Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Deterrence and Individual Differences Among Convicted Offenders

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Journal of Quantitative Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Conflicting evidence exists on how criminal propensity moderates deterrent effects, and there is little empirical evidence on this issue from relatively experienced offenders. This study tested how variation in criminal propensity (operationalized as “low self-control”) moderates deterrent effects in a sample of convicted offenders in New Jersey’s Intensive Supervision Program in 1989 and 1990. Offenders’ perceptions of the risks and consequences from violating ISP were associated with whether they successfully completed ISP. Moreover, lower self-control did not diminish, and if anything, enhanced these deterrent effects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Tittle and Botchkovar (2005) distinguished “propensity” from “low self-control,” but recognized the two are correlated with one another.

  2. In this view, highly conformist individuals do not offend, irrespective of instrumental concerns, because they are morally or extralegally restrained. Highly criminally prone individuals are also unresponsive to instrumental considerations because their commitment to nonconformity supercedes the consideration of sanction threats. A similar typology was elaborated by Pogarsky (2002).

  3. Individuals convicted of murder, robbery, and sexual offenses are ineligible.

  4. A large number, approximately 5–10% of adult parolees and probationers, participate in ISP (Petersilia 1998).

  5. A partial exception is Maxwell and Gray (2000). Their study only examined the deterrent effect of the perceived certainty of punishment in ISP. The present study investigates a more extensive set of issues, including the deterrent effect of the perceived severity of punishment and the moderating impacts of self-control.

  6. Missing data reduced the available sample from approximately 512 to 434. There was no evidence of differential attrition. That is, some excluded respondents nevertheless had intact information for one or more study variables. There were no statistically discernible differences between included and excluded respondents on any of the key measures.

  7. Each active participant had served at least 505 days in ISP when data collection terminated. The vast majority of program failures occurred during the offender’s first year in ISP. Among offenders with known outcomes who served at least 505 days in the program, 83% successfully completed ISP. Therefore, the 76 active participants were treated as successes in later analyses. The findings were fundamentally unchanged when these 76 respondents were excluded.

  8. Some offenders even prefer a modest period of incarceration over ISP because they consider ISP overly punitive and believe there is a high probability of violation and return to prison (Petersilia 1990; Wood and Grasmick 1999).

  9. Some participants may not necessarily believe they will serve their entire remaining prison term if they violate ISP. The length of the suspended prison sentence was intended to reflect nominal “jail exposure.”

  10. Several measures that were unused in the present analyses support this assumption. Respondents rated on an 11-point Likert scale (from “hate it” to “love it”) their feelings about the curfew, counseling, and job requirements in ISP. The perceived certainty measure was positively related to all three measures, although the relationship was only significant at P < 0.05 for the first two. This provides some indication that respondents’ views about different aspects of ISP supervision were intercorrelated. In any case, comparable findings were produced with both outcomes. The second outcome was whether or not drug use was detected in ISP and, therefore, more closely “matched” the perceived certainty measure.

  11. Thought was given to using survival analysis to model the time before exiting ISP (if at all). Although a similar pattern of findings was obtained with survival models, this modeling strategy seemed inappropriate. In addition to determining who is admitted into ISP, the judicial panel also periodically reviews each case and either discharges the offender from custody, continues supervision, or violates the offender back to prison. Thus, after the 18-month minimum for successful discharge, ISP can end for one of two opposite reasons: either the offender is successfully discharged or violated. Earlier it was observed that a majority of offenders who remain in ISP beyond the minimum 18-month period successfully complete the program. Still, some offenders were violated after a long but ultimately unsuccessful period of supervision. For each active case then, it is unclear whether a longer time in ISP is positive because the offender has not yet violated ISP, or negative because the offender has not yet persuaded the judicial panel to discharge him or her from custody. This makes “time to failure” a conceptually ambiguous reflection of ISP outcome.

  12. There was no interaction between the two.

  13. This percentage is high because violent offenders are ineligible for ISP and property offending is often related to drug use.

  14. Since past drug use is so explicit a criterion for this subsample, the variable is excluded from Model (5).

  15. Although in the latter model, the coefficient is not statistically distinguishable from zero (P < 0.065).

  16. Logistic regressions take the form, ln \(\left[ {\frac{P} {{1 - P}}} \right]\, = \,\alpha \, + \,{{\bf {\beta}}}\,{{\bf X}}{\hbox{,}}\) where P is the probability of success, and β and X are vectors of regression coefficients and variables, respectively. Solving for P yields \( P\, = \,\frac{1} {{1\, + \,{\hbox{e}}^{ - ({\hbox{ $ \alpha $ }}\, + \,{\hbox{ $ \beta $ X}})} }}, \) thus permitting the calculations in Fig. 1.

  17. An alternative interpretation of the findings merits brief discussion. If the perceived certainty measure actually captured the respondents’ perceived skill at avoiding detection for drug use in ISP, the present deterrent effects might be somewhat spurious. Yet all models controlled for both the respondents’ and their friends’ prior drug use, factors that are likely to be correlated with perceived skill at avoiding detection for drug use. This counsels against this possible alternative interpretation.

  18. The distribution of low self-control was split at its median.

  19. The certainty coefficients are in the expected positive direction.

  20. According to Allison (1999), the more conservative coefficient comparison test is only warranted if the residual variation in fact differs across groups. Using stata macros developed by Hoetker (2004), the null hypothesis of equal residual variation across groups is only rejected at P < 0.05 for detected drug use (models 6 and 7), but not for success in ISP (models 8 and 9). Nevertheless, Allison’s more conservative coefficient comparison test is used in both cases.

  21. Consistent with this view, several studies suggest low self-control is multidimensional (Arneklev et al. 1993; Longshore et al. 1996; but see Piquero and Rosay 1998).

References

  • Allison PD (1999) Comparing logit and probit coefficients across groups. Sociol Methods Res 28:186–208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andenaes J (1974) Punishment and deterrence. Ann Arbor: Univ. of Michigan Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Arneklev BJ, Grasmick HG, Tittle CR, Bursik RJ Jr (1993) Low self-control and imprudent behavior. J Quant Criminol 9:225–247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bureau of Justice Statistics (2004) Probation and parole in the United States, 2003. U. S. Department of Justice, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Clear TR, Braga AA (1995) Community corrections. In Wilson JQ, Petersilia J (eds), Crime, Institute for Contemporary Studies, San Francisco, CA, pp 421–444

    Google Scholar 

  • Cullen FT, Wright JP, Applegate BK (1995) Control in the community: the limits of reform? In Harland AT (eds), Choosing correctional options that work, Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA, pp 69–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Decker S, Wright R, Logie R (1993) Perceptual deterrence among active residential burglars: a research note. Criminology 31:135–147

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etzioni A (1988) The moral dimension: toward new economics. The Free Press, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs JP (1975) Crime, punishment, and deterrence. Elsevier Scientific Publishing Co., Amsterdam, The Netherlands

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T (1990) A general theory of crime. Macmillan Publishing Co., New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Grasmick HG, Bryjak G (1980) The deterrent effect of perceived severity of punishment. Soc Forces 59:471–491

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoetker G (2004) Confounded coefficients: extending recent advances in the accurate comparison of logit and probit coefficients across groups. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, College of Business, working paper

  • Klepper S, Nagin DS (1989) Certainty and severity of punishment revisited. Criminology 27:721–746

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Longshore D, Turner S, Stein JA (1996) Self-control in a criminal sample: an examination of construct validity. Criminology 34:209–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maruna S, Copes H (2004) What have we learned from five decades of neutralization research? In Tonry M (ed) Crime and justice: a review of research. University of Chicago Press, pp 221–320

  • Maxwell SR, Gray MK (2000) Deterrence: testing the effects of perceived sanction certainty on probation violations. Sociol Inq 70:117–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan KD (1993) Factors influencing probation outcome: a review of the literature. Fed Probat 57:23–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagin DS, Paternoster R (1994) Personal capital and social control: the deterrence implications of a theory of individual differences in criminal offending. Criminology 32:581–604

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagin DS, Paternoster R (1993) Enduring individual differences and rational choice theories of crime. Law Soc Rev 27:467–496

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagin DS, Paternoster R (1991) On the relationship of past to future participation in delinquency. Criminology 29:163–189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagin DS, Pogarsky G (2004) Time and punishment: delayed consequences and criminal behavior. J Quant Criminol 20:295–317

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagin DS, Pogarsky G (2001) Integrating celerity, impulsivity, and extralegal sanction threats into a model of general deterrence: theory and evidence. Criminology 39:404–430

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parsons T (1937) The structure of social action. The Free Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Paternoster R (1989) Decisions to participate in and desist from four types of common delinquency: deterrence and the rational choice perspective. Law Soc Rev 23:7–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paternoster R (1987) The deterrent effect of the perceived certainty and severity of punishment: a review of the evidence and issues. Justice Q 4:173–217

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paternoster R, Iovanni L (1986) The deterrent effect of perceived severity: a reexamination. Soc Forces 64:751–777

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson FS (2002) Deterring drug use with intensive probation in New Jersey, 1989–1990 [computer file]. ICPSR version. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University, Department of Sociology, Institute for Criminological research [producer], 1990. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor], 2002

  • Pearson FS (1988) Evaluation of New Jersey’s intensive supervision program. Crime Delinq 34:437–448

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson FS, Harper AG (1990) Contingent intermediate sentences: New Jersey’s intensive supervision program, Crime Delinq 36:75–86

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersilia J (1990) When probation becomes more dreaded than prison. Fed Probat 54:23–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersilia J (1998) Community corrections: probation, parole and intermediate sanctions. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersilia J, Turner S (1993) Intensive probation and parole. Crim Justice 17:281–335

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pilliavin I, Thornton C, Gartner R, Matsueda R (1986) Crime, deterrence, and rational choice. Am Sociol Rev 51:101–119

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR, Pogarsky G (2002) Beyond Stafford and Warr’s reconceptualization of deterrence: personal and vicarious experiences, impulsivity, and offending behavior. J Res Crime Delinq 39:153–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR, Rengert GF (1999) Studying deterrence with active residential Burglars. Justice Q 16(2):451–471

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR, Rosay AB (1998) The reliability and validity of Grasmick et al.’s self-control scale: a comment on Longshore et al. Criminology 36:157–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR, Tibbetts S (1996) Specifying the direct and indirect effects of low self-control and situational factors in offenders’ decision making: toward a more complete model of rational offending. Justice Q 13:481–510

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pogarsky G (2002) Identifying “deterrable” offenders: implications for research on deterrence. Justice Q 19:431–452

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tittle CR, Botchkovar EV (2005) Self-control, criminal motivation and deterrence: an investigation using Russian respondents. Criminology 43:307–353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tittle CR, Rowe AR (1973) Moral appeal, sanction threat and deviance; an experimental test. Soc Probl 20: 488–498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tittle CR, Ward DA, Grasmick HG (2004) Capacity for self-control and individuals’ interest in exercising self-control. J Quant Criminol 20:143–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams K, Hawkins R (1986) Perceptual research on general deterrence: a critical overview. Law Soc Rev 20: 545–572

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood P, Grasmick H (1999) Toward the development of punishment equivalencies: male and female inmates rate the severity of alternative sanctions compared to prison. Justice Q 16:19–50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood P, May DC (2003) Racial differences in perceptions of the severity of sanctions: a comparison of prison with alternatives. Justice Q 20:605–632

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright BRE, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Paternoster R (2004) Does the perceived risk of punishment deter criminally-prone individuals? Rational choice, self-control, and crime. J Res Crime Delinq 41:180–213

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimring FE, Hawkins GJ (1968) Deterrence and marginal groups. J Res Crime Delinq 2:110–114

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Valuable input into this manuscript is acknowledged from Shawn Bushway, Bill McCarthy, Daniel Nagin, Alex Piquero, Min Xie, and participants at the 2005 Criminology and Economics Summer Workshop organized by the Population Research Center at the University of Maryland.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Greg Pogarsky.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Pogarsky, G. Deterrence and Individual Differences Among Convicted Offenders. J Quant Criminol 23, 59–74 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-006-9019-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-006-9019-6

Keywords

Navigation