Effects of stimulus frequency and intensity on habituation and sensitization in acute spinal cat

https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(69)90194-2Get rights and content

Abstract

Habituation and sensitization of the flexor withdrawal reflex (tibialis anterior muscle) were studied in acute spinal cat as a function of stimulus intensity and frequency for frequencies ranging above 12 sec. The following relationships were consistently observed: (1) habituation is an exponential decline in response amplitude which occurs as a function of iterated stimulation of cutaneous afferents. In some instances habituation may be described as linear over much of its course; (2) habituation occurs more rapidly and to a greater degree of response decrement for higher frequencies of stimulation; (3) habituation is more pronounced for low intensities of stimulation than for high intensities, and; (4) sensitization occurs as a response increment with repetitive stimulation of cutaneous afferents under certain circumstances and may show two distinct time courses, short-term and long-term sensitization. These two phenomena may be differentiated on the basis of stimulus intensity, frequency and their interaction.

References (21)

  • J.P. Griffin et al.

    The effect of bladder distension on habituation of the flexor withdrawal reflex in the decerebrate spinal cat

    Brain Res.

    (1968)
  • F.B. Beswick et al.

    Reflex effects of repetitive stimulation of Group I muscle afferent fibres

    J. Physiol.

    (1955)
  • J.S. Buchwald et al.

    Progressive changes in efferent unit response to repeated cutaneous stimulation in spinal cats

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1965)
  • J.P. Griffin et al.

    Habituation of the flexor reflex in the rat

    J. Physiol.

    (1967)
  • J.D. Harris

    Habituatory response decrement in the intact organism

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1943)
  • R. Hinde
  • G. Horn

    Neuronal mechanisms of habituation

    Nature

    (1967)
  • J.I. Hubbard et al.

    An electrophysiological investigation of mammalian motor nerve terminals

    J. Physiol.

    (1963)
  • D.P. Lloyd

    Electrotonus in dorsal nerve roots

  • D.P. Lloyd

    Post-tetanic potentiation of responses in monosynaptic reflex pathways of the spinal cord

    J. gen. Physiol.

    (1949)
There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (59)

  • Are the persistent effects of “gate control” stimulation on nociception a form of generalization of habituation that is endocannabinoid-dependent?

    2018, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
    Citation Excerpt :

    Of the total 327 animals tested across all experiments, 45 animals were excluded based on this criteria. In many studies of habituation, the ability to reverse habituation (dishabituation) is used to confirm that the observed response decrement is not due to fatigue or sensory adaption (Groves, Lee, & Thompson, 1970; Rankin et al., 2009). Since noxious stimuli are often effective at producing dishabituation, we used the post-test delivery of the nociceptive thermal stimulus (i.e. the Hargreaves apparatus) as a convenient dishabituating stimulus (Fig. 2A).

  • Learning from the spinal cord: How the study of spinal cord plasticity informs our view of learning

    2014, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
    Citation Excerpt :

    While seemingly odd, a similar effect has been observed in other paradigms and is anticipated by an older model of learning (Hull, 1943; Overmier, Payne, Brackbill, Linder, & Lawry, 1979). Increasing the ISI generally weakens non-associative habituation (Groves et al., 1969). Given this, we assessed the impact of increasing the ISI 5-fold, from 2 to 10 min.

  • Adaptation of the repellency response to DEET in rhodnius prolixus

    2011, Journal of Insect Physiology
    Citation Excerpt :

    However, there are some features associated with sensory adaptation that allow discarding this phenomenon and confirming habituation in learning experiments. In sensory adaptation, stronger stimuli produce a higher decrease in the response; the opposite is expected in habituation, where response decrements to higher stimulus intensities take longer (Asztalos et al., 2007; Groves et al., 1969; Kuenen and Baker, 1981). In addition, the recovery of the response is usually faster in sensory adaptation, while habituation shows more temporal stability (Devaud et al., 2001; Kuenen and Baker, 1981).

  • Auditory stimulation dishabituates anti-predator escape behavior in hermit crabs (Coenobita clypeatus)

    2011, Behavioural Processes
    Citation Excerpt :

    A crucial difference that determines whether habituation or sensitization occurs is the intensity of the stimulus. Habituation is more rapid with low-intensity stimulation (e.g., Groves et al., 1968); with intense or noxious stimuli, sensitization is likely to occur first, followed by habituation (e.g., Davis et al., 1982). The presentation of a novel, arousing (i.e., sensitizing) stimulus can cause the recovery of a previously habituated response; this effect is known as dishabituation (Thompson and Spencer, 1966; Castellucci and Kandel, 1976).

View all citing articles on Scopus

Supported in part by research grant NB 02161 from the National Institutes of Health and MH 1, 279-01 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Research Scientist award MH 6650 from the National Institute of Mental Health, and the National Science Foundation Undergraduate Research Participation Program GY 4507. The authors gratefully acknowledge the skilled technical assistance of Sarah Beydler.

View full text