Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 1/2010

01.03.2010 | Original Paper

A Computational Model of the Citizen as Motivated Reasoner: Modeling the Dynamics of the 2000 Presidential Election

verfasst von: Sung-youn Kim, Charles S. Taber, Milton Lodge

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 1/2010

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

A computational model of political attitudes and beliefs is developed that incorporates contemporary psychological theory with well-documented findings from electoral behavior. We compare this model, John Q. Public (JQP), to a Bayesian learning model via computer simulations of observed changes in candidate evaluations over the 2000 presidential campaign. In these simulations, JQP reproduces responsiveness, persistence, and polarization of political attitudes, while the Bayesian learning model has difficulty accounting for persistence and polarization. We conclude that “motivated reasoning”—the discounting of information that challenges priors along with the uncritical acceptance of attitude-consistent information—is the reason our model can better account for persistence and polarization in candidate evaluations.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
Our model is developed within the ACT-R cognitive architecture (The A daptive C haracter of T hought – R ational; Anderson et al. 2004), which is a leading theoretical and modeling framework used in cognitive science for a wide range of learned behaviors, among them language comprehension, the recognition and recall of information, inferencing, the formation of beliefs, and the learning of complex skills. However, while ACT-R provides comprehensive, integrated sets of cognitive mechanisms for learning, it lacks affective mechanisms, which are essential to current theories of political information-processing, and it lacks specific mechanisms for preference updating. Consequently, much of our work developing JQP was devoted to building affective and updating mechanisms and integrating them with the cognitive processes in ACT-R.
 
2
This notation for random noise is conventional in computational modeling because it makes transparent the manipulation of the normal density function.
 
3
S ji represents the strength of association from node j to node i. It is an increasing function of the number of times node j has sent activation to node i, and it is not symmetrical (Anderson 1993). C ji  = 1 when nodes j and i share the same valence (positive or negative), C ji  = −1 when they have different valences, and C ji  = 0 when either of them is neutral.
 
4
The number of buffers could be manipulated to represent greater or lesser cognitive limitations, but we do not explore this implication here.
 
5
To test the internal validity of the model (i.e., whether the model behaves as expected in our theory), we conducted a series of purely formal computational experiments (Kim 2005) in which the model successfully reproduced practice, recency, and spreading of activation effects on recall; cognitive and attitude priming effects; question order and wording effects in the survey response; both on-line and memory-based processing; and the ability to learn by adjusting beliefs and attitudes in response to campaign events. These tests were essential to establish that the model is in fact consistent with a wide range of “well-known phenomena” from the empirical cognitive literatures. These tests are available from the first author.
 
6
For the trait perceptions to be useful, we need to assign a value to each trait. That is, when a respondent says “Bush is trustworthy,” we need to have some idea how positive this attribution is. Fortunately, most human traits and other general concepts have been normed by large samples of respondents, so we can scale the evaluative implications of a given trait (positivity, negativity, intensity, etc.) by consulting the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW; Bradley and Lang 1999) and N. Anderson (1968), which provide means and standard deviations for a large number of trait concepts.
 
7
Recall also that transient random noise is added to the activation levels of all objects in memory each time an object is retrieved (Eq. 1), which also approximates individual differences in processing.
 
8
The average of correlation between the actual and simulated evaluations over time and that across groups was used as a fit measure. However, the same qualitative results were obtained with wide ranges of parameter values.
 
9
The parameter search space was extensive; the values within the interval [0, 10] with stride 0.1 for P 0 , h, and q, and those within [0, 1] with stride 0.01 for γ were first examined. Then, the values within [0, 1] with stride 0.01 for γ, h, and q were examined in a refined search. For very small values of h and q (0, 0.01, and 0.02), simulation results and model fits were almost identical. With the optimized parameter values, the weight, K t , did not converge during the simulation.
 
10
The values within [0, 1] with stride 0.01 were examined.
 
11
Formally, JQP will be a motivated reasoner as long as γ > 0, 0 < δ < 1, 0 < ρ < 1 and its belief structure is reasonably consistent.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Abelson, R. (1963). Computer simulation of personality. New York: Wiley. Abelson, R. (1963). Computer simulation of personality. New York: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Achen, C. (1992). Social psychology, demographic variables, and linear regression: Breaking the iron triangle in voting research. Political Behavior, 14, 195–211.CrossRef Achen, C. (1992). Social psychology, demographic variables, and linear regression: Breaking the iron triangle in voting research. Political Behavior, 14, 195–211.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson, N. (1968). Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 272–279.CrossRef Anderson, N. (1968). Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 272–279.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of mind. Psychological Review, 111, 1036–1060.CrossRef Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of mind. Psychological Review, 111, 1036–1060.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: Partisan bias in political perceptions. Political Behavior, 24, 117–150.CrossRef Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: Partisan bias in political perceptions. Political Behavior, 24, 117–150.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley, M., & Lang, P. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW). Gainesville, FL: The NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida. Bradley, M., & Lang, P. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW). Gainesville, FL: The NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida.
Zurück zum Zitat Bullock, J. G. (2009). Partisan bias and the Bayesian ideal in the study of public opinion. Journal of Politics, 71, 1109–1124.CrossRef Bullock, J. G. (2009). Partisan bias and the Bayesian ideal in the study of public opinion. Journal of Politics, 71, 1109–1124.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: Harper and Row. Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: Harper and Row.
Zurück zum Zitat Fazio, R. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 115–141.CrossRef Fazio, R. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 115–141.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gerber, A., & Green, D. P. (1998). Rational learning and partisan attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 42, 794–818.CrossRef Gerber, A., & Green, D. P. (1998). Rational learning and partisan attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 42, 794–818.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hamill, R., & Lodge, M. (1985). Cognitive consequences of political sophistication. In R. R. Lau & D. O. Sears (Eds.), Political cognition (pp. 69–93). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hamill, R., & Lodge, M. (1985). Cognitive consequences of political sophistication. In R. R. Lau & D. O. Sears (Eds.), Political cognition (pp. 69–93). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Hastie, R., & Pennington, N. (1989). Notes on the distinction between memory-based and on-line judgment. In J. Bassili (Ed.), On-line cognition in person perception (pp. 1–19). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hastie, R., & Pennington, N. (1989). Notes on the distinction between memory-based and on-line judgment. In J. Bassili (Ed.), On-line cognition in person perception (pp. 1–19). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim, S. (2005). A model of political information processing: The dynamics of candidate evaluation and strategic behaviors in social dilemma games. Unpublished Dissertation, Stony Brook University. Kim, S. (2005). A model of political information processing: The dynamics of candidate evaluation and strategic behaviors in social dilemma games. Unpublished Dissertation, Stony Brook University.
Zurück zum Zitat Kinder, D. R. (1998). Opinion and action in the realm of politics. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 778–867). Boston: McGraw-Hill. Kinder, D. R. (1998). Opinion and action in the realm of politics. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 778–867). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Zurück zum Zitat LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon and Schuster. LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Zurück zum Zitat LeDoux, J. (2003). Synaptic self: How our brains become who we are. London: Penguin Books. LeDoux, J. (2003). Synaptic self: How our brains become who we are. London: Penguin Books.
Zurück zum Zitat Lodge, M., Steenbergen, M., & Brau, S. (1995). The responsive voter: Campaign information and the dynamics of candidate evaluation. American Political Science Review, 89, 309–326.CrossRef Lodge, M., Steenbergen, M., & Brau, S. (1995). The responsive voter: Campaign information and the dynamics of candidate evaluation. American Political Science Review, 89, 309–326.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2000). Three steps toward a theory of motivated political reasoning. In A. Lupia, M. McCubbins, & S. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason (pp. 183–213). New York: Cambridge University Press. Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2000). Three steps toward a theory of motivated political reasoning. In A. Lupia, M. McCubbins, & S. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason (pp. 183–213). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2005). The primacy of affect for political candidates, groups, and issues: An experimental test of the hot cognition hypothesis. Political Psychology, 26, 333–487.CrossRef Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2005). The primacy of affect for political candidates, groups, and issues: An experimental test of the hot cognition hypothesis. Political Psychology, 26, 333–487.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Miller, G. A. (1957). The magic number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–93.CrossRef Miller, G. A. (1957). The magic number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–93.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Niemi, R. G., & Jennings, M. K. (1991). Issues and inheritance in the formation of party identification. American Journal of Political Science, 35, 970–988.CrossRef Niemi, R. G., & Jennings, M. K. (1991). Issues and inheritance in the formation of party identification. American Journal of Political Science, 35, 970–988.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Page, B. I., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1992). The rational public. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Page, B. I., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1992). The rational public. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Romer, D., Kenski, K., Waldman, P., Adasiewicz, C., & Jamieson, K. H. (2003). Capturing campaign dynamics: The National Annenberg Election Survey. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Romer, D., Kenski, K., Waldman, P., Adasiewicz, C., & Jamieson, K. H. (2003). Capturing campaign dynamics: The National Annenberg Election Survey. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Taber, C. S. (2003). Information processing and public opinion. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology (pp. 433–476). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Taber, C. S. (2003). Information processing and public opinion. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology (pp. 433–476). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755–769.CrossRef Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755–769.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Taber, C. S., & Steenbergen, M. R. (1995). Computational experiments in electoral behavior. In M. Lodge & K. McGraw (Eds.), Political judgment: Structure and process (pp. 141–206). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. Taber, C. S., & Steenbergen, M. R. (1995). Computational experiments in electoral behavior. In M. Lodge & K. McGraw (Eds.), Political judgment: Structure and process (pp. 141–206). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of the survey response. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of the survey response. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Zajonc, R. B. (1984). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.CrossRef Zajonc, R. B. (1984). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Feeling and thinking: Closing the debate over the independence of affect. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 31–58). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Feeling and thinking: Closing the debate over the independence of affect. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 31–58). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press. Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Zaller, J., & Feldman, S. (1992). A simple theory of the survey response. American Journal of Political Science, 36, 579–616.CrossRef Zaller, J., & Feldman, S. (1992). A simple theory of the survey response. American Journal of Political Science, 36, 579–616.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
A Computational Model of the Citizen as Motivated Reasoner: Modeling the Dynamics of the 2000 Presidential Election
verfasst von
Sung-youn Kim
Charles S. Taber
Milton Lodge
Publikationsdatum
01.03.2010
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 1/2010
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-009-9099-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2010

Political Behavior 1/2010 Zur Ausgabe