Skip to main content
Log in

Altruism across disciplines: one word, multiple meanings

  • Published:
Biology & Philosophy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Altruism is a deep and complex phenomenon that is analysed by scholars of various disciplines, including psychology, philosophy, biology, evolutionary anthropology and experimental economics. Much confusion arises in current literature because the term altruism covers variable concepts and processes across disciplines. Here we investigate the sense given to altruism when used in different fields and argumentative contexts. We argue that four distinct but related concepts need to be distinguished: (a) psychological altruism, the genuine motivation to improve others’ interests and welfare; (b) reproductive altruism, which involves increasing others’ chances of survival and reproduction at the actor’s expense; (c) behavioural altruism, which involves bearing some cost in the interest of others; and (d) preference altruism, which is a preference for others’ interests. We show how this conceptual clarification permits the identification of overstated claims that stem from an imprecise use of terminology. Distinguishing these four types of altruism will help to solve rhetorical conflicts that currently undermine the interdisciplinary debate about human altruism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The actual term ‘altruism’ started to be used around the mid-nineteenth century. Auguste Comte (18511854) defined it as the motivation to act benevolently—as opposed to ‘egoistic’ motives, which are directed towards the agent’s self-interests.

  2. Behavioural and brain sciences have recently made important advancements in our understanding of human decision-making (Bargh et al. 2010). Philosophical questions originally formulated on dated views of the cognitive architecture of the mind—in our case, that action results from a single causal chain starting with one primary motive—might have simply become inadequate or confusing in view of current scientific understanding of the mind.

  3. Interestingly, proximate mechanisms can themselves be accounted for with ultimate explanations (Clavien and Chapuisat 2012; West et al. 2007).

  4. We use quotation marks because the term homo economicus is also used for referring to economic theories that might not fit this description because they take no stance on which preferences are contained in human’s utility function (Kirchgässner 2008).

  5. Some of Ken Binmore’s big claims—e.g. he describes himself as a Hobbesian (2006)—might lead to think that he is an advocate of the “homo economicus” model. However, it should be noted that he does not deny the existence of sympathetic preferences—at least toward closely related individuals (2005: chap. 7).

  6. In practical cases, the fitness consequences of a behaviour are often estimated over a shorter period, but with the assumption that they are representative of an effect on the final life-time fitness of the individuals.

  7. Other example of this type of confusion between proximate and ultimate explanation are to be found in (Gintis et al. 2008: 249; Chaudhuri 2011: 78).

  8. We are of course not denying the fact that instances of behavioural altruism can be moral. Our point is that these behaviours unlikely qualify as moral in virtue of being behaviourally altruistic.

  9. One could debate on this point however because it is not always clear whether the authors defend a flexible or a more demanding form of other-regarding motivation (Vromen 2012). Moreover, it is in principle possible to formalize fine-grained other-regarding motivations in terms of utility functions (see Clavien 2012b).

  10. It is still a matter of debate however, to what extent this would help to improve the axiomatic theory used in economic theory (Binmore 2005).

References

  • Andrade MCB, Banta EM (2002) Value of male remating and functional sterility in redback spiders. Anim Behav 63:857–870. doi:10.1006/anbe.2002.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni J (1990) Impure altruism and donations to public goods: a theory of warm-glow giving. Econ J 100(401):464–477

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM, Oettingen G (2010) Motivation. In: Fiske ST, Gilbert DT, Lindzey G (eds) Handbook of social psychology, 5th edn. Wiley, New York, pp 268–316

    Google Scholar 

  • Batson CD (1991) The altruism question: toward a social psychological answer. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Binmore KG (2005) Natural justice. Oxford University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Binmore KG (2006) Why do people cooperate? Politics Philos Econ 5(1):81–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu P (2000) Les structures sociales de l’économie. Seuil, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles S, Gintis H (2004) The evolution of strong reciprocity: cooperation in heterogeneous populations. Theor Popul Biol 65(1):17–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles S, Gintis H (2011) A cooperative species: human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles S, Fehr E, Gintis H (2003) Strong reciprocity may evolve with or without group selection. Working paper edn

  • Butler J (1991) Fifteen sermons. In: Raphael DD (ed) British moralists, 1650–1800: selected and edited with comparative notes and analytical index, vol 1. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 325–377

  • Cabanac M, Guillaume J, Balasko M, Fleury A (2002) Pleasure in decision-making situations. BMC Psychiatry 2(1):7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter JP (2007) The demand for punishment. J Econ Behav Organ 62(4):522–542

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter JP, Bowles S, Gintis H, Hwang SH (2009) Strong reciprocity and team production: theory and evidence. J Econ Behav Organ 71(2):221–232. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2009.03.011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charness G, Gneezy U (2008) What’s in a name? Anonymity and social distance in dictator and ultimatum games. J Econ Behav Organ 68(1):29–35

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaudhuri A (2011) Sustaining cooperation in laboratory public goods experiments: a selective survey of the literature. Exp Econ 14(1):47–83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini RB, Schaller M, Houlihan D, Arps K, Fultz J, Beaman AL (1987) Empathy-based helping: is it selflessly or selfishly motivated? J Pers Soc Psychol 52(4):749–758

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clavien C (2012a) Altruistic emotional motivation: an argument in favour of psychological altruism. In: Plaisance K, Reydon T (eds) Philosophy of behavioral biology. Boston Studies in Philosophy of Science. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 275–296

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clavien C (2012b), Kitcher’s revolutionary reasoning inversion in ethics. Analyse & Kritik 1

  • Clavien C, Chapuisat M (2012) Altruism—a philosophical analysis, eLS. Wiley, Chichester. doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0003442.pub2

    Google Scholar 

  • Clavien C, Klein RA (2010) Eager for fairness or for revenge? Psychological altruism in economics. Econ Philos 26:267–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Comte A (1851–1854) Système de politique positive, ou, traité de sociologie instituant la religion de l’humanité. L. Mathias, Paris

  • Crow JF, Kimura M (1970) An introduction to population genetics theory. Harper & Row, New York

  • Dawkins R (1976) The selfish gene. Oxford University Press, New York

  • de Quervain DJF, Fischbacher U, Treyer V, Schellhammer M, Schnyder U, Buck A, Fehr E (2004) The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science 305(5688):1254–1258. doi:10.1126/science.1100735

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellingsen T, Johannesson M, Tjotta S, Torsvik G (2010) Testing guilt aversion. Game Econ Behav 68(1):95–107. doi:10.1016/j.geb.2009.04.021

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Camerer C (2007) Social neuroeconornics: the neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends Cogn Sci 11(10):419–427. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2003) The nature of human altruism. Nature 425(6960):785–791

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2004a) Social norms and human cooperation. Trends Cogn Sci 8(4):185–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2004b) Third-party punishment and social norms. Evol Human Behav 25(2):63–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Gächter S (2002) Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415:137–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Rockenbach B (2003) Detrimental effects of sanctions on human altruism. Nature 422(6928):137–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Schmidt KM (1999) A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quart J Econ 114(3):817–868. doi:10.1162/003355399556151

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foster KR (2008) Altruism. In: Jorgensen SE, Fath B (eds) Encyclopedia of ecology. pp 154–159

  • Frank SA (1998) Foundations of social evolution. Monographs in behavior and ecology. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghiselin MT (1974) The economy of nature and the evolution of sex. University of California Press, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer G (2008) Gut feelings: short cuts to better decision making. Penguin Books, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H (2000) Game theory evolving: a problem-centered introduction to modeling strategic behavior. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H, Bowles S, Boyd R, Fehr E (2003) Explaining altruistic behavior in humans. Evol Human Behav 24(3):153–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H, Bowles S, Boyd R, Fehr E (eds) (2005) Moral sentiments and material interests: the foundations of cooperation in economic life. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H, Henrich J, Bowles S, Boyd R, Fehr E (2008) Strong reciprocity and the roots of human morality. Social Justice Res 21(2):241–253

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glimcher PW, Camerer CF, Fehr E, Poldrack RA (eds) (2009) Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain. Academic Press, London

  • Grafen A (1984) Natural selection kin selection and group selection. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Sinauer, Sunderland, pp 62–84

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafen A (1985) A geometric view of relatedness. In: Dawkins R, Ridley M (eds) Oxford surveys in evolutionary biology, vol 2. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 905–907

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. J Theor Biol 7(1):1–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1970) Selfish and spiteful behaviour in an evolutionary model. Nature 288(5277):1218–1220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1975) Innate social aptitudes of man; an approach from evolutionary genetics. In: Fox R (ed) Biosocial anthropology. Malaby Press, London, pp 133–155

    Google Scholar 

  • Harbaugh WT, Mayr U, Burghart DR (2007) Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science 316(5831):1622–1625

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles S, Camerer C, Fehr E, Gintis H, McElreath R (2001) In search of homo economicus: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Am Econ Rev 91(2):73–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles S, Camerer C, Fehr E, Gintis H, McElreath R, Alvard M, Barr A, Ensminger J, Henrich NS, Hill K, Gil-White F, Gurven M, Marlowe FW, Patton JQ, Tracer D (2005) “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behav Brain Sci 28(6):795–855

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobbes T (2005) Leviathan. Broadview Press, Peterborough

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman E, McCabe K, Smith V (1996) Social distance and other-regarding behavior in dictator games. Am Econ Rev 86(3):653–660

    Google Scholar 

  • Houser D, Xiao E (2010) Inequality-seeking punishment. Econ Lett 109(1):20–23. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2010.07.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutcheson F (2004) An inquiry into the original of our ideas of beauty and virtue. Liberty Fund, Indianapolis

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen K (2010) Punishment and spite, the dark side of cooperation. Philos T R Soc B 365(1553):2635–2650. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A (1982) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirchgässner G (ed) (2008) Homo oeconomicus: the economic model of behaviour and its applications in economics and other social sciences. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher P (2011) The ethical project. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Knoch D, Gianotti LRR, Baumgartner T, Fehr E (2010) A neural marker of costly punishment behavior. Psychol Sci 21(3):337–342. doi:10.1177/0956797609360750

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban R, DeScioli P, O’Brien E (2007) Audience effects on moralistic punishment. Evol Human Behav 28(2):75–84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann L, Keller L (2006) The evolution of cooperation and altruism; a general framework and a classification of models. J Evol Biol 19(5):1365–1376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewisch PG, Ottone S, Ponzano F (2011) Free-riding on altruistic punishment? Experimental comparison of third-party-punishment in a stand-alone and in an in-group environment. Rev Law Econ 7(1):165–194. doi:10.2202/1555-5879.1460

  • Mandeville B (1997) The fable of the bees: and other writings. Hackett Publishing company, Indianapolis

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J (1976) Group selection. Quart Rev Bio 51: 277-283

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J (1989) Evolutionary genetics. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr E (1961) Cause and effect in biology. Science 134:1501–1506

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr U, Harbaugh WT, Tankersley D (2009) Neuroeconomics of charitable giving and philantropy. In: Glimcher PW, Camerer CF, Fehr E, Poldrack RA (eds) Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain, 1st edn. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 303–320

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagel T (1970) The possibility of altruism. Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Okasha S (2007) Evolution and the levels of selection. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Peacock MS, Schefczyk M, Schaber P (2005) Altruism and the indispensibility of motives. Anal Kritik 27:188–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Queller DC (1992) A general-model for kin selection. Evolution 46(2):376–380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rand A (1964) The virtue of selfishness: a new concept of egoism. New American Library, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruse M (1998) Taking darwin seriously: a naturalistic approach to philosophy. Prometheus Books, Buffalo

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanfey AG (2007) Social decision-making: insights from game theory and neuroscience. Science 318:598–602

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon HA (1996) The sciences of the artificial, 3rd edn. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer T, Fehr E (2005) The neuroeconomics of mind reading and empathy. Am Econ Rev 95(2):340–345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer T, Kiebel SJ, Winston JS, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (2004) Brain responses to the acquired moral status of faces. Neuron 41(4):653–662. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00014-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith A (2002) The theory of moral sentiments. Cambridge texts in the history of philosophy. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Sober E (1992) Hedonism and Butler’s stone. Ethics 103(1):97–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sober E (1993) Evolutionary altruism, psychological egoism and morality; disentangling the phenotypes. In: Nitecki MH et al (eds) Evolutionary ethics. SUNY Press, Albany, pp 199–216

    Google Scholar 

  • Sober E, Wilson DS (1998) Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Stich SP (2007) Evolution, altruism and cognitive architecture: a critique of sober and Wilson’s argument for psychological altruism. Biol Philos 22(2):267–281

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stich SP, Doris JM, Roedder E (2010) Altruism. In: Doris JM (ed) The moral psychology handbook. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 147–205

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stocks EL, Lishner DA, Decker SK (2009) Altruism or psychological escape: why does empathy promote prosocial behavior? Eur J Soc Psychol 39(5):649–665

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen N (1963) On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 20:410–433

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46(1):35–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vromen J (2012) Human cooperation and reciprocity. In Okasha S, Binmore K (eds) Evolution and rationality: decisions, cooperation and strategic behavior, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

  • West SA, Griffin A, Gardner A (2007) Social semantics: altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. J Evol Biol 20:415–432

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West SA, El Mouden C, Gardner A (2011) Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evol Human Behav 32(4):231–262

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams GC (1966) Adaptation and natural selection; a critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton University Press, Princeton

  • Wilson DS (1975) A theory of group selection. P Natl Acad Sci USA 72(1):143–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi A (1975) Mate selection—a selection for a handicap. J Theor Biol 53(1):205–214

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the editor, an anonymous reviewer, Chloë FitzGerald, Conrad Heilmann, Laurent Keller, Laurent Lehmann, Jessica Purcell, and Jack Vromen for comments on the manuscript. We are grateful to Philip Kitcher who gave us the impulsion to work out the concept of “preference altruism”. Our research is supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation and the Fondation du 450ème of the University of Lausanne.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christine Clavien.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Clavien, C., Chapuisat, M. Altruism across disciplines: one word, multiple meanings. Biol Philos 28, 125–140 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-012-9317-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-012-9317-3

Keywords

Navigation