Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Awareness of a gender stereotype, personal beliefs and self-perceptions regarding math ability: when boys do not surpass girls

  • Published:
Social Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The negative reputation of women in mathematics and its consequences on their self-perceptions have been extensively demonstrated. However, in France and other countries, the younger the students, the less pronounced these gender differences are. The focus of this study was to explore whether children of two age groups (fourth graders and seventh graders) are aware of a math-ability gender stereotype favorable to boys, and to determine their personal beliefs on mathematics ability. The link between this gender stereotype and self-perceptions was also examined. As expected, there was not a clear-cut awareness of a math-ability gender stereotype favorable to boys. More surprising, girls in both age groups and seventh-grade boys believed that girls do better than boys. Moreover, when their gender identity was made salient, the boys who believed in girl superiority perceived their own performance in mathematics as lower. The girls, on the other hand, regardless of their age and stereotype awareness or personal beliefs, perceived their performance in math as higher when their gender identity was made salient than when it was not.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abrams D., Sparkes K., Hogg M.A. (1985). Gender salience and social identity: The impact of sex of siblings on educational and occupational aspirations. British Journal of Educational Psychology 55, 224–232

    Google Scholar 

  • Abrams D., Thomas J., Hogg M.A. (1990). Numerical distinctiveness, social identity and gender salience. British Journal of Social Psychology 29, 87–92

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport G.W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Addison-Wesley, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambady N., Shih M., Kim A. (2001). Stereotype susceptibility in children: Effects of identity activation on quantitative performance. Psychological Science 12, 385–390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura A. (1982). The self and mechanisms of agency. In: Suls J. (eds). Psychological perspectives on the self Vol. 1. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 3–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action : A social cognitive theory. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Benassi V., Lane A., Lane J. (2001). Self-efficacy and academic performance. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal 29, 687–693

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beyer S. (1999). The accuracy of academic gender stereotypes. Sex Roles 40, 787–813

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanton H., Christie C., Dye M. (2002). Social identity versus reference frame comparisons: The moderating role of stereotype endorsement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38, 253–267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block J.H. (1976). Issues, problems, and pitfalls in assessing sex differences: A critical review of ‘The Psychology of Sex Differences.’ Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 22, 283–308

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown R.P., Pinel E.C. (2003). Stigma on my mind: Individual differences in the experience of stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39, 626–633

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bussey K., Bandura A. (1992). Self-regulatory mechanisms governing gender development. Child Development 63, 1236–1250

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chatard, A., Guimond, S., & Selimbegovic, L. (2006). “How good are you in math?” The effect of gender stereotypes on students’ recollection of their school marks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.10.024.

  • Crain R.M. (1996). The influence of age, race, and gender on child and adolescent multidimensional self-concept. In: Bracken B.A. (eds). Handbook of self-concept: Developmental, social and clinical considerations. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 240–280

    Google Scholar 

  • Deaux K. (1985). Sex and gender. Annual Review of Psychology 36, 49–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Devine P.G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56, 5–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Devine P.G., Elliot A.J. (1995). Are racial stereotypes really fading? The Princeton trilogy revisited. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21, 1139–1150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly A.H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles J.S., Adler T.F., Futterman R., Goff S.B., Kaczala C.M., Meece J., Midgley C. (1983). Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors. In: Spence J.T. (eds). Achievement and achievement motives. Freeman, San Francisco, pp. 75–146

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles J.S., Wigfield A., Harold R.D., Blumenfeld P.C. (1993). Age and gender differences in children’s achievement self-perceptions during elementary school years. Child Development 64, 830–847

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan X., Chen M., Matsumoto A.R. (1997). Gender differences in mathematics achievement: Findings from the National Education Longitudinal Study of 1988. Journal of Experimental Education 65, 229–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guimond S., Chatard A., Martinot D., Crisp R., Redersdorff S. (2006). Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differences in self-construals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90, 221–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guimond S., Roussel L. (2001). Bragging about one’s school grades: Gender stereotyping and students’ perception of their abilities in science, mathematics and language. Social Psychology of Education 4, 275–293

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harter S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. The Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Herbert J., Stipek D. (2005). The emergence of gender differences in children’s perceptions of their academic competence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 26, 276–295

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill J.P., Lynch M.E. (1983). The intensification of gender related role expectations during early adolescence. In: Brooks-Gunn J., Peterson A.C. (eds). Girls at puberty. Plenum Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman C., Hurst N. (1990). Gender stereotypes: Perception or rationalization? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58, 197–208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huston A.C. (1987). The development of sex typing: Themes from recent research. In: Chess S., Thomas A. (eds). Annual progress in child psychiatry and child development. Brunner/Mazel, Philadelphia, pp. 168–186

    Google Scholar 

  • Inzlicht M., Ben-Zeev T. (2000). A threatening intellectual environment: Why females are susceptible to experiencing problem-solving deficits in the presence of males?. Psychological Science 11, 365–371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs J.E., Lanza S., Osgood D.W., Eccles J.S., Wigfield A. (2002). Changes in children’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Development 73, 509–527

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost J.T., Banaji M.R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system justification and the production of false consciousness. British Journal of Social Psychology 33, 1–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd C.M., Ryan C.S., Park B. (1991). Accuracy in the judgment of in-group and out-group variability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61, 366–379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jussim L. (1991). Social perception and social reality: A reflection-construction model. Psychological Review 98, 54–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski D., Sheridan E.M. (1984). Children’s perceptions of sex stereotyping: A five-year study. International Journal of Women’s Studies 7, 24–36

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz P.A., Kofkin J.A. (1997). Race, gender, and young children. In: Luthar S.S., Burack J.A., Cicchetti D., Weisz J.R. (eds). Developmental psychopathology: Perspectives on adjustment, risk, and disorder. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 51–74

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller J. (2002). Blatant stereotype threat and women’s math performance: Self-handicapping as a strategic means to cope with obtrusive negative performance expectations. Sex Roles 47, 193–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leyens J.-Ph., Désert M., Croizet J.C., Darcis C. (2000). Stereotype threat: are lower status and history of stigmatization preconditions of stereotype threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26, 1189–1199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenzi-Cioldi, F., & Doise, W. (1994). Identité sociale et identité personnelle. In R. Y. Bourhis & J. Ph. Leyens (Eds.), Stéréotypes, discrimination et relations entre groupes (pp. 69–96). Liège: Mardaga.

  • Maccoby E.E., Jacklin C.N. (1987). Gender segregation in childhood. Advances in Child Development and Behaviour 20, 239–287

    Google Scholar 

  • Marsh H.W., Yeung A.S. (1998). Longitudinal structural equation models of academic self-concept and achievement: Gender differences in the development of math and English constructs. American Educational Research Journal 35, 705–738

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKown C., Weinstein R.S. (2003). The development and consequences of stereotype consciousness in middle childhood. Child Development 74, 498–515

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ministère de l’Education Nationale (MEN) (2004). http://www.education.gouv.fr/stateval/

  • Nicholls J.G., Miller A.T. (1984). Reasoning about the ability of self and others: A developmental study. Child Development 55, 1990–1999

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development, Programme for International Student Assessment (OECD PISA) (2001 and 2003). http://www.pisa.oecd.org

  • Pierrehumbert B., Plancherel B., Jankech-Carreta C. (1987). Image de soi et perception des compétences propre chez l’enfant. Revue de Psychologie Appliquée 37, 359–377

    Google Scholar 

  • Powlishta K.K. (1995). Gender bias in children’s perceptions of personality traits. Sex Roles 32, 17–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn D.M., Spencer S.J. (2001). The interference of stereotype threat with women’s generation of mathematical problem-solving strategies. Journal of Social Issues, Special Issue: Stigma: An Insider’s Perspective 57(1): 55–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Quintana S.M. (1994). A model of ethnic perspective-taking ability applied to Mexican-American children and youth. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 18, 419–448

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quintana S.M. (1998). Children’s developmental understanding of ethnicity and race. Applied & Preventive Psychology 7, 27–45

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal R., Jacobson L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom: Teacher expectation and pupils’ intellectual development. Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), W. Damon (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 993–1016). New York: Wiley.

  • Schmader T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38, 194–201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmader T., Johns M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85, 440–452

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmader T., Johns M., Barquissau M. (2004). The costs of accepting gender differences: the role of stereotype endorsement in women’s experience in the math domain. Sex Roles 50, 835–850

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Selimbegovic, L., Chatard, A., & Mugny, G. (in press). Can we encourage girls’ mobility towards science-related careers? Disconfirming stereotype belief through expert influence. European Journal of Psychology of Education.

  • Shih M., Pittinsky T.L., Ambady N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: Identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Science 10, 80–83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith J.L., White P.H. (2002). An examination of implicitly activated, explicitly activated, and nullified stereotypes on mathematical performance: It’s not just a woman’s issue. Sex Roles 47, 179–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith J.L., Morgan C.L., White P.H. (2005). Investigating a measure of computer technology domain identification: A tool for understanding gender differences and stereotypes. Educational and Psychological Measurement 65, 336–355

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer S., Steele C.M., Quinn D.M. (1999). Under suspicion of inability: Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 35, 4–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele J. (2003). Children’s gender stereotypes about math: The role of stereotype stratification. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 33, 2587–2606

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele C.M., Aronson J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69, 797–811

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele C.M., Spencer S.J., Aronson J. (2002). Contending with group image : The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. In: Zanna M.P. (eds). Advances in experimental social psychology Vol. 34. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 379–440

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek D.J. (1984). The development of achievement motivation. In: Ames C., Ames R. (eds). Research on motivation and education: Student motivation Vol. 1. Academic, San Diego, CA, pp. 145–174

    Google Scholar 

  • Swim J.K. (1994). Perceived versus meta-analytic effect sizes: An assessment of the accuracy of gender stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66, 21–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh M., Hickey C., Duffy J. (1999). Influence of item content and stereotype situation on gender differences in mathematical problem solving. Sex Roles 41, 219–240

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yee M., Brown R. (1994). The development of gender differentiation in young children. British Journal of Social Psychology 33, 183–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeldin A.L., Pajares F. (2000). Against the odds: Self-efficacy beliefs of women in mathematical, scientific, and technological careers. American Educational Research Journal 37, 215–246

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Delphine Martinot.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Martinot, D., Désert, M. Awareness of a gender stereotype, personal beliefs and self-perceptions regarding math ability: when boys do not surpass girls. Soc Psychol Educ 10, 455–471 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-007-9028-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-007-9028-9

Keywords

Navigation