 component of the event-related brain potential. In P. K. Ackles, J. R. Jennings, & M. G. H. Coles (Eds.), Advances in psychophysiology (Vol. 2, pp. 1–78). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farwell, L. A., & Donchin, E. (1991). The truth will out: Interrogative polygraphy (“lie detection”) with event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 28, 531–547.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grier, J. B. (1971). Non-parametric indexes for sensitivity and bias: Computing formulas. Psychological Bulletin, 75, 424–429.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gronau, N., Ben-Shakhar, G., & Cohen, A. (2005). Behavioral and physiological measures in the detection of concealed information. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 147–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, R. (1988). The amplitude of the P300 component of the event-related potential: Review and synthesis. In P. Ackles, J. R. Jennings, & M. G. H. Coles (Eds.), Advances in psychophysiology: A research annual (Vol. 3, pp. 69–137). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D. T. (1959). The GSR in the detection of guilt. Journal of Applied Psychology, 43, 385–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D. T. (1960). The validity of the guilty knowledge technique: The effects of faking. Journal of Applied Psychology, 44, 258–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D. T. (1981). A tremor in the blood. Reading, Mass: Perseus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meijer, E. H., Smulders, F. T. Y., Merckelbach, H. L. G. J., & Wolf, A. G. (2007). The P300 is sensitive to face recognition. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66(3), 231–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meixner, J. B., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2009). Countermeasure mechanisms in a P300-based concealed information test. Psychophysiology (in press).

  • Mertens, R., & Allen, J. B. (2008). The role of psychophysiology in forensic assessments: Deception detection, ERPs, and virtual mock crime scenarios. Psychophysiology, 45(2), 286–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, J. P., Cantwell, G., Nasman, V. T., Wojdac, V., Ivanov, S., & Mazzeri, L. (1988). A modified, event-related potential-based guilty knowledge test. International Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 157–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, J. P, & Labkovsky, E. (2009). Updated P300-based complex trial protocol to detect concealed information: Resistance to mental countermeasures against only half the irrelevant stimuli. Psychophysiology (submitted).

  • Rosenfeld, J. P., Labkovsky, E., Lui, M. A., Winograd, M., Vandenboom, C., & Chedid, K. (2008). The Complex Trial Protocol (CTP): A new, countermeasure-resistant, accurate P300-based method for detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology, 45(6), 906–919.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, J. P., Soskins, M., Bosh, G., & Ryan, A. (2004). Simple effective countermeasures to P300-based tests of detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology, 41, 205–219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, J. P., Tang, M., Meixner, J. B., Winograd, M., & Labkovsky, E. (2009). The effects of asymmetric versus symmetric probability of targets following probe and irrelevant stimuli in the complex trial protocol (CTP) for detection of concealed information with P300. Physiology and Behavior (in press).

  • Seymour, T., Seifert, C. M., Shafto, M. G., & Mosmann, A. L. (2000). Using response time measures to assess “guilty knowledge”. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 30–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soskins, M., Rosenfeld, J. P., & Niendam, T. (2001). The case for peak-to-peak measurement of P300 recorded at.3 hz high pass filter settings in detection of deception. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40(17), 3–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, S., Braren, M., Zubin, J., & John, E. R. (1965). Evoked potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science, 150, 1187–1188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • The National Research Council. (2003). The polygraph and lie detection. Washington DC: The National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman, S., & Bockenholt, U. (1989). Bootstrapping: Applications to psychophysiology. Psychophysiology, 26, 208–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Defense Academy for Credibility Assessment DODP198-P-0001 and DoDPI04-P-0002 awarded to J. Peter Rosenfeld.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John B. Meixner.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Meixner, J.B., Haynes, A., Winograd, M.R. et al. Assigned Versus Random, Countermeasure-Like Responses in the P300 Based Complex Trial Protocol for Detection of Deception: Task Demand Effects. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 34, 209–220 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-009-9091-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-009-9091-4

Keyword

Navigation