Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 3/2020

01.12.2018 | Original Paper

Attribute Affinity: U.S. Natives’ Attitudes Toward Immigrants

verfasst von: Adam J. Berinsky, Tesalia Rizzo, Leah R. Rosenzweig, Elisha Heaps

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 3/2020

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

We examine the extent to which relevant social identity traits shared between two individuals—what we term “attribute affinity”—can moderate out-group hostility. We argue that in-group affinity is a powerful force in shaping preferences over potential immigrants. We focus on two closely related, yet distinct, dimensions of identity: religion and religiosity. Using evidence from three surveys that included two embedded experiments, we show that sharing strength in religious practice can diminish strong aversion to immigrants of different religious affiliations. We find that, among highly religious U.S. natives, anti-Muslim bias is lower toward very religious Muslims, compared to non-religious Muslims. This attenuating effect of attribute affinity with respect to religiosity on anti-Muslim bias presents the strongest evidence supporting our argument.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
In fact, irrespective of other abilities, most natives prefer potential immigrants with high skill levels (using level of education as a proxy for skill). These studies find that highly skilled immigrants are preferred to low-skilled immigrants, regardless of a native’s own skill level, suggesting that this preference is not driven by different levels of threat perception about one’s own job.
 
2
Political scientists also incorporate social identity into studies of public opinion. Most notably, Kinder and Kam (2009) focus on what they argue is a tendency to “divide the world into in groups and out groups” or ethnocentrism (76). For instance, the authors argue that high levels of ethnocentrism among U.S. natives drive negative attitudes toward immigrants.
 
3
Although drawing from common in-group identity, our argument is not about any aspect of identity becoming superordinate or recategorized – instead we suggest that simply the recognition of shared characteristics on one dimension may override aversion along another.
 
4
In this study, we use frequency of attendance at religious services as a proxy for religiosity. To test its validity, we correlated frequency of attendance with other measures of religiosity. See footnote 19 for details.
 
5
Though, as we note below, such a pattern does not hold for individuals from “outsider” religions, such as Muslims in the present day.
 
6
While attitudes toward Muslims tend to differ by political affiliation, Americans on both sides of the aisle support keeping Muslims out of the country or subjecting them to additional surveillance (Sides and Mogahed 2018). The same survey evidence suggests that Americans view Muslims as religious and believe they hold outdated views of women, gays, and lesbians.
 
7
Attitudes toward Muslims have likely been influenced by the September 11, 2001 events and the resulting portrayal of Muslims in the media, where they are frequently associated with threatening images of terrorists (Cimino 2005; Oswald 2005; Putnam 2002; Schildkraut 2002; Shaheen 2003). It should be noted that Creighton and Jamal (2015) find that U.S. natives are equally opposed to Muslim and Christian immigrants. While U.S. Christians are explicitly biased against Muslim immigrants, they also hold implicit biases against Christian immigrants of specific nationalities and ethnicities.
 
8
YouGov interviewed 1089 respondents from May 2 to May 6, 2013. Then, respondents were matched down to a sample of 1000 to produce the final dataset. The respondents were matched on gender, age, race, education, party identification, ideology, and political interest. Using the 2010 American Community Survey, YouGov then weighted the matched set of survey respondents to known characteristics of the general population of the U.S.
 
9
We emphasized that we were solely concerned with legal immigrants to rule out natives’ attitudes toward illegal immigrants, which may be driven by other socially undesirable characteristics (Wright et al. 2015).
 
10
We oversampled religious respondents on MTurk using contacts from previous studies on religion because, in general, respondents on MTurk are less likely to be affiliated with a religion than the overall United States population (Berinsky et al. 2012).
 
11
SSI recruits participants through various online communities, social networks, and website advertisements. SSI makes efforts to recruit hard-to-reach groups, such as ethnic minorities and seniors. These potential participants are then screened and invited into the panel. When deploying a particular survey, SSI randomly selects panel participants for survey invitations. We did not employ quotas but asked SSI to recruit a target population that matched the (18 and over) 2010 census population on education, gender, age, geography, and income (based on the premeasured profile characteristics of the respondents). The resulting sample is not a probability sample but a diverse national sample. SSI samples have been used in a number of recent publications in political science.
 
12
Hainmueller et al. (2015) test several vignette experimental designs and find advantages to both measures, both in terms of their accuracy in calculating effects and predicting real life outcomes.
 
13
We selected major world religions: Christianity (Catholic and Protestant denominations), Islam, and Hinduism. We also included atheists and Jews because we wanted enough variation in our variable of interest and, particularly, enough variation in religious affiliation for each country of origin in our experiment.
 
14
Although we assigned the attribute-levels at random, we included one restriction over the possible profiles that the respondents saw. “Atheist” is one of the levels of the religion attribute and it is unrealistic to present respondents with atheist immigrants who attended religious services. Therefore, we restricted the possible combinations of immigrants such that atheist immigrants would always “Never” attend religious services. Hainmueller et al. (2014) state that the randomization of profiles (an identification assumption in conjoint analysis), requires that the randomization scheme assign a non-zero probability to all the possible attribute combinations for which the potential outcomes are defined. Because the control observation for each attribute-level is all other attribute-levels, removing theoretically problematic combinations makes it impossible to analyze causal quantities for these attribute-levels. We are not particularly concerned, as these combinations are not crucial for our results.
 
15
Immigrants’ religions were: Protestant, Catholic, Mormon, Jewish, Muslim, atheist/agnostic, Hindu or Buddhist, and Eastern or Greek Orthodox.
 
16
Psychologists have long known that individuals often fail to accurately report their underlying mental processes (Nisbett and Wilson 1977). When individuals are asked to report their cognitive processes, they often do so on the basis of a priori judgments. Our results demonstrate that irrespective of the stated importance of religion, respondents’ own religion greatly influenced their preferences.
 
17
Because our treatment and outcome variables are binary (either an immigrant has or does not have an attribute and a respondent either accepts one or the other immigrant), the linear regression estimator is fully nonparametric.
 
18
The relatively small sample size of United States respondents identifying with the smaller religions randomly assigned to the immigrant profiles (Hinduism, Islam) prevent us from drawing inferences about these groups.
 
19
We additionally tested whether our measure of religiosity (attendance at religious services) corresponds to the salience of religiosity as an identity. In our YouGov—religion survey, we asked respondents how important religion is in their lives (“Extremely important”,“Very important”,“Somewhat important”,“Not very important”,“Not at all important”). Figure 1 in the Supporting Information shows that our measure of religiosity is highly correlated with self-reported levels of importance of religion, suggesting that our measure is capturing both the strength and salience of religiosity.
 
20
We exclude atheists from both the respondent sample and immigrant profiles since they are not considered religious people. However, the results do not change if atheists are included.
 
21
We ran another similar experiment in 2015 where we included an additional treatment arm that presented respondents with a Muslim immigrant without any information about religiosity. The experiment presented respondents with two immigrant profiles sequentially. We managed to replicate our results as we did with the conjoint analysis only in the first period, not the second. We find that, in line with our theory of attribute affinity, religious respondents prefer religious over non-religious Muslims. However, we are careful in interpreting these results because of their instability (see Supporting Information for details). Although we cannot explain this instability, we suspect that events relating to Islamic extremism that occurred between our experiments, such as the Charlie Hebdo attacks and the increased media coverage of the Islamic State, may have influenced our respondents’ answers in ways we did not anticipate. However, given the strong findings from our first two experiments—conducted using different methods on different samples—we are confident that the initial results we report in this paper are not simply statistical flukes.
 
22
The findings in this sample cannot rule out that this pattern is particular to religious Catholic respondents because in the full sample of religious respondents there is a slight preference for religious Muslims over non-religious Muslim immigrants (difference in means of 0.06, p-value = 0.86), but the difference is not statistically distinguishable from zero.
 
23
Similar to the MTurk–religion/religiosity experiment, we asked respondents to rate each immigrant on a 7-point scale (see Fig. 1). Including atheists that report attending religious services does not change the results.
 
24
The difference in means between religious respondents’ evaluations of religious and non-religious Muslims is practically the same size as when measured using a 7 point scale rating dependent variable and is significant at \(p < 0.08\).
 
25
Instead of seven pairs, we showed 10 pairs of profiles to respondents of minority religions such as Mormons, Greek Orthodox, Jews, Muslims, Buddhists, Hindus, and atheists. Therefore, when we pool all religious respondents, we are in fact over-representing these religions relative to their population size. We ran the analyses both with all profiles and as well as with just the first seven that each respondent saw. The results do not change with the modification in the sample size.
 
26
We designed the SSI–conjoint experiment in 2014 to specifically test our theory on religion and religiosity, but we also use the data to test the relevance of attribute affinity with respect to other interactive aspects of identity-specifically, between race on the one hand and language skills on the other. We find that although the statistical significance fails to reach standard levels, attribute affinity mitigates negative reactions in these cases. Although only suggestive, this evidence further strengthens our theory and emphasizes the importance of studying the effect of the interaction of different identity attributes on attitudes toward out-groups.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Adida, C. L., Laitin, D. D., & Valfort, M.-A. (2015). Religious homophily in a secular country: Evidence from a voting game in France. Economic Inquiry, 53(2), 1187–1206. Adida, C. L., Laitin, D. D., & Valfort, M.-A. (2015). Religious homophily in a secular country: Evidence from a voting game in France. Economic Inquiry, 53(2), 1187–1206.
Zurück zum Zitat Adida, C. L., Laitin, D. D., & Valfort, M.-A. (2016). Why Muslim integration fails in Christian-heritage societies. Harvard: Harvard University Press. Adida, C. L., Laitin, D. D., & Valfort, M.-A. (2016). Why Muslim integration fails in Christian-heritage societies. Harvard: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Bloom, B.-N., Pazit, G. A., & Courtemanche, M. (2015). Religious social identity, religious belief, and anti-immigration sentiment. American Political Science Review, 109(2), 203–221. Bloom, B.-N., Pazit, G. A., & Courtemanche, M. (2015). Religious social identity, religious belief, and anti-immigration sentiment. American Political Science Review, 109(2), 203–221.
Zurück zum Zitat Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s mechanical turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351–368. Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s mechanical turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351–368.
Zurück zum Zitat Bobo, L. (1983). Whites’ opposition to busing: Symbolic racism or realistic group conflict? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(6), 1196–1210. Bobo, L. (1983). Whites’ opposition to busing: Symbolic racism or realistic group conflict? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(6), 1196–1210.
Zurück zum Zitat Brader, T., Valentino, N. A., & Suhay, E. (2008). What triggers public opposition to immigration? Anxiety, group cues, and immigration threat. American Journal of Political Science, 52(4), 959–978. Brader, T., Valentino, N. A., & Suhay, E. (2008). What triggers public opposition to immigration? Anxiety, group cues, and immigration threat. American Journal of Political Science, 52(4), 959–978.
Zurück zum Zitat Brewer, M. B. (1979). In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A cognitive-motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 307–324. Brewer, M. B. (1979). In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A cognitive-motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 307–324.
Zurück zum Zitat Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 429–444. Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 429–444.
Zurück zum Zitat Brewer, M. B. (2007). The importance of being we: Human nature and intergroup relations. American Psychologist, 62(8), 728. Brewer, M. B. (2007). The importance of being we: Human nature and intergroup relations. American Psychologist, 62(8), 728.
Zurück zum Zitat Chandra, K. (2012). Constructivist theories of ethnic politics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Chandra, K. (2012). Constructivist theories of ethnic politics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Cimino, R. (2005). No god in common: American evangelical discourse on Islam after 9/11. Review of Religious Research, 47(2), 162–174. Cimino, R. (2005). No god in common: American evangelical discourse on Islam after 9/11. Review of Religious Research, 47(2), 162–174.
Zurück zum Zitat Citrin, J., Green, D. P., Muste, C., & Wong, C. (1997). Public opinion toward immigration reform: The role of economic motivations. The Journal of Politics, 59(3), 858–881. Citrin, J., Green, D. P., Muste, C., & Wong, C. (1997). Public opinion toward immigration reform: The role of economic motivations. The Journal of Politics, 59(3), 858–881.
Zurück zum Zitat Citrin, J., & Wright, M. (2009). Defining the circle of we: American identity and immigration policy. The Forum, 7(3), 1–20. Citrin, J., & Wright, M. (2009). Defining the circle of we: American identity and immigration policy. The Forum, 7(3), 1–20.
Zurück zum Zitat Creighton, M. J., & Jamal, A. (2015). Does Islam play a role in anti-immigrant sentiment? An experimental approach. Social Science Research, 53, 89–103. Creighton, M. J., & Jamal, A. (2015). Does Islam play a role in anti-immigrant sentiment? An experimental approach. Social Science Research, 53, 89–103.
Zurück zum Zitat Crisp, R. J., & Hewstone, M. (2007). Multiple social categorization. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 163–254. Crisp, R. J., & Hewstone, M. (2007). Multiple social categorization. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 163–254.
Zurück zum Zitat Dancygier, R. M., & Donnelly, M. J. (2013). Sectoral economies, economic contexts, and attitudes toward immigration. The Journal of Politics, 75(1), 17–35. Dancygier, R. M., & Donnelly, M. J. (2013). Sectoral economies, economic contexts, and attitudes toward immigration. The Journal of Politics, 75(1), 17–35.
Zurück zum Zitat Daniels, J. P., & Von Der Ruhr, M. (2005). God and the global economy: Religion and attitudes towards trade and immigration in the United States. Socio-Economic Review, 3(3), 467–489. Daniels, J. P., & Von Der Ruhr, M. (2005). God and the global economy: Religion and attitudes towards trade and immigration in the United States. Socio-Economic Review, 3(3), 467–489.
Zurück zum Zitat Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Validzic, A., Matoka, K., Johnson, B., & Frazier, S. (1997). Extending the benefits of recategorization: Evaluations, self-disclosure, and helping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(4), 401–420. Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Validzic, A., Matoka, K., Johnson, B., & Frazier, S. (1997). Extending the benefits of recategorization: Evaluations, self-disclosure, and helping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(4), 401–420.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunning, T., & Harrison, L. (2010). Cross-cutting cleavages and ethnic voting: An experimental study of cousinage in Mali. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 21–39. Dunning, T., & Harrison, L. (2010). Cross-cutting cleavages and ethnic voting: An experimental study of cousinage in Mali. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 21–39.
Zurück zum Zitat Fetzer, J. S. J., & Soper, C. (2003). The roots of public attitudes toward state accommodation of European Muslims’ religious practices before and after September 11. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 42(2), 247–258. Fetzer, J. S. J., & Soper, C. (2003). The roots of public attitudes toward state accommodation of European Muslims’ religious practices before and after September 11. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 42(2), 247–258.
Zurück zum Zitat Foner, N., & Alba, R. (2008). Immigrant religion in the US and Western Europe: Bridge or barrier to inclusion? International Migration Review, 42(2), 360–392. Foner, N., & Alba, R. (2008). Immigrant religion in the US and Western Europe: Bridge or barrier to inclusion? International Migration Review, 42(2), 360–392.
Zurück zum Zitat Ford, R, Morrell, G., & Heath, A. (2012). Fewer but better? Public views about immigration. The British Social Attitudes, 26–44. Ford, R, Morrell, G., & Heath, A. (2012). Fewer but better? Public views about immigration. The British Social Attitudes, 26–44.
Zurück zum Zitat Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup identity model. London: Psychology Press. Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup identity model. London: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Green, J. C. (2007). The faith factor: How religion influences American elections. Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing Group. Green, J. C. (2007). The faith factor: How religion influences American elections. Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing Group.
Zurück zum Zitat Guth, J. L., & Green, J. C. (1993). Salience: The core concept. In D. C. Leege & L. A. Kellstadt (Eds.), Rediscovering the religious factor in American politics (pp. 157–174). Armonk, NY: ME Sharpe. Guth, J. L., & Green, J. C. (1993). Salience: The core concept. In D. C. Leege & L. A. Kellstadt (Eds.), Rediscovering the religious factor in American politics (pp. 157–174). Armonk, NY: ME Sharpe.
Zurück zum Zitat Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2014). Causal inference in conjoint analysis: Understanding multidimensional choices via stated preference experiments. Political Analysis, 22(1), 1–30. Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2014). Causal inference in conjoint analysis: Understanding multidimensional choices via stated preference experiments. Political Analysis, 22(1), 1–30.
Zurück zum Zitat Hainmueller, Jens, Hangartner, Dominik, & Yamamoto, Teppei. (2015). Validating vignette and conjoint survey experiments against real-world behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences United States of Americs, 112(8), 2395–2400. Hainmueller, Jens, Hangartner, Dominik, & Yamamoto, Teppei. (2015). Validating vignette and conjoint survey experiments against real-world behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences United States of Americs, 112(8), 2395–2400.
Zurück zum Zitat Hainmueller, J., & Hiscox, M. J. (2010). Attitudes toward highly skilled and low-skilled immigration: Evidence from a survey experiment. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 61–84. Hainmueller, J., & Hiscox, M. J. (2010). Attitudes toward highly skilled and low-skilled immigration: Evidence from a survey experiment. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 61–84.
Zurück zum Zitat Hainmueller, J., Hiscox, M. J., & Margalit, Y. (2015). Do concerns about labor market competition shape attitudes toward immigration? New evidence. Journal of International Economics, 97(1), 193–207. Hainmueller, J., Hiscox, M. J., & Margalit, Y. (2015). Do concerns about labor market competition shape attitudes toward immigration? New evidence. Journal of International Economics, 97(1), 193–207.
Zurück zum Zitat Hanson, G. H., Scheve, K., & Slaughter, M. J. (2007). Public finance and individual preferences over globalization strategies. Economics & Politics, 19(1), 1–33. Hanson, G. H., Scheve, K., & Slaughter, M. J. (2007). Public finance and individual preferences over globalization strategies. Economics & Politics, 19(1), 1–33.
Zurück zum Zitat Kalkan, O. K., Layman, G. C., & Uslaner, E. M. (2009). Bands of others? Attitudes toward Muslims in contemporary American society. The Journal of Politics, 71(3), 847–862. Kalkan, O. K., Layman, G. C., & Uslaner, E. M. (2009). Bands of others? Attitudes toward Muslims in contemporary American society. The Journal of Politics, 71(3), 847–862.
Zurück zum Zitat Kenworthy, J. B., Canales, C. J., Weaver, K. D., & Miller, N. (2003). Negative incidental affect and mood congruency in crossed categorization. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(3), 195–219. Kenworthy, J. B., Canales, C. J., Weaver, K. D., & Miller, N. (2003). Negative incidental affect and mood congruency in crossed categorization. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(3), 195–219.
Zurück zum Zitat Kinder, D. R., & Kam, C. D. (2009). Us against them: The ethnocentric foundations of American opinion. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Kinder, D. R., & Kam, C. D. (2009). Us against them: The ethnocentric foundations of American opinion. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Kinder, D. R., & Sears, D. O. (1981). Prejudice and politics: Symbolic racism versus racial threats to the good life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(3), 414. Kinder, D. R., & Sears, D. O. (1981). Prejudice and politics: Symbolic racism versus racial threats to the good life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(3), 414.
Zurück zum Zitat Knoll, B. R. (2009). And who is my neighbor? Religion and immigration policy attitudes. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 48(2), 313–331. Knoll, B. R. (2009). And who is my neighbor? Religion and immigration policy attitudes. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 48(2), 313–331.
Zurück zum Zitat Layman, G. C. (1997). Religion and political behavior in the United States: The impact of beliefs, affiliations, and commitment from 1980 to 1994. Public Opinion Quarterly, 61(2), 288–316. Layman, G. C. (1997). Religion and political behavior in the United States: The impact of beliefs, affiliations, and commitment from 1980 to 1994. Public Opinion Quarterly, 61(2), 288–316.
Zurück zum Zitat Malhotra, N., Margalit, Y. M., & Mo, C. H. (2013). Economic explanations for opposition to immigration: Distinguishing between prevalence and conditional impact. American Journal of Political Science, 57(2), 391–410. Malhotra, N., Margalit, Y. M., & Mo, C. H. (2013). Economic explanations for opposition to immigration: Distinguishing between prevalence and conditional impact. American Journal of Political Science, 57(2), 391–410.
Zurück zum Zitat Margolis, M. F. (2018). How far does social group influence reach? Identities, elites, and immigration attitudes. The Journal of Politics, 80(3), 000–000. Margolis, M. F. (2018). How far does social group influence reach? Identities, elites, and immigration attitudes. The Journal of Politics, 80(3), 000–000.
Zurück zum Zitat Mayda, A. M. (2006). Who is immigration? A cross-country investigation of individual attitudes toward immigrants. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 88(3), 510–530. Mayda, A. M. (2006). Who is immigration? A cross-country investigation of individual attitudes toward immigrants. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 88(3), 510–530.
Zurück zum Zitat McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27(1), 415–444. McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27(1), 415–444.
Zurück zum Zitat Mockabee, S. T., Monson, J. Q., & Grant, J. T. (2001). Measuring religious commitment among Catholics and Protestants: A new approach. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 40(4), 675–690. Mockabee, S. T., Monson, J. Q., & Grant, J. T. (2001). Measuring religious commitment among Catholics and Protestants: A new approach. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 40(4), 675–690.
Zurück zum Zitat Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Rourke, K. H., & Sinnott, R. (2006). The determinants of individual attitudes towards immigration. European Journal of Political Economy, 22(4), 838–861. O’Rourke, K. H., & Sinnott, R. (2006). The determinants of individual attitudes towards immigration. European Journal of Political Economy, 22(4), 838–861.
Zurück zum Zitat Oswald, D. L. (2005). Understanding anti-arab reactions post-9/11: The role of threats, social categories, and personal ideologies. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35(9), 1775–1799. Oswald, D. L. (2005). Understanding anti-arab reactions post-9/11: The role of threats, social categories, and personal ideologies. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35(9), 1775–1799.
Zurück zum Zitat Putnam, R. D. (2002). Bowling together. The American Prospect, 13(3), 20–22. Putnam, R. D. (2002). Bowling together. The American Prospect, 13(3), 20–22.
Zurück zum Zitat Riek, B. M., Mania, E. W., Gaertner, S. L., McDonald, S. A., & Lamoreaux, M. J. (2010). Does a common ingroup identity reduce intergroup threat? Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(4), 403–423. Riek, B. M., Mania, E. W., Gaertner, S. L., McDonald, S. A., & Lamoreaux, M. J. (2010). Does a common ingroup identity reduce intergroup threat? Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(4), 403–423.
Zurück zum Zitat Scheve, K. F., & Slaughter, M. J. (2001). Labor market competition and individual preferences over immigration policy. Review of Economics and Statistics, 83(1), 133–145. Scheve, K. F., & Slaughter, M. J. (2001). Labor market competition and individual preferences over immigration policy. Review of Economics and Statistics, 83(1), 133–145.
Zurück zum Zitat Schildkraut, D. J. (2002). The more things change... American identity and mass and elite responses to 9/11. Political Psychology, 23(3), 511–535. Schildkraut, D. J. (2002). The more things change... American identity and mass and elite responses to 9/11. Political Psychology, 23(3), 511–535.
Zurück zum Zitat Sears, D. O., Sidanius, J., & Bobo, L. (2000). Racialized politics: The debate about racism in America. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Sears, D. O., Sidanius, J., & Bobo, L. (2000). Racialized politics: The debate about racism in America. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaheen, J. G. (2003). Reel bad arabs: How hollywood vilifies a people. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 588(1), 171–193. Shaheen, J. G. (2003). Reel bad arabs: How hollywood vilifies a people. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 588(1), 171–193.
Zurück zum Zitat Sherif, M. (1966). Group conflict and co-operation: Their social psychology. London: Routledge & K. Paul. Sherif, M. (1966). Group conflict and co-operation: Their social psychology. London: Routledge & K. Paul.
Zurück zum Zitat Sides, J., & D, Mogahed. (2018). Muslims in America: Public perceptions in the Trump Era. Washington, DC: Arab American Institute. Sides, J., & D, Mogahed. (2018). Muslims in America: Public perceptions in the Trump Era. Washington, DC: Arab American Institute.
Zurück zum Zitat Sniderman, P. M., & Hagendoorn, A. (2007). When ways of life collide: Multiculturalism and its discontents in the Netherlands. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Sniderman, P. M., & Hagendoorn, A. (2007). When ways of life collide: Multiculturalism and its discontents in the Netherlands. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Strabac, Z., & Listhaug, O. (2008). Anti-Muslim prejudice in Europe: A multilevel analysis of survey data from 30 countries. Social Science Research, 37(1), 268–286. Strabac, Z., & Listhaug, O. (2008). Anti-Muslim prejudice in Europe: A multilevel analysis of survey data from 30 countries. Social Science Research, 37(1), 268–286.
Zurück zum Zitat Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33(1), 1–39. Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33(1), 1–39.
Zurück zum Zitat Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–37). Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–37). Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole.
Zurück zum Zitat Vanman, E. J., Paul, B. Y., Ito, T. A., & Miller, N. (1997). The modern face of prejudice and structural features that moderate the effect of cooperation on affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(5), 941–959. Vanman, E. J., Paul, B. Y., Ito, T. A., & Miller, N. (1997). The modern face of prejudice and structural features that moderate the effect of cooperation on affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(5), 941–959.
Zurück zum Zitat Worchel, S., Rothgerber, H., Day, E. A., Hart, D., & Butemeyer, J. (1998). Social and identity and individual productivity within groups. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 37(4), 389–413. Worchel, S., Rothgerber, H., Day, E. A., Hart, D., & Butemeyer, J. (1998). Social and identity and individual productivity within groups. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 37(4), 389–413.
Zurück zum Zitat Wright, M., Levy, M., & Citrin, J. (2016). Public attitudes toward immigration policy across the legal/illegal divide: The role of categorical and attribute-based decision-making. Political Behavior, 38(1), 229–253. Wright, M., Levy, M., & Citrin, J. (2016). Public attitudes toward immigration policy across the legal/illegal divide: The role of categorical and attribute-based decision-making. Political Behavior, 38(1), 229–253.
Metadaten
Titel
Attribute Affinity: U.S. Natives’ Attitudes Toward Immigrants
verfasst von
Adam J. Berinsky
Tesalia Rizzo
Leah R. Rosenzweig
Elisha Heaps
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2018
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 3/2020
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9518-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2020

Political Behavior 3/2020 Zur Ausgabe