Skip to main content
Log in

Narrative Discourse in Adults with High-Functioning Autism or Asperger Syndrome

  • Original paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We report a study comparing the narrative abilities of 12 adults with high-functioning autism (HFA) or Asperger Syndrome (AS) versus 12 matched controls. The study focuses on the use of referential expressions (temporal expressions and anaphoric pronouns) during a story-telling task. The aim was to assess pragmatics skills in people with HFA/AS in whom linguistic impairments are more subtle than in classic autism. We predicted no significant differences in general narrative abilities between the two groups, but specific pragmatic deficits in people with AS. We predicted they use fewer personal pronouns, temporal expressions and referential expressions, which require theory of mind abilities. Results confirmed both predictions. These findings provide initial evidence of how social impairments can produce mild linguistic impairments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • APA. (1994). DSM-IV diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. (1991). Intention in the child’s theory of mind. In D. Frye & C. Moore, (Eds.), Children’s theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J., & Jenkins, J. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311–1320.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Attwood, T. (1997). Asperger syndrome: a guide for parents and professionals. Jessica Kingsley Pub.

  • Baltaxe, C., & Simmons, J. (1983). Communication deficits in the adolescent and adult autistic. Seminars in Speech and Language, 4, 27–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1988a). Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: Cognitive or affective? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 379–402.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1988b). Without a theory of mind one cannot participate in a conversation. Cognition, 29, 83–84.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 113–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker's direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68(1), 48–57.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Two new theories of autismo: hyper-systemisng and addortative mating. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 91, 2–5.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997a). Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813–822.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. (1997b). Is there a “ Language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4, 311–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioural and Intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Jones, R., Stone, V., & Plaisted, K. (1999). A new test of social sensitivity: Detection of faux pas in normal children and children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407–418.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test Revised Version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–251.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J., & Feldman, C. (1993). Theory of mind and the problem of autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Losh, M., & Thurber, C. (2000). “The frog Ate the Bug and made his mouth sad”: narrative competence in children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 193–204.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M., Fritz, A., & Hala, S. (1989). Small-scale deceit: deception as a marker of two, three, and four-years-olds’ early theories of mind. Child Development, 60, 1263–1277.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M., Lazenby, A., & Lockyer, L. (2001). Inferential language in high-function children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 47–54.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • De Villiers J. (2000). Language and theory of mind: What are the developmental relationships? In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Villiers, J. G. & de Villiers, R. A. (2000). Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false beliefs. In P. Mitchell & K. Riggs (Eds.), Children's reasoning and the mind (pp. 189–226). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

  • Enc, M. (1987). Anchoring condition for tense. Linguistic Inquiry, 18, 633–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghaziuddin, M., & Gerstein, L. (1996). Pedantic speaking style differentiates Asperger’s syndrome from high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 585–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1994). An advance test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feeling by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal f Autism and Development Disorders, 24, 129–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843–855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ICD-10, (1994). International classification of diseases. Geneva: World Health Organization.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 61–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Shultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2001). The social dysfunction in autism defying and quantifying the phenotype. New Haven, CT 06520: Yale University School of medicine, Child Study Centre.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook Jr., E., Leventhal, B., DiLavore, P., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (1999). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism.

  • Loveland, K., & Tunali, B. (1993). Narrative language in autism and the theory of mind hypothesis: A wider perspective. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., McEvoy, R. E., Tunali, B., & Kelley, M. L. (1990). Narrative story telling in autism and Down’s syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 9–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, M., (1965). Frog where are you? New York: Dial Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In: S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds), Understanding other minds (pp. 335–366). Oxford Medical Publications.

  • Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1996). An exploration of right-hemisphere contributions to the pragmatic impairments of autism. Brain and Language, 52, 411–434.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. C., & Siegal M. (2000). Insight into theory of mind from deafness and autism. Mind and Language, 15, 123–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, J., Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1990). Once more with feeling: Affect and language in atypical populations. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 367–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D., & Berman, R. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A cross linguistic developmental study. Hilsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., & Wilson, K. (1986). Relevance: communication and cognition. Oxford, Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & van der Lely, H. (1996). Are children with autism deaf to Gricean Maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 55–72.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (1992). Autistic children’s talk about psychological states: Deficits in the early acquisition of a theory of mind. Child Development, 63, 161–172.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (1995). ‘Once upon a ribbit’: Stories narrated by autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 45–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Anderson, M. (1991). The development of contingent discourse ability in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1123–1134.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1994). Predicting and explaining behavior: a comparison of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1059–1075.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1995). Attributing mental states to story characters: A comparison of narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 241–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Lely, H. (1997). Narrative discourse in grammatical specific language impaired children: a modular language deficit? Journal of Child Language, 24, 221–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D., (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: The psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziatas, K., Durkin, K., & Pratt, C. (1998). Belief term development in children with autism, asperger syndrome, specific language impairment and normal development : links to theory of mind development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 5, 755–763.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participants for their help, and Chris Ashwin, Alison Clare and Jacqueline Hill for their assistance. Livia Colle was supported by the Ministero italiano dell’Istruzione e dell’Università e della Ricerca (FIRB Project, “Assessment dei disturbi della comunicazione in un’ottica riabilitativa” code n. RBAU01JEYW_001) in the development of this work. Simon Baron-Cohen and Sally Wheelwright were supported by the MRC and Heather van der Lely was supported by the Wellcome Trust (Grant-nos: 044179; 059876; 063713) during the period of this work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Livia Colle.

Appendix 1

Appendix 1

Analysis of the references to the two main protagonists

The following counts were made:

Type of NPs:

  • indef. NPs

  • def. NPs

  • pronouns

  • zero anaphors

  • total number of references (indef. NPs + def. NPs + pronouns + zeros + relative pronouns + possessive determiners)

Episodes and expressions used for maintaining reference:

  • total number of episodes

  • Episodes with at least one maintenance of reference

  • Episodes with one or more pronouns and/or zero referring back to an expression in that episode

  • Number of pronouns and zeros referring back to a previous expression

Ambiguous references:

  • Number of ambiguous references for (i) pronouns; (ii) possessive pronouns

Inappropriate indef. NPs:

  • number of indef. NPs used inappropriately

Theory of mind expressions:

  • number of verbs and adjective that describes any characters’ affective and cognitive states (e.g., “the boy was sad”, “he believed it was a bush”)

Temporal expressions:

  • number of adverbs, across the episodes, which relate temporally two sentences (e.g., “when he was in the pound, he hear...”)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S. et al. Narrative Discourse in Adults with High-Functioning Autism or Asperger Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 38, 28–40 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0357-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0357-5

Keywords

Navigation