Skip to main content
Log in

Accurate and biased perceptions of responsive support predict well-being

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The current research examined effects of accurate and biased perceptions of romantic partners’ responsive support provision on perceivers’ well-being. Perceivers discussed a personal problem with their romantic partners (“targets”). Perceivers’ perceptions of targets’ responsive support following the discussion were related to external indicators of targets’ behavior, but these perceptions also were predicted by perceivers’ sentiments toward targets, suggesting that processes underlying perceivers’ perceptions were a blend of both accuracy and bias. In addition, both accurately perceived and biased perceptions of targets’ responsiveness predicted perceivers’ personal well-being (i.e., affect, coping, self-efficacy) and interpersonal well-being (i.e., more positive sentiments toward targets) immediately after the support interaction, 2 weeks later, and 6 months later. Results suggest that accurate and biased cognition during interpersonal interactions can have important consequences for perceivers’ personal and interpersonal well-being through effects on perceived partner responsiveness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Despite our description of romantic involvement as an eligibility criterion, 19 relationships (7.7 %) were described as friendships. It is unclear whether these relationships are casual romantic relationships or whether they are platonic friendships. Responses from these participants were retained in our analyses.

  2. Perceivers who failed to complete the 6-month follow-up did not significantly differ from perceivers who completed the 6-month follow-up on initial valuing of targets, trust, positive reframing, self-distraction, self-efficacy, positive affect, negative affect, felt acceptance, perceptions of targets' responsive behavior, or the index of targets' responsive behavior. However, perceivers who completed the 6-month follow-up did have higher scores on the index of active planning/coping (M = 6.31) relative to participants who did not complete the follow-up (M = 5.88), t(242) = −2.03, p < .05.

  3. One SD below the mean on the index of perceivers' valuing still represented absolutely positive sentiments because of the high mean (M = 7.78, SD = .94). When we examined discrepancies for perceivers who scored 2 SDs below the mean on valuing of targets, the underestimation effect reached conventional significance levels, b = −.53, t = −.2.71, p < .01.

  4. In models predicting perceivers’ valuing of targets, we used subsequent valuing, rather than a residual change score, as the criterion because the residual change score would be purged of all variance associated with earlier valuing of targets and therefore using this score as the criterion would preclude us from testing the mediation model depicted in Fig. 1 (i.e., it would be impossible to examine the total, direct, and indirect effects of perceivers' earlier valuing of targets). Moreover, by including earlier valuing of targets as a predictor, this model controls for initial standing on the criterion, similar to the models predicting other outcomes. Hence, this model involves the same statistical rigor as the other models.

  5. Technically, these total effects should be considered adjusted total effects because they are adjusted for the other predictor in the model. That is, the effect of targets' behavior is adjusted for perceivers' sentiments and vice versa.

  6. The effects of targets' responsive behavior and perceivers' valuing on change in perceivers' self-efficacy reversed direction in Model 2 relative to Model 1, indicating statistical suppression. Under conditions of statistical suppression, a significant total effect of a predictor on an outcome is not necessary to demonstrate indirect effects (see MacKinnon et al. 2000; Shrout and Bolger 2002).

References

  • Abbey, A., Andrews, F. M., & Halman, L. J. (1995). Provision and receipt of social support and disregard: What is their impact on the marital life quality of infertile and fertile couples? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 455–469.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. R., McNulty, J. K., Overall, N. C., Lambert, N. M., & Fincham, F. D. (2013). How do relationship maintenance behaviors affect individual well-being? A contextual perspective. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 282–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolger, N., Zuckerman, A., & Kessler, R. C. (2000). Invisible support and adjustment to stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 953–961.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Branje, S. J., van Aken, M. A., & van Lieshout, C. F. (2002). Relational support in families with adolescents. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., Dangelmayer, G., & Schultheiss, O. C. (1996). Personal goals and social support in close relationships: Effects on relationship mood and marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1006–1019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol’too long: Consider the brief cope. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 92–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, M. S., & Lemay, E. P, Jr. (2010). Close relationships. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 898–940). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, S. (1976). Presidential address-1976. Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 38, 300–314.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, S., & Hoberman, H. M. (1983). Positive events and social supports as buffers of life change stress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13, 99–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2000). A safe haven: An attachment theory perspective on support seeking and caregiving in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1053–1073.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coriell, M., & Cohen, S. (1995). Concordance in the face of a stressful event: When do members of a dyad agree that one person supported the other? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 289–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cutrona, C. E. (1996). Social support as a determinant of marital quality: The interplay of negative and supportive behaviors. In G. R. Pierce & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Handbook of social support and the family (pp. 173–194). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dunkel-Schetter, C., & Bennett, T. L. (1990). Differentiating the cognitive and behavioral aspects of social support. In B. R. Sarason, I. G. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce (Eds.), Social support: An interactional view (pp. 267–296). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feeney, B. C. (2004). A secure base: Responsive support of goal strivings and exploration in adult intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 631–648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finch, J. F. (2008). Social undermining, support satisfaction and affect: A domain-specific lagged effects model. Journal of Personality, 66, 315–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, M. S., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychological Science, 18, 233–239.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaul, M., & Lakey, B. (2003). Where is the support in perceived support? The role of generic relationship satisfaction and enacted support in perceived support’s relation to low distress. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22, 59–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakey, B., & Cassady, P. B. (1990). Cognitive processes in perceived social support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 337–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lemay, E. P, Jr., & Clark, M. S. (2008). How the head liberates the heart: Projection of communal responsiveness guides relationship promotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 647–671.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lemay, E. P, Jr., Clark, M. S., & Feeney, B. C. (2007). Projection of responsiveness to needs and the construction of satisfying communal relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 834–853.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lemay, E. P, Jr., & Melville, M. C. (in press). Diminishing self-disclosure to maintain security in partners’ care. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

  • Lemay, E. P, Jr., & Neal, A. M. (2013). The wishful memory of interpersonal responsiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 653–672.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1, 173–181.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maisel, N. C., & Gable, S. L. (2009). The paradox of received social support: The importance of responsiveness. Psychological Science, 20, 928–932.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maisel, N. C., Gable, S. L., & Strachman, A. M. Y. (2008). Responsive behaviors in good times and in bad. Personal Relationships, 15, 317–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manne, S. L., & Zautra, A. J. (1989). Spouse criticism and support: Their association with coping and psychological adjustment among women with rheumatoid arthritis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 608–617.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martz, J. M., Verette, J., Arriaga, X. B., Slovik, L. F., Cox, C. L., & Rusbult, C. E. (1998). Positive illusion in close relationships. Personal Relationships, 5, 159–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCaskill, J. W., & Lakey, B. (2000). Perceived support, social undermining, and emotion: Idiosyncratic and shared perspectives of adolescents and their families. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 820–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNulty, J. K., O’Mara, E. M., & Karney, B. R. (2008). Benevolent cognitions as a strategy of relationship maintenance: “Don’t sweat the small stuff”….But it is not all small stuff. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 631–646.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mills, J., Clark, M. S., Ford, T. E., & Johnson, M. (2004). Measurement of communal strength. Personal Relationships, 11, 213–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, S. L., & Holmes, J. G. (1997). A leap of faith? Positive illusions in romantic relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 586–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996). The benefits of positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfaction in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 79–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neely, L. C., Lakey, B., Cohen, J. L., Barry, R., Orehek, E., Abeare, C. A., et al. (2006). Trait and social processes in the link between social support and affect: An experimental, laboratory investigation. Journal of Personality, 74, 1015–1046.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pasch, L. A., Bradbury, T. N., & Sullivan, K. T. (1997). Social support in marriage: An analysis of interindividual and interpersonal components. In G. R. Pierce, B. Lakey, I. G. Sarason, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Sourcebook of social support and personality (pp. 229–256). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, G. R., Sarason, B. R., & Sarason, I. G. (1992). General and specific support expectations and stress as predictors of perceived supportiveness: An experimental study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, G. R., Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1996). Coping and social support. In M. Zeidner & N. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of coping (pp. 434–451). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). Spss and sas procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K., & Hayes, A. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., & Holmes, J. G. (2004). Perceived partner responsiveness as an organizing construct in the study of intimacy and closeness. In D. J. Mashek & A. P. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 201–225). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H. T., Collins, W. A., & Berscheid, E. (2000). The relationship context of human behavior and development. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 844–872.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as an interpersonal process. In S. Duck, D. F. Hay, S. E. Hobfoll, & W. Ickes (Eds.), Handbook of personal relationships: Theory, research and interventions (pp. 367–389). Oxford: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rusbult, C. E., Lange, P. A. M. V., Wildschut, T., Yovetich, N. A., & Verette, J. (2000). Perceived superiority in close relationships: Why it exists and persists. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 521–545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rusbult, C. E., Martz, J. M., & Agnew, C. R. (1998). The investment model scale: Measuring commitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and investment size. Personal Relationships, 5, 357–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarason, I. G., Sarason, B. R., & Pierce, G. R. (1995). Stress and social support. In S. E. Hobfoll & M. W. deVries (Eds.), Extreme stress and communities: Impact and intervention (pp. 179–197). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

  • Schaefer, C., Coyne, J. C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). The health-related functions of social support. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 381–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Krieger, L. S. (2004). Does legal education have undermining effects on law students? Evaluating changes in motivation, values, and well-being. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 22, 261–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 422–445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Srivastava, S., McGonigal, K. M., Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2006). Optimism in close relationships: How seeing things in a positive light makes them so. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 143–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Veenstra, A. L., Lakey, B., Cohen, J. L., Neely, L. C., Orehek, E., Barry, R., et al. (2011). Forecasting the specific providers that recipients will perceive as unusually supportive. Personal Relationships, 18, 677–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vinokur, A., Schul, Y., & Caplan, R. D. (1987). Determinants of perceived social support: Interpersonal transactions, personal outlook, and transient affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1137–1145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wieselquist, J., Rusbult, C. E., Foster, C. A., & Agnew, C. R. (1999). Commitment, pro-relationship behavior, and trust in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 942–966.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank members of the Interpersonal Relationships Lab, University of New Hampshire, for their assistance with data collection and coding, including Kevin Cannon, Christine Coyne, Heather Balch, Manuel Lopez, Aparecio Peggins, Ryan Keene, Monica Rosskothen, Ashley Scheidegger, Choe Shannon, Kerry Spongberg, and Benjamin Stucker. Other findings from this study have been published elsewhere (Lemay and Neal 2013; Lemay and Melville in press), but none of the findings reported in the current manuscript have been previously reported. This research was supported by a research grant awarded to the first author by the National Science Foundation (grant number BCS 1145349).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Edward P. Lemay Jr..

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lemay, E.P., Neal, A.M. Accurate and biased perceptions of responsive support predict well-being. Motiv Emot 38, 270–286 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9381-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9381-2

Keywords

Navigation