Skip to main content
Log in

Decontextualised Minds: Adolescents with Autism are Less Susceptible to the Conjunction Fallacy than Typically Developing Adolescents

  • original paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The conjunction fallacy has been cited as a classic example of the automatic contextualisation of problems. In two experiments we compared the performance of autistic and typically developing adolescents on a set of conjunction fallacy tasks. Participants with autism were less susceptible to the conjunction fallacy. Experiment 2 also demonstrated that the difference between the groups did not result from increased sensitivity to the conjunction rule, or from impaired processing of social materials amongst the autistic participants. Although adolescents with autism showed less bias in their reasoning they were not more logical than the control group in a normative sense. The findings are discussed in the light of accounts which emphasise differences in contextual processing between typical and autistic populations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolphs, R. (1999). The human amygdala and emotion. The Neuroscientist, 5, 125–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV) (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennetto, L., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., & Berthollier, N. (2004). Source memory in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 533–542.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., Norbury, C. F., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-movements. Cognition, 108, 896–904.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burnette, C. P., Mundy, P. C., Meyer, J. A., Sutton, S. K., Vaughan, A. E., & Charak, D. (2005). Weak central coherence and its relations to theory of mind and anxiety in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 63–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caron, M.-J., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2006). Cognitive mechanisms, specificity and neural underpinnings of visuo-spatial peaks in autism. Brain, 129, 1789–1802.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Gernsbacher, M. A., & Mottron, L. (2007). The level and nature of autistic intelligence. Psychological Science, 18, 657–662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50, 115–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Snowling, M. (1983). Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic and dyslexic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 329–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1992). Bully for Brontosaurus: Further reflections in natural history. London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Handley, S., Capon, A., Beveridge, M., Dennis, I., & Evans, J. S. B. T. (2004). Working memory, inhibitory control, and the development of children’s reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 10, 175–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 216–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 5–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertwig, R., Benz, B., & Krauss, S. (2008). The conjunction fallacy and the many meanings of “and”. Cognition, 108, 740–753.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertwig, R., & Gigerenzer, G. (1999). The “conjunction fallacy” revisited: How intelligent inferences look like reasoning errors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 275–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, D. J., Foss, D. J., & Carroll, P. (1995). Effects of global and local context on lexical processing during language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 62–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L., Bartmess, E., White, S., & Frith, U. (2007). Can autistic children predict behavior by social stereotypes? Current Biology, 17, 451–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., & Brock, J. (2004). To match or not to match? Methodological issues in autism-related research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 81–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory: Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome—Is local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71, 149–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome: Can global coherence be achieved? A further test of central coherence theory. Psychological Medicine, 30, 1169–1187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics & Biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 49–81). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kokis, J., Macpherson, R., Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Heuristic and analytic processing: Age trends and associations with cognitive ability and cognitive styles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 26–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • López, B., & Leekam, S. R. (2003). Do children with autism fail to process information in context? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44, 285–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrory, E., Henry, L., & Happé, F. (2007). Eye-witness memory and suggestibility in children with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 482–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morsanyi, K., & Handley, S. J. (2008). How smart do you need to be to get it wrong? The role of cognitive capacity in the development of heuristic-based judgment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99(1), 18–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morsanyi, K., & Holyoak, K. J. (2009). Analogical reasoning ability in autistic and typically-developing children. Developmental Science (in press).

  • Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Stauder, J. E. A., & Robaey, P. (1999). Perceptual processing among high-functioning persons with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 203–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, R. G. (2006). Confidence intervals for an effect size measure based on the Mann–Whitney statistic. Part 1: General issues and tail-area-based methods. Statistics in Medicine, 25, 543–557.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., Maybery, M., & Durkin, K. (2005). Central coherence in typically developing preschoolers: Does it cohere and does it relate to mindreading and executive control? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 543–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pexman, P. M. (2008). It’s fascinating research: The cognition of verbal irony. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 286–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Politzer, G. & Noveck, I. A. (1991). Are conjunction rule violations the result of conversational rule violations? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 83–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C. (1938). Progressive matrices: A perceptual test of intelligence. London: H. K. Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Manual for Raven’s advanced progressive matrices (1998th ed.). Oxford, England: Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saldana, D., & Frith, U. (2007). Do readers with autism make bridging inferences from world knowledge? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 310–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sattler, J. M. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applications (4th ed.). San Diego, CA: Jerome M. Sattler.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., & Frith, U. (1986). Comprehension in ‘hyperlexic’ readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 392–415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences in framing and conjunction effects. Thinking and Reasoning, 4, 289–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tentori, K., Bonini, N., & Osherson, D. (2004). The conjunction fallacy: A misunderstanding about conjunction? Cognitive Science, 28, 467–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1982). Judgements of and by representativeness. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovich, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 84–98). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional vs intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90, 293–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (WISC-III) (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S., O’Reilly, H., & Frith, U. (2009). Big heads, small details and autism. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1274–1281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kinga Morsanyi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Morsanyi, K., Handley, S.J. & Evans, J.S.B.T. Decontextualised Minds: Adolescents with Autism are Less Susceptible to the Conjunction Fallacy than Typically Developing Adolescents. J Autism Dev Disord 40, 1378–1388 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0993-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0993-z

Keywords

Navigation