Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 1/2014

01.03.2014 | Original Paper

The Relationship Between Campaign Negativity, Gender and Campaign Context

verfasst von: Yanna Krupnikov, Nichole M. Bauer

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 1/2014

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Are female candidates disproportionately punished for relying on negative campaign ads? While scholars agree that sponsoring negativity works against traditional gender stereotypes, it is less clear how relying on negativity affects voter evaluations of female candidates. In this manuscript we reconsider the relationship between candidate gender and negativity. Relying on theories of conditional stereotype use, we argue that negative ads translate to significantly poorer evaluations for the female candidate when two conditions are met: (1) the female candidate is perceived as the instigator of negativity and (2) she is of a different party than the voter. We test our predictions using an experiment and show that female candidates only face a disproportionate punishment for relying on negativity under our two specific conditions. In contrast, voters are much more forgiving when they believe that a female candidate simply followed her opponent’s lead in using negative ads or when negativity is used to promote the voter’s party. While our research suggests that—compared to their male counterparts—female candidates do face some added constraints, our findings have broader implications. Not only are voters more or less likely to use gender stereotypes under certain conditions, but these conditions are highly dependent on the campaign context.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
Other work also considers negativity and gender. Kahn (1993) and Nelson (2008) find few differences in negative ad use for male and female candidates. Dinzes et al. (1994) show that target gender does not affect response; Fridkin et al. (2009) show that negative ads lower evaluations of male targets. Other scholars find that female candidates are most successful with negative ads on women’s issues (Woodall 2005; Herrnson and Lucas 2006).
 
2
Bailey’s name was previously used in Brooks (2011). We conducted a separate pre-test to ensure Larson’s name did not produce any confounding effects.
 
3
We acknowledge that there are different definitions of negativity, which consider not only the tone of ads, but also the content (see Lau and Rovner 2009 for a review). We focus on Geer’s definition as previous experimental research on negativity has relied on this approach.
 
4
Our sample was recruited following Berinsky et al. (2012). Using this method, Berinsky et al. (2012) replicated many canonical psychology experiments, concluding that this approach is an effective and innovative way to recruit subjects and conduct online studies. This method relies on recruiting through an innovative new platform to Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk). Berinsky et al. (2012) have conducted extensive testing on MTurk and have found that the samples obtained through MTurk are more representative of the general population than undergraduate samples and other Internet-based opt-in samples. In our sample, 23 were excluded for either failing a simple attention check or refusing to answer various questions. Inclusion or exclusion of those who failed the check does not shift our results, though the results presented here do exclude them.
 
5
Power of 0.90, α = 0.05; differences in power calculation based on pilot study.
 
6
We also conduct a randomization check to ensure that there are no systematic differences within our groups (on similar covariates). Using a multinomial logit we show that these factors do not jointly predict assignment to our groups (χ 2 = 78.41, p = 0.680). We also use a logit to consider whether these factors predict assignment to either a control or treatment version of our manipulations; they do not suggesting that there are no systematic differences on these covariates within our groups which disrupt the randomization process. Finally, we use an ANOVA and a Bonferroni test to compare the levels across the covariates to ensure balance. No pair-wise Bonferroni comparisons reach significance, reinforcing the idea that there are no group imbalances.
 
7
Preferences for the candidate of the opposing party at baseline (ANOVA): F = 1.20 (p = 0.310); preferences for the candidate of the subject’s party at baseline (ANOVA): F = 1.16 (p = 0.328).
 
8
Furthermore these control results are similar to previous research that has considered the baseline interaction between gender and partisanship for hypothetical candidates (see for example Dolan (2010) on willingness to vote for a female President of a certain party).
 
9
We replicate a similar pattern by estimating a model that predict the likelihood of having a reason to vote against a candidate and uses the following controls: gender of subject, interest in politics, strength of partisanship, region of residence and income. The model is as follows: Reasons to Vote Against Candidate = f(Order, Gender, PID (relative to subject), Order × Gender, Gender × PID, Order × PID, Gender × Order × PID, Control for Opponent, Controls) This model show that while going negative first uniformly increases the likelihood of a subject having a reason to vote against the candidate, this is largest and significant when we consider a female candidate of the opposing party. Specifically, when we consider the change in likelihood of having a reason to vote against the candidate as a function of going negative first we obtain the following results (pooling opponent gender for ease of interpretation): Woman, Different Party + 0.24 (significant at p < 0.01), Woman, Same Party + 0.03, Man, Different Party, +0.08, Man, Same Party +0.06. In sum, much like Table 3 suggests, the punishment is highest and reaches significance only under predicted conditions. To obtain these results controls were set to mode (categorical).
 
10
We coded this index in two ways. First, we maintain the same order coding for both male and female candidates, meaning that for a female candidate a lower score means more counter-stereotypic judgments, but for a male candidate a higher score means a more counter-stereotypic judgments. Second, we also reverse-code the scale for a male candidate, meaning that for all candidates a lower score means more counter-stereotypic evaluations. These changes in coding do not have any effect on our analysis.
 
11
For this variable, a positive score for a female candidate would mean counter-stereotypic perceptions, much like a negative score would mean counter-stereotypic perceptions of a male candidate. Again, we also create a reverse-code measure such that a positive score means counter-stereotypic evaluations for both female and male candidates and again, this has no effect on our results.
 
12
Our results indicate that for the female instigator of the opposing party the ACME is significant at the 95 % level. We present the 90 % confidence intervals to reinforce the fact that the ACME is not distinguishable from 0 for other cases.
 
13
Using the Huddy and Terkildsen (1993) measures. Percentages using the Brooks (2011)/Rudman et al. (2001) measure are substantively similar.
 
14
We ease the assumption of a non-interactive relationship between the treatment and mediator.
 
15
We also consider which of our stereotype index components are most effective at shifting evaluations. To do so, we follow Huddy and Terkildsen (1993) and separate the index into “warmth and expressiveness” factors and “instrumental” factors. We find evidence that the instrumental factors—the masculine stereotypes—have the stronger mediational effect on the evaluations of the female candidate of the opposing party. This offers more evidence for our theoretic approach: it is not enough to simply be less feminine, but a woman has to be more fully counter-stereotypic and display more masculine characteristics.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Ansolabehere, S., & Iyengar, S. (1995). Going negative: How campaign advertising shrinks and polarizes the electorate. New York: The Free Press. Ansolabehere, S., & Iyengar, S. (1995). Going negative: How campaign advertising shrinks and polarizes the electorate. New York: The Free Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Banjo, S. (2011). The time a softer approach. The Wall Street Journal, A17. Banjo, S. (2011). The time a softer approach. The Wall Street Journal, A17.
Zurück zum Zitat Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Using mechanical turk as a subject recruitment tool for experimental research. Political Analysis, 20, 351–368.CrossRef Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Using mechanical turk as a subject recruitment tool for experimental research. Political Analysis, 20, 351–368.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brader, T. (2005). Striking a responsive chord: How political ads motivate and persuade voters by appealing to emotions. American Journal of Political Science, 49, 388–405.CrossRef Brader, T. (2005). Striking a responsive chord: How political ads motivate and persuade voters by appealing to emotions. American Journal of Political Science, 49, 388–405.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brooks, D. J. (2011). Testing the double standard for candidate emotionality: Voter reactions to the tears and anger of male and female politicians. Journal of Politics, 73, 597–615.CrossRef Brooks, D. J. (2011). Testing the double standard for candidate emotionality: Voter reactions to the tears and anger of male and female politicians. Journal of Politics, 73, 597–615.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dinzes, D., Cozzens, M. D., & George, C. M. (1994). The role of gender in ‘attack ads’: Revisiting negative political advertising. Communication Research Reports, 11, 67–75.CrossRef Dinzes, D., Cozzens, M. D., & George, C. M. (1994). The role of gender in ‘attack ads’: Revisiting negative political advertising. Communication Research Reports, 11, 67–75.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dolan, K. (2010). The impact of gender stereotyped evaluations on support for women candidate. Political Behavior, 32, 69–88.CrossRef Dolan, K. (2010). The impact of gender stereotyped evaluations on support for women candidate. Political Behavior, 32, 69–88.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dolan, K., & Kira, S. (2011). Candidate gender and experimental political science. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science. New York: Cambridge University Press. Dolan, K., & Kira, S. (2011). Candidate gender and experimental political science. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598.CrossRef Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fowler, E. F., & Travis, N. R. (2010). Advertising trends in 2010. The Forum, 8(4), 1–16. Fowler, E. F., & Travis, N. R. (2010). Advertising trends in 2010. The Forum, 8(4), 1–16.
Zurück zum Zitat Fridkin, K. L., Kenney, P. J., & Woodall, G. S. (2009). Bad for men, better for women: The impact of stereotypes during political campaigns. Political Behavior, 31, 53–77.CrossRef Fridkin, K. L., Kenney, P. J., & Woodall, G. S. (2009). Bad for men, better for women: The impact of stereotypes during political campaigns. Political Behavior, 31, 53–77.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Geer, J. G. (2006). In defense of negativity: Attack ads in presidential campaigns. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Geer, J. G. (2006). In defense of negativity: Attack ads in presidential campaigns. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Geer, J. G. (2012). The news media and the rise of negativity in presidential campaigns. PS: Political Science and Politics, 45, 422–434. Geer, J. G. (2012). The news media and the rise of negativity in presidential campaigns. PS: Political Science and Politics, 45, 422–434.
Zurück zum Zitat Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 509–517.CrossRef Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 509–517.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gordon, A., Shafie, D. M., & Crigler, A. N. (2003). Is negative advertising effective for female candidates?: An experiment in voters’ uses of gender stereotypes. The Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, 8, 35–53.CrossRef Gordon, A., Shafie, D. M., & Crigler, A. N. (2003). Is negative advertising effective for female candidates?: An experiment in voters’ uses of gender stereotypes. The Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, 8, 35–53.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hayes, D. (2011). When gender and party collide: Stereotyping in candidate trait attribution. Politics & Gender, 7, 133–165.CrossRef Hayes, D. (2011). When gender and party collide: Stereotyping in candidate trait attribution. Politics & Gender, 7, 133–165.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Herrnson, P. S., Celeste Lay, J., & Stokes, A. K. (2003). Women running as women: Candidate gender, campaign issues, and voter-targeting strategies. The Journal of Politics, 65, 244–255.CrossRef Herrnson, P. S., Celeste Lay, J., & Stokes, A. K. (2003). Women running as women: Candidate gender, campaign issues, and voter-targeting strategies. The Journal of Politics, 65, 244–255.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Herrnson, P. S., & Lucas, J. C. (2006). The fairer sex? Gender and negative campaigning in U.S. elections. American Politics Research, 34, 69–94.CrossRef Herrnson, P. S., & Lucas, J. C. (2006). The fairer sex? Gender and negative campaigning in U.S. elections. American Politics Research, 34, 69–94.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hitchon, J. C., & Chang, C. (1995). Effects of gender schematic processing on the reception of political commercials for men and women candidates. Communication Research, 22, 430–458.CrossRef Hitchon, J. C., & Chang, C. (1995). Effects of gender schematic processing on the reception of political commercials for men and women candidates. Communication Research, 22, 430–458.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hitchon, J. C., Chang, C., & Harris, R. (1997). Should women emote? Perceptual bias and opinion change in response to political ads for candidates of different genders. Political Communication, 14, 49–69.CrossRef Hitchon, J. C., Chang, C., & Harris, R. (1997). Should women emote? Perceptual bias and opinion change in response to political ads for candidates of different genders. Political Communication, 14, 49–69.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Holloway, R., & Johnston, L. (2006). Evaluating the evaluators: Perceptions of interviewers by rejected job applicants as a function of interviewer and applicant sex. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 2635–2648.CrossRef Holloway, R., & Johnston, L. (2006). Evaluating the evaluators: Perceptions of interviewers by rejected job applicants as a function of interviewer and applicant sex. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 2635–2648.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Houston, D. A., & Doan, K. (1999). Can you back that up? Media Psychology, 1, 191–206.CrossRef Houston, D. A., & Doan, K. (1999). Can you back that up? Media Psychology, 1, 191–206.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huddy, L., & Terkildsen, N. (1993). Gender stereotypes and the perception of male and female candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 119–147.CrossRef Huddy, L., & Terkildsen, N. (1993). Gender stereotypes and the perception of male and female candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 119–147.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Imai, K., Keele, L., Tingley, D., & Yamamoto, T. (2011). Unpacking the black box of causality: Learning about causal mechanisms from experimental and observational studies. American Political Science Review, 105, 765–789.CrossRef Imai, K., Keele, L., Tingley, D., & Yamamoto, T. (2011). Unpacking the black box of causality: Learning about causal mechanisms from experimental and observational studies. American Political Science Review, 105, 765–789.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Imai, K. & Teppei, Y. (2012). Identification and sensitivity analysis for multiple causal mechanisms: Revisiting evidence from framing experiments. Working Paper. Imai, K. & Teppei, Y. (2012). Identification and sensitivity analysis for multiple causal mechanisms: Revisiting evidence from framing experiments. Working Paper.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnston, A., & White, A. B. (1994). Communication styles and female candidates: A study of the political advertising during the 1986 senate elections. Journalism Quarterly, 71, 321–329.CrossRef Johnston, A., & White, A. B. (1994). Communication styles and female candidates: A study of the political advertising during the 1986 senate elections. Journalism Quarterly, 71, 321–329.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kahn, K. F. (1993). Gender differences in campaign messages: The political advertisements of men and women candidates for the U.S. senate. Political Research Quarterly, 46, 481–502. Kahn, K. F. (1993). Gender differences in campaign messages: The political advertisements of men and women candidates for the U.S. senate. Political Research Quarterly, 46, 481–502.
Zurück zum Zitat Koch, J. W. (2000). Do citizens apply gender stereotypes to infer candidates’ ideological orientations? The Journal of Politics, 62, 414–429.CrossRef Koch, J. W. (2000). Do citizens apply gender stereotypes to infer candidates’ ideological orientations? The Journal of Politics, 62, 414–429.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunda, Z., & Sinclar, L. (1999). Motivated reasoning with stereotypes: Activation, application, and inhibition. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 12–22.CrossRef Kunda, Z., & Sinclar, L. (1999). Motivated reasoning with stereotypes: Activation, application, and inhibition. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 12–22.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kunda, Z., & Spencer, S. (2003). When do stereotypes come to mind and when do they color judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework for stereotype activation and application. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 522–544.CrossRef Kunda, Z., & Spencer, S. (2003). When do stereotypes come to mind and when do they color judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework for stereotype activation and application. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 522–544.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lau, R. R. (1982). Negativity in political perception. Political Behavior, 4, 353–378.CrossRef Lau, R. R. (1982). Negativity in political perception. Political Behavior, 4, 353–378.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lau, R. R., & Rovner, I. B. (2009). Negative campaigning. Annual Review of Political Science, 12, 285–306.CrossRef Lau, R. R., & Rovner, I. B. (2009). Negative campaigning. Annual Review of Political Science, 12, 285–306.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lau, R. R., Sigelman, L., & Rovner, I. B. (2007). The effects of negative political campaigns: A meta-analytic reassessment. The Journal of Politics, 69, 1176–1209.CrossRef Lau, R. R., Sigelman, L., & Rovner, I. B. (2007). The effects of negative political campaigns: A meta-analytic reassessment. The Journal of Politics, 69, 1176–1209.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (2004). Designing experiments and analyzing data. New York: Routledge Academic. Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (2004). Designing experiments and analyzing data. New York: Routledge Academic.
Zurück zum Zitat McDermott, M. L. (1998). Voting cues in low-information elections: candidate gender as a social information variable in contemporary United States elections. American Journal of Political Science, 41, 270–283.CrossRef McDermott, M. L. (1998). Voting cues in low-information elections: candidate gender as a social information variable in contemporary United States elections. American Journal of Political Science, 41, 270–283.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mezey, S. G. (1978). Does sex make a difference: A case study of women in politics. Western Political Quarterly, 31, 492–501.CrossRef Mezey, S. G. (1978). Does sex make a difference: A case study of women in politics. Western Political Quarterly, 31, 492–501.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson, K. (2008). If you can’t say something nice: The gender dynamics of negative advertising. Boston, MA: American Political Science Association, Annual Meeting. Nelson, K. (2008). If you can’t say something nice: The gender dynamics of negative advertising. Boston, MA: American Political Science Association, Annual Meeting.
Zurück zum Zitat Panagopoulos, C. (2004). Boy talk/girl talk gender differences in campaign communication strategies. Women & Politics, 34, 131–155.CrossRef Panagopoulos, C. (2004). Boy talk/girl talk gender differences in campaign communication strategies. Women & Politics, 34, 131–155.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Peterson, D. A. M., & Djupe, P. A. (2005). When primary campaigns go negative: The determinants of campaign negativity. Political Research Quarterly, 58, 45–54. Peterson, D. A. M., & Djupe, P. A. (2005). When primary campaigns go negative: The determinants of campaign negativity. Political Research Quarterly, 58, 45–54.
Zurück zum Zitat Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 629–645.CrossRef Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 629–645.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., & McGhee, D. E. (2001). Implicit self-concept and evaluative implicit gender stereotypes: Self and ingroup share desirable traits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1164–1178.CrossRef Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., & McGhee, D. E. (2001). Implicit self-concept and evaluative implicit gender stereotypes: Self and ingroup share desirable traits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1164–1178.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schneider, M. C., & Bos, A. L. (forthcoming). Measuring Stereotypes of Female Politicians. Political Psychology. Schneider, M. C., & Bos, A. L. (forthcoming). Measuring Stereotypes of Female Politicians. Political Psychology.
Zurück zum Zitat Sinclair, L., & Kunda, Z. (2000). Motivated stereotyping of women: She’s fine if she praised me but incompetent if she criticized me. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1329–1342.CrossRef Sinclair, L., & Kunda, Z. (2000). Motivated stereotyping of women: She’s fine if she praised me but incompetent if she criticized me. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1329–1342.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Skaperdas, S., & Grofman, B. (1995). Modeling negative campaigning. American Political Science Review, 89, 49–61.CrossRef Skaperdas, S., & Grofman, B. (1995). Modeling negative campaigning. American Political Science Review, 89, 49–61.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Trent, J., & Sabourin, T. (1993). Sex still counts: Women’s use of televised advertising during the decade of the 80’s. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 21, 21–40.CrossRef Trent, J., & Sabourin, T. (1993). Sex still counts: Women’s use of televised advertising during the decade of the 80’s. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 21, 21–40.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat West, D. (2005). Air wars: Television advertising in election campaigns, 1952–2004 (4th ed.). Washington, DC: CQ Press. West, D. (2005). Air wars: Television advertising in election campaigns, 1952–2004 (4th ed.). Washington, DC: CQ Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Woodall, G. S. (2005). Playing hardball in a dress? An examination of gender differences in the effectiveness of negative campaigns. Dissertation. Arizona State University. Woodall, G. S. (2005). Playing hardball in a dress? An examination of gender differences in the effectiveness of negative campaigns. Dissertation. Arizona State University.
Metadaten
Titel
The Relationship Between Campaign Negativity, Gender and Campaign Context
verfasst von
Yanna Krupnikov
Nichole M. Bauer
Publikationsdatum
01.03.2014
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 1/2014
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-013-9221-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2014

Political Behavior 1/2014 Zur Ausgabe