Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Text signals influence second language expository text comprehension: knowledge structure analysis

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Educational Technology Research and Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This quasi-experimental investigation describes the influence of text signals on second language expository science text comprehension. In two course sections, mixed proficiency Korean English language learners (n = 88) read one of two print-based English expository text passage versions. Participants in one section (n = 44) were given a version with interesting but non-important subtopics (NIS) underlined, while participants in the other section (n = 44) were given an alternate version with the same number of substantively important subtopics (SIS) underlined. Participants read the text passage and created a visual map of the text, and then completed a comprehension posttest that measured global inferences (all in English). Analysis of variance of the comprehension posttest data revealed significant differences for the two main factors, proficiency level (Low or High) and text signal condition (NIS or SIS), and for the interaction of proficiency and text signal condition; with Cohen effect sizes, d = Low-NIS (.16) < High-NIS (.56) < Low-SIS (.74) < High-SIS (1.37). Descriptively, both headings and important subtopic terms predominated in the SIS maps, but only non-important subtopic terms and not headings predominated in the NIS maps. Further, the visual map forms, as measured by vector pattern matching and by graph centrality, were also substantially different. Compared to the NIS maps, on average the SIS map forms were more relational and more like the expert’s map, while the NIS map forms were more linear, and showed a primacy effect. These results suggest that coherent text signals in these print-based readings strongly influenced bilinguals’ science expository text comprehension.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barry, S., & Lazarte, A. (2000). A comparison of first and second language readers’ situation models as evidenced in a recall task. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 441 219) http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED441219.pdf.

  • Cashen, V. M., & Leicht, K. L. (1970). Role of the isolation effect in a formal educational setting. Journal of Educational Psychology, 61(6p1), 484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clariana, R. B. (2010). Deriving group knowledge structure from semantic maps and from essays. In D. Ifenthaler, P. Pirnay-Dummer, & N. M. Seel (Eds.), Computer-based diagnostics and systematic analysis of knowledge (pp. 117–130). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clariana, R. B., Draper, D., & Land, S. M. (2011). An automated measure of group knowledge structure convergence. In annual meeting of the AECT International Convention. Retrieved September (Vol. 22, p. 2014).

  • Clariana, R. B., Engelmann, T., & Yu, W. (2013). Using centrality of concept maps as a measure of problem space states in computer-supported collaborative problem solving. Educational Technology Research and Development, 61(3), 423–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clariana, R. B., Rysavy, M. D., & Taricani, E. (2015). Text signals influence team artifacts. Educational Technology Research and Development, 63(1), 35–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clariana, R. B., Wolfe, M. B., & Kim, K. (2014). The influence of narrative and expository lesson text structures on knowledge structures: Alternate measures of knowledge structure. Educational Technology Research and Development, 62(5), 601–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crouse, J. H., & Idstein, P. (1972). Effects of encoding cues on prose learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 63(4), 309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Degand, L., & Sanders, T. (2002). The impact of relational markers on expository text comprehension in L1 and L2. Reading and Writing, 15(7–8), 739–757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fecteau, M. L. (1999). First-and second-language reading comprehension of literary texts. The Modern Language Journal, 83(4), 475–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fesel, S. S., Segers, E., Clariana, R. B., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). Quality of children’s knowledge representations in digital text comprehension: Evidence from pathfinder networks. Computers in Human Behavior, 48, 135–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, R. L., & Barker, A. S. (1974). Effectiveness of highlighting for retention of text material. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59(3), 358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A. (1991). Cognitive processes and mechanisms in language comprehension: The structure building framework. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 27, pp. 217–263). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartley, J., Bartlett, S., & Branthwaite, A. (1980). Underlining can make a difference—sometimes. The Journal of Educational Research, 73(4), 218–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izumi, S. (2002). Output, input enhancement, and the noticing hypothesis. Studies in second language acquisition, 24(04), 541–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H. (1985). The technology of text: Principles for structuring, designing, and displaying text (Vol. 2). Englewood Cliffs: Educational Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H., Beissner, K., & Yacci, M. (1993). Structural knowledge: techniques for representing, conveying, and acquiring structural knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, M. (2012). Cross-validation study on methods and technologies to assess mental models in a complex problem solving situation. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(2), 703–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K., & Clariana, R. B. (2015). Knowledge structure measures of reader’s situation models across languages: Translation engenders richer structure. Technology, Knowledge and Learning, 20, 249–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction- integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laborda, J. G. (2009). Building a validity argument for the Test of English as a Foreign Language™. ELT Journal, 63(3), 291–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBrozzi, R. M. (2016). The effects of textual enhancement type on L2 form recognition and reading comprehension in Spanish. Language Teaching Research, 20, 75–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambiotte, J. G., Dansereau, D. F., Cross, D. R., & Reynolds, S. B. (1989). Multi-relational semantic maps. Educational Psychology Review, 1(4), 331–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. K. (2007). Effects of textual enhancement and topic familiarity on Korean EFL students’ reading comprehension and learning of passive form. Language Learning, 57(1), 87–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, J. W., & Schallert, D. L. (1997). The relative contribution of L2 language proficiency and L1 reading ability to L2 reading performance: A test of the threshold hypothesis in an EFL context. TESOL Quarterly, 31(4), 713–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lemarié, J., Lorch, J. R. F., & Péry-Woodley, M. P. (2012). Understanding how headings influence text processing. Discours. Revue de linguistique, psycholinguistique et informatique, (10).

  • Leow, R. P. (2001). Do learners notice enhanced forms while interacting with the L2?: An online and offline study of the role of written input enhancement in L2 reading. Hispania, 84, 496–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, S. C., Pow, J. W. C., & Cheung, W. C. (2015). A delineation of the cognitive processes manifested in a social annotation environment. Journal of Computer Assisted learning, 31, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, P., Zhang, F., Tsai, E., & Puls, B. (2014). Language history questionnaire (LHQ 2.0): A new dynamic web-based research tool. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(03), 673–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R., Lemarié, J., & Grant, R. (2011). Signaling hierarchical and sequential organization in expository text. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(3), 267–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R. F., Jr., & Lorch, E. P. (1995). Effects of organizational signals on text-processing strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(4), 537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R. F., Lorch, E. P., Ritchey, K., McGovern, L., & Coleman, D. (2001). Effects of headings on text summarization. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26(2), 171–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J., Brandt, D. M., & Bluth, G. J. (1980). Use of top-level structure in text: Key for reading comprehension of ninth-grade students. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 72–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J. F., Ray, M. N., & Middlemiss, W. (2012). Children’s use of comparative text signals: The relationship between age and comprehension ability. Discours[Enligne]. Retrived from http://discours.revues.org/8637.

  • Meyer, B. J., & Rice, G. E. (1983). Learning and memory from text across the adult life span. In J. Fine & R. O. Freedle (Eds.), Developmental studies in discourse (pp. 291–306). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nist, S. L., & Hogrebe, M. C. (1987). The role of underlining and annotating in remembering textual information. Literacy Research and Instruction, 27(1), 12–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overstreet, M. H. (2002). The effect of textual enhancement on second language learner reading comprehension and form recognition (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign).

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1989). There are generalized abilities and one of them is reading. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 307–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pirnay-Dummer, P., & Ifenthaler, D. (2010). Automated knowledge visualization and assessment. In D. Ifenthaler, P. Pirnay-Dummer, & N. M. Seel (Eds.), Computer-based diagnostics and systematic analysis of knowledge. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritchey, K., Schuster, J., & Allen, J. (2008). How the relationship between text and headings influences readers’ memory. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33(4), 859–874.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, C. (2013). American Community Survey Reports: Language use in the United States: 2011. Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/prod/2013pubs/acs-22.pdf.

  • Shook, D. J. (1994). FL/L2 Reading, grammatical information, and the input-to-intake phenomenon. Applied Language Learning, 5(2), 57–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spector, J. M., Johnson, T. E., & Young, P. A. (2015). An editorial on replication studies and scaling up efforts. Education Technology Research and Development, 63, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spyridakis, J. H., & Standal, T. C. (1987). Signals in expository prose: Effects on reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 22(3), 285–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies in discourse comprehension. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Hell, J. G., & Kroll, J. F. (2013). Using electrophysiological measures to track the mapping of words to concepts in the bilingual brain: A focus on translation. In J. Altarriba & L. Isurin (Eds.), Memory, language, and bilingualism: Theoretical and applied approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Villalon, J., & Calvo, R. A. (2011). Concept maps as cognitive visualizations of writing assignments. Educational Technology & Society, 14(3), 16–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilks, C., Meara, P., & Wolter, B. (2005). A further note on simulating word association behaviour in a second language. Second Language Research, 21(4), 359–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, W. (2003). Textual enhancement and simplified input: Effects on L2 comprehension and acquisition of non-meaningful grammatical form. Applied Language Learning, 13, 109–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zareva, A. (2007). Structure of the second language mental lexicon: how does it compare to native speakers’ lexical organization? Second Language Research, 23(2), 123–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zareva, A., & Wolter, B. (2012). The ‘promise’of three methods of word association analysis to L2 lexical research. Second Language Research, 28(1), 41–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank my advisor and co-author of this paper, Dr. Roy B. Clariana, for his assistance and comments that greatly improved the manuscript (You are the best ever advisor). I also thank “anonymous” reviewers for their so-called insights.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kyung Kim.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Appendix

Appendix

The Cave of Lascaux text passage with the NIS terms shown within brackets and SIS terms shown with underlines.

Lascaux Cave Paintings

In Southwest France in the 1940s, playing children discovered Lascaux Grotto, a series of [narrow cave chambers] that contain huge prehistoric paintings of animals. Many of these beasts are as large as 16 feet (almost 5 meters). Some follow each other in solemn parades, but others swirl about, sideways and upside down. The animals are bulls, wild horses, reindeer, bison, and mammoths outlined with charcoal and painted mostly in reds, yellow, and browns. Scientific analysis reveals that the colors were derived from ocher and other iron oxides ground into a fine powder. Methods of applying color varied: some colors were brushed or smeared on rock surfaces and others were blown or sprayed. It is possible that [tubes made from animal bones] were used for spraying because hollow bones, some stained with pigment, have been found nearby.

One of the most puzzling aspects of the paintings is their location. Other rock paintings—for example, those of Bushmen in South Africa—are either located [near cave entrances or completely in the open]. Cave paintings in France and Spain, however, are in recesses and caverns far removed from original cave entrances. This means that artists were forced to work in cramped spaces and without sources of natural light. It also implies that whoever made them did not want them to be easily found. Since [cave dwellers normally lived close to entrances], there must have been some reason why so many generations of Lascaux cave dwellers hid their art.

Migration Opinion

Scholars offer three related but different opinions about the mysterious origin and significance of these paintings. One opinion is that the paintings were a record of seasonal migrations made by herds. Because some paintings were made directly over others overpainting, obliterating them, it is probable that a painting’s value ended with the migration it pictured. Unfortunately, this explanation fails to explain the hidden locations, unless the migrations were celebrated with [secret ceremonies].

Hunting Opinion

Another opinion is that the paintings were directly related to hunting and were an essential part of a special preparation ceremony. This opinion holds that the pictures and whatever ceremony they accompanied were an ancient method of psychologically motivating hunters. It is conceivable that before going hunting the hunters would draw or study pictures of animals and imagine a successful hunt. Considerable support exists for this opinion because several animals in the pictures are wounded by arrows and spears. This opinion also attempts to solve the overpainting by explaining that an animal’s picture had no further use after the hunt.

Ceremonial Opinion

A third opinion takes psychological motivation much further into the realm of tribal ceremonies and mystery: the belief that certain animals assumed mythical significance as ancient ancestors or protectors of a given tribe or clan. Two types of images substantiate this theory: the strange, indecipherable geometric shapes that appear near some animals, and the few drawings of men. Wherever men appear they are crudely drawn and their bodies are elongated and rigid. Some men are in a prone position and some have bird or animal heads. Advocates for this opinion point to reports from people who have experienced a trance state, a highly suggestive state of low consciousness between waking and sleeping. Uniformly, these people experienced weightlessness and the sensation that their bodies were being stretched lengthwise. Advocates also point to people who believe that the forces of nature are inhabited by spirits, particularly shamans who believe that an animal’s spirit and energy is transferred to them while in a trance. One Lascaux narrative picture, which shows [a man with a birdlike head] and a wounded animal, would seem to lend credence to this third opinion, but there is still much that remains unexplained. For example, where is the proof that the man in the picture is a shaman? He could as easily just be a hunter wearing a headmask. Many tribal hunters, including some (Native Americans), camouflaged themselves by wearing animal heads and hides.

Perhaps so much time has passed that there will never be satisfactory answers to the cave images, but their mystique only adds to their importance. Certainly a great art exists, and by its existence reveals that ancient human beings were not without intelligence, skill, and sensitivity.

The four headings

Lascaux cave paintings, Migration opinion, Hunting opinion, Ceremonial opinion.

The seven non-important subtopic terms (NIS)

Narrow cave, tubes, near entrances, cave dwellers, secret ceremonies, birdlike head, and Native Americans.

The seven important subtopic terms (SIS)

Paintings of animals, puzzling, location, overpainting, motivation, mystical significance, and shamans.

The eight unsignaled terms

Intelligent, ancient human, arrow and spear, trance state, France and Spain, children, and hidden (in order from high to low frequency).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, K., Clariana, R.B. Text signals influence second language expository text comprehension: knowledge structure analysis. Education Tech Research Dev 65, 909–930 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-016-9494-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-016-9494-x

Keywords

Navigation