Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

How important are student-selected versus instructor-selected literature resources for students’ learning and motivation in problem-based learning?

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In problem-based learning students are responsible for their own learning process, which becomes evident when they must act independently, for example, when selecting literature resources for individual study. It is a matter of debate whether it is better to have students select their own literature resources or to present them with a list of mandatory instructor-selected literature resources. The current study investigated the effect of using instructor-selected literature resources or student-selected literature resources (from a predetermined set of literature) on several learning outcome variables. The results demonstrated that students in the student-selected literature condition scored higher on autonomous motivation and perceived competence, and lower on perceptions of mental effort during studying. Students in the instructor-selected condition had better test performance on factual test items, whereas no difference was found for the conceptual questions. Overall, the results indicate that letting students select their own literature resources can be beneficial in terms of autonomous motivation, perceived competence, and perceptions of mental effort invested during learning and does not differentially affect conceptual knowledge.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Results of all analyses were the same with and without including these participants.

  2. Results before removal of outliers: Autonomous motivation, t(58) = 2.32, p = 0.02; Controlled motivation, t(58) = −0.24, p = 0.82.

  3. Results before removal of outliers: Hotelling’s T² = 0.01, F(2, 57) < 1.

References

  • Barrows, H. S. (1996). Problem-based learning in medicine and beyond: A brief overview. In L. Wilkerson & W. H. Gijselaers (Eds.), Bringing problem-based learning to higher education: Theory and practice. New directions in teaching and learning (Vol. 68, pp. 3–12). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. doi:10.1002/tl.37219966804.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, A. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The effects of instructors’ autonomy support and students’ autonomous motivation on learning organic chemistry: A self-determination theory perspective. Science Education, 84, 740–756. doi:10.1002/1098-237X(200011)84:6<740:AID-SCE4>3.0.CO;2-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Candy, P. C. (1991). Self-direction for lifelong learning: A comprehensive guide to theory and practice. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahlgren, M. A., & Dahlgren, L. O. (2002). Portraits of PBL: Students’ experiences of the characteristics of problem-based learning in physiotherapy, computer engineering and psychology. Instructional Science, 30, 111–127. doi:10.1023/A:1014819418051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Grave, W. S., Dolmans, D. H. J. M., & Van der Vleuten, C. P. M. (2002). Student perspectives on critical incidents in the tutorial group. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 7, 201–209. doi:10.1023/A:1021104201303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Self-determination theory: A macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Canadian Psychology, 49, 182–185. doi:10.1037/a0012801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dochy, F., Segers, M., Van den Bossche, P., & Gijbels, D. (2003). Effects of problem-based learning: A meta-analysis. Learning and Instruction, 13, 533–568. doi:10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00025-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gijbels, D., Dochy, F., Van den Bossche, P., & Segers, M. (2005). Effects of problem-based learning: A meta-analysis from the angle of assessment. Review of Educational Research, 75, 27–61. doi:10.3102/00346543075001027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeong, H., & Hmelo-Silver, C. E. (2010). Productive use of learning resources in an online problem-based learning environment. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 84–99. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2009.08.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, I., & Assor, A. (2007). When choice motivates and when it does not. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 429–442. doi:10.1007/s10648-006-9027-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krathwohl, D. R. (2002). A revision of Bloom’s taxonomy: An overview. Theory Into Practice, 41, 212–218. doi:10.1207/s15430421tip4104_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loyens, S.M.M., Kirschner, P.A., & Paas, F. (2012). Problem-based learning. In K. R. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA Educational Psychology Handbook: Application to learning and teaching (Vol. 3, pp. 403–425). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/13725-016.

  • Loyens, S. M. M., Rikers, R. M. J. P., & Schmidt, H. G. (2007a). The impact of students’ conceptions of constructivist assumptions on academic achievement and drop-out. Studies in Higher Education, 32, 581–602. doi:10.1080/03075070701573765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loyens, S. M. M., Rikers, R. M. P. J., & Schmidt, H. G. (2007b). Students’ conceptions of distinct constructivist assumptions. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 22, 179–199. doi:10.1007/BF03173521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L., Gava, M., & Boldrin, A. (2008). On warm conceptual change: The interplay of text, epistemological beliefs, and topic interest. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 291–309. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miflin, B. M., Campbell, C. B., & Price, D. A. (1999). A lesson from the introduction of a problem-based, graduate entry course: The effects of different views of self-direction. Medical Education, 33, 801–807. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2923.1999.00399.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miflin, B. M., Campbell, C. B., & Price, D. A. (2000). A conceptual framework to guide the development of self-directed, lifelong learning in problem-based medical curricula. Medical Education, 34, 299–306. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2923.2000.00564.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moust, J. H. C., Van Berkel, H. J. M., & Schmidt, H. G. (2005). Signs of erosion: Reflections on three decades of problem-based learning at Maastricht University. Higher Education, 50, 665–683. doi:10.1007/s10734-004-6371-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nievelstein, F., Van Gog, T., Van Dijck, G., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (2011). Instructional support for novice law students: Reducing search processes and explaining concepts in cases. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 408–413. doi:10.1002/acp.1707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, G. R., & Schmidt, H. G. (1992). The psychological basis of problem-based learning: A review of the evidence. Academic Medicine, 67, 557–565. doi:10.1097/00001888-199209000-00002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429–434. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, C. (2008). The effects of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: A meta-analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 270–300. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plant, E. A., Ericsson, A., Hill, L., & Asberg, K. (2005). Why study time does not predict grade point average across college students: Implications of deliberate practice for academic performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30, 96–116. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.06.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000a). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 54–67. doi:10.1006/ceps.1999.1020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000b). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.55.1.68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, H. G. (2000). Assumptions underlying self-directed learning may be false. Medical Education, 34, 243–245. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2923.2000.0656a.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, H. G., Loyens, S. M. M., Van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2007). Problem-based learning is compatible with human architecture: Commentary on Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark (2006). Educational Psychologist, 42, 91–97. doi:10.1080/00461520701263350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, H. G., & Moust, J. H. C. (2000). Factors affecting small-group tutorial learning: A review of research. In D. H. Evensen & C. E. Hmelo (Eds.), Problem-based learning: A research perspective on learning interactions (pp. 19–52). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, H. G., Van der Molen, H. T., Te Winkel, W. W. R., & Wijnen, W. H. F. W. (2009). Constructivist, problem-based learning does work: A meta-analysis of curricular comparisons involving a single medical school. Educational Psychologist, 44, 227–249. doi:10.1080/00461520903213592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherif, M. (1966). In common predicament. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, J. P. (2002). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Te Winkel, W., Rikers, R., Loyens, S., & Schmidt, H. (2006). Influence of learning resources on study time and achievement scores in a problem-based curriculum. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 11, 381–389. doi:10.1007/s104559-006-9016-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Berkel, H. J. M., & Schmidt, H. G. (2000). Motivation to commit oneself as a determinant of achievement in problem-based learning. Higher Education, 40, 231–242. doi:10.1023/A:1004022116365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Blankenstein, F. M., Dolmans, D. H. J. M., Van der Vleuten, C. P. M., & Schmidt, H. G. (2011). Which cognitive processes support learning during small-group discussion? The role of providing explanations and listening to others. Instructional Science, 39, 189–204. doi:10.1007/s11251-009-9124-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Blankenstein, F. M., Dolmans, D. H. J. M., Van der Vleuten, C. P. M., & Schmidt, H. G. (2013). Relevant prior knowledge moderates the effect of elaboration during small group discussion on academic achievement. Instructional Science, 41, 729–744. doi:10.1007/s11251-012-9252-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Gog, T., Kirschner, F., Kester, L., & Paas, F. (2012). Timing and frequency of mental effort measurement: Evidence in favour of repeated measures. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 833–839. doi:10.1002/acp.2883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2008). Instructional efficiency: Revisiting the original construct in educational research. Educational Psychologist, 43, 16–26. doi:10.1080/00461520701756248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vansteenkiste, M., Sierens, E., Soenens, B., Luyckx, K., & Lens, W. (2009). Motivational profiles from a self-determination perspective: The quality of motivation matters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 671–688. doi:10.1037/a0015083.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learning, performance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and autonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 246–260. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wijnia, L., Loyens, S. M. M., & Derous, E. (2011). Investigating effects of problem-based versus lecture-based learning environments on student motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 101–113. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.11.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. C., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Internalization of biopsychological values by medical students: A test of self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 767–779. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the employees of the Erasmus Behavioral Lab for their support in developing the materials of this experiment. We would like to thank Marit Wijnen for her help with the data collection. We are also greatly indebted to Emily Fox for proofreading an earlier version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lisette Wijnia.

Appendix 1: Overview of open-ended questions and coding scheme

Appendix 1: Overview of open-ended questions and coding scheme

  1. Question 1:

    Give two theoretical explanations that could account for the conflict between the Rattlers and the Eagles.

    2 Points for naming realistic conflict theory (1 point) and providing a correct description stating that intergroup conflict is the result of direct competition (0.5 point) of two groups over a scarce but desirable resource/prize (0.5 point).

    2 Points for naming social identity theory (1 point) and providing a correct description explaining that belonging to a group can lead to conflict (0.5 point) and that people favor in-groups over out-groups in order to enhance their self-esteem (0.5 point).

  2. Question 2:

    Describe two solutions to solve intergroup conflict.

    2 Points for naming superordinate goals (1 point) and providing a correct description: Goals that can only be achieved when the two groups work together (1 point).

    2 Points for naming (1 point) and correctly describing (1 point) a structural solution imposed by a powerful authority. Structural solutions could include: limiting the number of people accessing the resource (via permits), limiting the amount of resources that people can take (via quotas), handing over management of the resource to a leader, facilitating free communication among those accessing the resource, and shifting the pay-off to ensure cooperation is favored over competition.

  3. Question 3:

    The Robbers’ Cave experiment is based on realistic conflict theory. Do you believe the results of this experiment can be explained by this theory? Describe both weak and strong elements of this theory in your answer.

    2 Points when participants mentioned that elements of realistic conflict theory were in line with some of the observations in the experiment. Possible answers could include: (a) explaining that Sherif and colleagues ruled out other explanations; (b) explaining that in-group solidarity, in-group identification, and negative out-group attitudes increased during the competition, or (c) explaining that both the winning and losing teams developed negative/hostile attitudes towards the other group.

    2 Points when participants mentioned that not all observations in the experiment could be explained by the theory. Possible correct answers could include: (a) The experimenters observed some signs of negative attitudes towards the other group even before the competition took place: The boys asked for competitive games when they noticed there was another group at the summer camp. (b) Simply being assigned to a group could promote conflict. (c) A realistic competition for resources might not always be necessary. Sometimes a perception or imagination of a competition is sufficient. Therefore, a “real” competition may not be necessary for intergroup conflict to arise. (d) It is not always necessary that a resource be scarce; sometimes the perception or experience that one is deprived of something relative to others is sufficient. In other words, a sense of relative deprivation may also lead to conflict.

  4. Question 4:

    If you were to design an experiment to examine intergroup conflict, what would you do the same as in the Robbers’ cave experiment and what would you do differently?

    2 Points for mentioning what elements they would keep the same and why (e.g., controlling for other explanations, such as dominant personality, or certain phases of the experiment such as the group formation phase, because this is relevant from both a realistic conflict or social identity perspective).

    2 Points for mentioning what they would change and why (e.g., explaining that not all observations were explained by social identity theory and that the role of direct competition or the role of scarce resources could be examined). Other possible answers could include tests of generalization, role of gender, role of a consolation prize, role of the appointment of an objective leader.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wijnia, L., Loyens, S.M.M., Derous, E. et al. How important are student-selected versus instructor-selected literature resources for students’ learning and motivation in problem-based learning?. Instr Sci 43, 39–58 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-014-9325-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-014-9325-6

Keywords

Navigation