Skip to main content
Log in

Teachers’ gestures and speech in mathematics lessons: forging common ground by resolving trouble spots

  • Original Article
  • Published:
ZDM Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research focused on how teachers establish and maintain shared understanding with students during classroom mathematics instruction. We studied the micro-level interventions that teachers implement spontaneously as a lesson unfolds, which we call micro-interventions. In particular, we focused on teachers’ micro-interventions around trouble spots, defined as points during the lesson when students display lack of understanding. We investigated how teachers use gestures along with speech in responding to such trouble spots in a corpus of six middle-school mathematics lessons. Trouble spots were a regular occurrence in the lessons (M = 10.2 per lesson). We hypothesized that, in the face of trouble spots, teachers might increase their use of gestures in an effort to re-establish shared understanding with students. Thus, we predicted that teachers would gesture more in turns immediately following trouble spots than in turns immediately preceding trouble spots. This hypothesis was supported with quantitative analyses of teachers’ gesture frequency and gesture rates, and with qualitative analyses of representative cases. Thus, teachers use gestures adaptively in micro-interventions in order to foster common ground when instructional communication breaks down.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Beat gestures that were superimposed on representational gestures were not counted separately, as coders had difficulty identifying these gestures reliably.

References

  • Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 169–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2007). Teachers’ gestures as a means of scaffolding students’ understanding: Evidence from an early algebra lesson. In R. Goldman, R. Pea, B. Barron, & S. J. Derry (Eds.), Video research in the learning sciences (pp. 349–365). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: Evidence from students’ and teachers’ gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 247–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., & Fujimori, Y. (2011). Gestures in the mathematics classroom: What’s the point? In N. Stein & S. Raudenbush (Eds.), Developmental Cognitive Science Goes To School (pp. 219–234). New York: Routledge, Taylor and Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alibali, M. W., Young, A. G., Crooks, N. M., Yeo, A., Ledesma, I. M., Nathan, M. J., et al. (2012). Students learn more when their teacher has learned to gesture effectively (Submitted).

  • Arzarello, F. (2006). Semiosis as a multimodal process. In Revista latinoamericana de investigacion en mathematic educativa, numero especial, pp. 267–299.

  • Arzarello, F., Paola, D., Robutti, O., & Sabena, C. (2009). Gestures as semiotic resources in the mathematics classroom. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 97–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, B., & Pope, T. (2008). Developmental psychology: Incorporating Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s theories in classrooms. Journal of Cross-Disciplinary Perspectives in Education, 1(1), 59–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brintoon, B., & Fujiki, M. (1989). Conversational management with language-impaired children. Rockville: Aspen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. B., Ayman-Nolley, S., & Mahootian, S. (2004). The role of gesture in bilingual education: Does gesture enhance learning? International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 7, 303–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. B., Garber, P., & Rogalski, K. (2007). The role of gesture in memory and social communication. Gesture, 7, 137–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Brennan, S. A. (1991). Grounding in communication. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition. Washington, DC: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition, 106, 1047–1058.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., Wood, T., & Yackel, E. (1993). Discourse, mathematical thinking and classroom practice. In E. Forman, N. Minick, & C. Stone (Eds.), Contexts for learning: Sociocultural dynamics in children’s development (pp. 91–119). New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Edwards, L. D. (2009). Gestures and conceptual integration in mathematical talk. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70, 127–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M. A., Feenstra, E., Ryon, E., & McNeill, D. (2011). A multimodal approach to coding discourse: Collaboration, distributed cognition, and geometric reasoning. Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 6, 253–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flevares, L. M., & Perry, M. (2001). How many do you see? The use of nonspoken representations in first-grade mathematics lessons. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 330–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. (2010). Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 586–596.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golab, J., Lind, C., & Okell, E. (2009). Conversation analysis of repair in interaction with adults who have acquired hearing impairment. In Hearing Care for Adults 2009. Chicago: Phonak.

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., & Singer, M. A. (2003). From children's hands to adults' ears: Gesture’s role in the learning process. Developmental Psychology, 39, 509–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., Kim, S., & Singer, M. (1999). What the teachers’ hands tell the students’ minds about math. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 720–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., Wein, D., & Chang, C. (1992). Assessing knowledge through gesture: Using children’s hands to read their minds. Cognition and Instruction, 9, 201–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2007). Environmentally coupled gestures. In S. Duncan, J. Cassell, & E. Levy (Eds.), Gesture and the dynamic dimensions of language (pp. 195–212). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1991). Sharing cognition through collective comprehension activity. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 331–348). Washington: APA.

  • Holler, J., & Stevens, R. (2007). The effect of common ground on how speakers use gesture and speech to represent size information. Journal of Language & Social Psychology, 26, 4–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 297–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 495–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2010). Language, gesture, action! A test of the gesture as simulated action framework. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 245–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter, A. B., Bieda, K., Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., & Knuth, E. J. (2006). Don’t just tell them, show them! Teachers can intentionally alter their instructional gestures. In R. Sun (Ed.), Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1523–1528). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurtig, R. (1977). Toward a functional theory of discourse. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), Discourse production and comprehension. Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, M., Roth, W.-M.,& Thom, J. (2011). Children’s gestures and the embodied knowledge of geometry. International Journal of Science & Mathematics Education, 9(1), 207–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2001). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marrongelle, K. (2007). The function of graphs and gestures in algorithmatization. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 26(3), 211–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeil, N. M., Alibali, M. W., & Evans, J. L. (2000). The role of gesture in children’s comprehension of spoken language: Now they need it, now they don’t. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 131–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (2010). Gesten der Macht und die Macht der Gesten (gestures of power and the power of gestures). In E. Fischer-Lichte & C. Wulf (Eds.), Gesten, Inszenierung, Aufführung und Praxis (Gesture, Staging, Performance, and Practice). Munich: Wilhelm Fink.

  • McNeill, D., & Duncan, S. (2000). Growth points in thinking-for-speaking. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 141–161). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nathan, M. J., & Alibali, M. W. (2011). How gesture use enables intersubjectivity in the classroom. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 257–266). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nathan, M. J., Eilam, B., & Kim, S. (2007). To disagree, we must also agree: How intersubjectivity structures and perpetuates discourse in a mathematics classroom. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 16(4), 525–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nathan, M. J., & Kim, S. (2009). Regulation of teacher elicitations in the mathematics classroom. Cognition and Instruction, 27(2), 91–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NCTM. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky, R., Rasmussen, C., Sweeney, G., & Wawro, M. (2012). When the classroom floor becomes the complex plane: Addition and multiplication as ways of bodily navigation. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 287–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R. (2005). Do real numbers really move? Language, thought, and gesture: The embodied cognitive foundations of mathematics. In F. Iida, R. Pfeifer, L. Steels, & Y. Kuniyoshi (Eds.), Embodied artificial intelligence (pp. 54–73). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, T. M. (2009). Online but off-topic: Negotiating common ground in small learning groups. Instructional Science, 37, 227–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L., Edwards, L., & Arzarello, F. (2009). Introduction: beyond words. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70, 91–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, C., Stephan, M., & Allen, K. (2004). Classroom mathematical practices and gesturing. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 23, 301–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richland, L. E., Zur, O., & Holyoak, K. J. (2007). Cognitive supports for analogies in the mathematics classroom. Science, 316, 1128–1129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2002). Gestures: Their role in teaching and learning. Review of Educational Research, 71, 365–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-correction in the organisation of repair in conversation. Language, 53, 361–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schleppenbach, M., Flevares, L. M., Sims, L. M., & Perry, M. (2007). Teachers’ responses to student mistakes in Chinese and U.S. mathematics classrooms. The Elementary School Journal, 108(2), 131–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A. H. (1998). Toward a theory of teaching-in-context. Issues in Education: Contributions from Cognitive Psychology, 4, 1–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seedhouse, P. (2004). The interactional architecture of the language classroom: A conversation analysis perspective. Malden: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2009). What’s all the fuss about gestures? A commentary. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 191–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, L. (2011). Embodied cognition. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidnell, J. (2010). Conversation analysis: An introduction. West Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, M. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Children learn when their teacher’s gestures and speech differ. Psychological Science, 16, 85–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, M. A., Radinsky, J., & Goldman, S. R. (2008). The role of gesture in meaning construction. Discourse Processes, 45, 365–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valenzeno, L., Alibali, M. W., & Klatzky, R. L. (2003). Teachers’ gestures facilitate students’ learning: A lesson in symmetry. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 187–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Trans.). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Grant # R305H060097 from the U. S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences (Alibali, PI). All opinions expressed herein are those of the authors and not the U. S. Department of Education. We thank Maia Ledesma, Kristen Bieda, and Elise Lockwood for their contributions to this research. Most of all, we thank the teachers and students who opened their classrooms to us and allowed us to videotape their instruction.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Martha W. Alibali.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Alibali, M.W., Nathan, M.J., Church, R.B. et al. Teachers’ gestures and speech in mathematics lessons: forging common ground by resolving trouble spots. ZDM Mathematics Education 45, 425–440 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-012-0476-0

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-012-0476-0

Keywords

Navigation