Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 1/2019

16.01.2018 | Original Paper

The Elusive Backfire Effect: Mass Attitudes’ Steadfast Factual Adherence

verfasst von: Thomas Wood, Ethan Porter

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 1/2019

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Can citizens heed factual information, even when such information challenges their partisan and ideological attachments? The “backfire effect,” described by Nyhan and Reifler (Polit Behav 32(2):303–330. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s11109-010-9112-2, 2010), says no: rather than simply ignoring factual information, presenting respondents with facts can compound their ignorance. In their study, conservatives presented with factual information about the absence of Weapons of Mass Destruction in Iraq became more convinced that such weapons had been found. The present paper presents results from five experiments in which we enrolled more than 10,100 subjects and tested 52 issues of potential backfire. Across all experiments, we found no corrections capable of triggering backfire, despite testing precisely the kinds of polarized issues where backfire should be expected. Evidence of factual backfire is far more tenuous than prior research suggests. By and large, citizens heed factual information, even when such information challenges their ideological commitments.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
Google News search for the “backfire”, “backlash”, or “boomerang” effect and the names of Nyhan or Reifler returns over 300 unique articles. The 2010 backfire paper has also enjoyed remarkable academic attention. Among all papers printed in Political Behavior in the last 10 years, “When Corrections Fail” has been cited four times as much as the next most cited paper.
 
2
In this way, the apparent difficulty in making one’s policy preferences fit with one’s factual attitudes is redolent of Americans’ struggle to have their policy preferences fit with each other—what Converse (1964) famously described as poor “constraint.”
 
3
To avoid the possibility of unintended panel conditioning, we excluded any Turk worker which had been participated in a prior study.
 
4
The choice of the OLS model, and the specific measures for agreement, ideology, and correction, were chosen to be consistent with Nyhan and Jason (2010).
 
5
This relationship persisted if we compare respondents along the partisan scale. This result is described in Sect. A.14.1 on p. xxvi.
 
6
Of course, the attitudinal consequence of this fact remains at a respondent’s discretion, but functional democratic competence would seem to require that voters adopt a common set of basic political facts.
 
7
Three articles were taken from study 3: the original Bush WMD article, the piece by Speaker Paul Ryan criticizing President Obama’s policy toward abortion, and Secretary Hillary Clinton claim that twice as many Americans were employed in solar than in the oil industry. Three novel mock articles were also provided: Senator Sanders claiming that the EPA had found fracking was responsible for polluting water supplies, Donald Trump claiming that his tax cut plan would grow federal tax receipts, and Trump claiming that the true unemployment rate was actually higher than 30%. These mock articles can be read in Sect. A.9, which can be found in the appendix on p. xvi. The items can be read in Table 11 on p. xxiii.
 
8
For this study, President Obama, Secretary Clinton, and Senator Sanders are deemed liberal speakers, and President Trump and President Bush are deemed conservative speakers.
 
9
For instance, the national representative panel who adopted the correction that the flu vaccines did not induce flu infections (Nyhan et al. 2015) or the national representative panel who accepted the correction that the MMR vaccines did not cause autism (Nyhan et al. 2014).
 
10
Coppock and McClellan (2017) report an extensive test of the Lucid sample, comparing it to Turk, the Census Bureau’s Current Population Survey, and the American National Election Study’s (ANES) face to face and online samples. Treating the ANES face-to-face sample as the “gold standard”, the Lucid sample is more psychologically similar to the ANES than the Turk sample on the Big-5 personality battery, and better matches the political knowledge and conservatism in the ANES. Coppock and McClellan also test the Lucid sample’s ability to recover treatment effects in canonical social psychology experiments. Both Lucid and Turk samples recover the same framing effect observed the General Social Survey (a massive face-to-face survey instrument), improving the appetite for public spending when it is described as “assistance to the poor” or “caring for the poor” rather than “welfare.” Both Lucid and Turk feature the same framing effect underpinning prospect theory [(the famous Tversky and Kahneman (1983) finding which shows risk tolerance is affected by framing possible outcomes as gains or losses.] Both Lucid and Turk recover indistinguishable experimental effects as observed in Hiscox (2006) in framing attitudes about free trade. Most importantly for this study, the one failed replication was on rumor corrections in the aforementioned Berinsky paper (2017), where Lucid respondents were unusually resistant to corrective information. This suggests that the Lucid sample is at least a comparably demanding sample in which to test factual adherence.
 
11
In brief—a weak correction might inadvertently advertise the weakness of the corrective case, or a strong correction might have more obvious factual implications, and therefore inspire more forceful counterargument.
 
12
These respondents were recruited on Mechanical Turk.
 
13
As a robustness check—there was no significant relationship between ideology and perceived accordance, for any of the tested pairs.
 
14
It’s instructive to consider those statement/correction pairs at either end of this spectrum. The statement by Senator Ted Cruz about the incidence of violence targeted at law enforcement, described above, was judged the most proximate correction. At the other end of this continuum is the 2012 claim by Congressman Paul Ryan that “Obama stands for an absolute, unqualified right to abortion—at any time, under any circumstances, and paid for by taxpayers” and the correction that “The number of abortions steadily declined during President Obama’s first term, with fewer abortions in 2012 than any year since 1973.” While Cruz makes a precise claim about the change in the incidence of killings of police officers, Ryan’s statements merely suggested a spike in the incidence of abortion.
 
15
An example of a proximate correction is Representative Gutiérrez’s promise that President Obama would be the “champion...[of the] undocumented” paired with the evidence that Obama was a prodigious deporter of these residents. This correction/statement pair was scored 81.7 on a 100-pt scale of accordance.
 
16
An example of a distant correction is Governor Romney’s description of the United States using “a credit card ...issued by the Bank of China” and the correction that China holds about 15% of US debt. This correction/statement pair was scored 47.2.
 
17
Contra our evidence in “Does Counterargument Explain Our Pattern of Findings?” section.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Barnes, L., Feller, A., Haselswerdt, J., & Porter, E. (2016). Information and preferences over redistributive policy: A field experiment. Working Paper. Barnes, L., Feller, A., Haselswerdt, J., & Porter, E. (2016). Information and preferences over redistributive policy: A field experiment. Working Paper.
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American Voter. New York: Wiley. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American Voter. New York: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Cochran, W. G., & Cox, G. M. (1957). Experimental designs. New York: Wiley. Cochran, W. G., & Cox, G. M. (1957). Experimental designs. New York: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Fowler, A., & Montagnes, B. P. (2015). College football, elections, and false-positive results in observational research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(45), 13800–13804.CrossRef Fowler, A., & Montagnes, B. P. (2015). College football, elections, and false-positive results in observational research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(45), 13800–13804.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Howell, W. G., & West, M. R. (2009). Educating the public. Education Next, 9(3), 40–47. Howell, W. G., & West, M. R. (2009). Educating the public. Education Next, 9(3), 40–47.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuklinski, J. H., & Quirk, P. J. (2000). Reconsidering the rational public: Cognition, heuristics, and mass opinion. In Elements of reason: Cognition, choice and the bounds of rationality (pp. 153–182). Kuklinski, J. H., & Quirk, P. J. (2000). Reconsidering the rational public: Cognition, heuristics, and mass opinion. In Elements of reason: Cognition, choice and the bounds of rationality (pp. 153–182).
Zurück zum Zitat Lippman, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company. Lippman, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company.
Zurück zum Zitat Nyhan, B., Reifler, J., Edelman, C., Passo, W., Banks, A., Boston, E., et al. (2015). Answering on cue?. Hanover, NH: Dartmouth College. Nyhan, B., Reifler, J., Edelman, C., Passo, W., Banks, A., Boston, E., et al. (2015). Answering on cue?. Hanover, NH: Dartmouth College.
Zurück zum Zitat Prior, M. (2007). Is partisan bias in perceptions of objective conditions real? The effect of an accuracy incentive on the stated beliefs of partisans. In Annual conference of the Midwestern Political Science Association. Prior, M. (2007). Is partisan bias in perceptions of objective conditions real? The effect of an accuracy incentive on the stated beliefs of partisans. In Annual conference of the Midwestern Political Science Association.
Zurück zum Zitat Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1993). Reasoning and choice: Explorations in political psychology. Cambridge University Press. Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1993). Reasoning and choice: Explorations in political psychology. Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
The Elusive Backfire Effect: Mass Attitudes’ Steadfast Factual Adherence
verfasst von
Thomas Wood
Ethan Porter
Publikationsdatum
16.01.2018
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 1/2019
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9443-y

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

Political Behavior 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe