Mismatch negativity: clinical research and possible applications

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00053-9Get rights and content

Abstract

This article provides a selective review on the perspectives of the clinical research and application of the mismatch negativity (MMN), a component of the auditory event-related potential generated by the brain's automatic response to any discriminable change in auditory stimulation. The MMN (and its magnetic equivalent MMNm) currently provide the only objective measure of auditory discrimination and sensory memory. It can be registered in the absence of attention and with no task requirements, which makes it particularly suitable for studying different clinical populations and infants.

Section snippets

The mismatch negativity: an introduction

The mismatch negativity (MMN: Näätänen et al., 1978), which can also be measured magnetically (MMNm), is an electric brain response (a negative component of the event-related potential, (ERP)) to any discriminable change (‘deviant’) in some repetitive aspect of auditory stimulation (‘standard’). Importantly, the MMN can be elicited even in the absence of attention, with the most dramatic demonstrations being provided by MMNs recorded in comatose patients (Kane et al., 1993, Kane et al., 1996,

MMN in pediatry and neuropediatry

One potential field of application of the MMN involves newborns and young infants (for a review, see Kraus and Cheour, 2000). Alho et al. (1990) were the first to record an MMN in newborns. Their standard stimuli were tones of 1000 Hz and deviant stimuli tones of 1200 Hz. During the recording, the newborns were sleeping. Interestingly, this MMN was elicited by vowel change, i.e., acoustically quite a modest change. Furthermore, Cheour-Luhtanen et al. (1996) found an MMN even in prematurely born

MMN and developmental disorders

The MMN has recently been used to study phonological and auditory dysfunctions in dyslexia. For example, Schulte-Körne et al. (1998) used the MMN to compare the discrimination of speech and non-speech stimuli in dyslexic and control adolescents. Their speech stimuli were syllables (/da/ as the standard stimulus and /ba/ as the deviant stimulus) and their non-speech stimuli sine-wave tones. It was found that the MMNs for the tone stimuli did not differ between the two groups, whereas the

Psychiatry

One of the most interesting clinical research lines using the MMN involves schizophrenia. Since the initial finding of Shelley et al., (1991), the majority of these studies have shown a considerably attenuated MMN in schizophrenic patients (for a review, see Michie, 2001). These results mainly involve MMNs to sound-duration and -frequency changes (Michie et al., 2000a), with other types of sound changes being used only seldom. Since this abnormality is similarly found both with long and short

Neurology

Early studies (Czigler et al., 1992, Woods et al., 1991) on the aging effects on the MMN amplitude suggested that this amplitude is reduced with aging. In a subsequent study, Pekkonen et al. (1993) found that with frequency change, this effect was confined to conditions with long ISIs, suggesting that it is not perception but rather sensory memory for auditory frequency that is affected by aging. Consistent with this, more recently, Pekkonen et al. (1996) observed that when the ISI was 0.5 s,

Acknowledgements

Special Didactic Lecture delivered at the 11th World Congress of Psychophysiology, the Olympics of the Brain, Celebrating the 20th Anniversary of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, associated with the United Nations (New York), July 29–August 3, 2002, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

References (76)

  • R. Näätänen et al.

    Early selective-attention effects on the evoked potential: a critical review and reinterpretation

    Biol. Psychol.

    (1979)
  • P. Paavilainen et al.

    Right-hemisphere dominance of different mismatch negativities

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1991)
  • E. Pekkonen et al.

    Mismatch negativity area and age-related auditory memory

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1993)
  • T. Rinne et al.

    Separate time behaviors of the temporal and frontal MMN sources

    NeuroImage

    (2000)
  • A.-M. Shelley et al.

    Diminished responsiveness of ERPs in schizophrenic subjects to changes in auditory stimulation parameters: implications for theories of cortical dysfunction

    Schizophr. Res.

    (1999)
  • A.M. Shelley et al.

    Mismatch negativity: an index of a preattentive processing deficit in schizophrenia

    Biol. Psychiatr.

    (1991)
  • N. Shinozaki et al.

    The difference in Mismatch negativity between the acute and post-acute phase of schizophrenia

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2002)
  • P. Tallal

    Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children

    Brain Lang

    (1980)
  • I. Winkler et al.

    Event-related potentials in auditory backward recognition masking: a new way to study the neurophysiological basis of sensory memory in humans

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (1992)
  • J. Ahveninen et al.

    Acute and chronic effects of alcohol on preattentive auditory processing as reflected by mismatch negativity

    Audiol. Neuro-Otol.

    (2000)
  • K. Alho

    Cerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes

    Ear Hearing

    (1995)
  • K. Alho et al.

    Effects of involuntary auditory attention on visual task performance and brain activity

    NeuroReport

    (1997)
  • T. Baldeweg et al.

    Impaired auditory frequency discrimination in dyslexia detected with mismatch evoked potentials

    Ann. Neurol.

    (1999)
  • S.V. Catts et al.

    Brain potential evidence for an auditory sensory memory deficit in schizophrenia

    Am. J. Psychiatr.

    (1995)
  • M. Cheour et al.

    Mismatch negativity (MMN) as an index of auditory sensory memory deficit in cleft-palate and catch-syndrome children

    NeuroReport

    (1998)
  • M. Cheour-Luhtanen et al.

    The ontogenetically earliest discriminative response of the human brain

    Psychophysiol. Special Report

    (1996)
  • N. Cowan

    On short and long auditory stores

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1984)
  • Csépe, V., Gyurkocza, E.E. Abnormal mismatch negativity to phonetic deviations in developmental dyslexia. Developmental...
  • M.E. Farmer et al.

    The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review

    Psychon. Bull. Rev.

    (1995)
  • A.M. Galaburda et al.

    Evidence for aberrant auditory anatomy in developmental dyslexia

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1994)
  • M.H. Giard et al.

    Separate representation of stimulus frequency, intensity and duration in auditory sensory memory: an event-related potential and dipole-model analysis

    J. Cog. Neurosci.

    (1995)
  • M.H. Giard et al.

    Brain generators implicated in processing of auditory stimulus deviance: a topographic event-related potential study

    Psychophysiology

    (1990)
  • Heim, S., Eulitz, C., Wienbruck, C., Elbert, T., 1995. Effects of syllabic training on literacy skills, phonological...
  • I.P. Jääskeläinen et al.

    Low dose of ethanol suppresses mismatch negativity of auditory event-related potentials

    Alcohol.: Clin. Exp. Res.

    (1995)
  • D.C. Javitt

    Intracortical mechanisms of mismatch negativity dysfunction in schizophrenia

    Audiol. Neuro-Otol.

    (2000)
  • D.C. Javitt et al.

    Impaired precision, but normal retention, of auditory sensory (‘echoic’) memory information in schizophrenia

    J. Abnorm. Psychol.

    (1997)
  • C. Javitt et al.

    Impaired mismatch negativity (MMN) generation in schizophrenia as a function of stimulus deviance, probability, and interstimulus/interdeviant interval

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1998)
  • Javitt, D.C., Shelley, A.-M., Silipo, G., Lieberman, J.A.. Deficits in AX-continuous performance test performance and...
  • Cited by (207)

    • Multi-feature mismatch negativity: How can reliable data be recorded in a short time?

      2022, Clinical Neurophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Generally, MMN peaks at 150–250 ms with an amplitude between 2 and 5 mV in adults (Näätänen et al., 1989, Näätänen et al., 2007, Tiitinen et al., 1994). Central auditory system function is objectively evaluated with MMN in the case of many clinical disorders including schizophrenia, dyslexia, coma, paralysis, aphasia, specific language disorders, aging, multiple sclerosis, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, epilepsy, and autism (Baldeweg et al., 2004, Cooper et al., 2006, Näätänen, 2003, O’connor, 2012, Pettigrew et al., 2005, Shinozaki et al., 2002). Moreover, it can also be utilized in evaluations of the benefit of amplification, auditory processing disorders, and different aspects of language as well as in the perception of signals at the cortical level and evaluations of neural plasticity in children and adults with impaired speech perception (Näätänen, 2003).

    • Cerebral representation of sequence patterns across multiple presentation formats

      2021, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      Spontaneous learning of sequence regularities and associated expectations can be evidenced by observing the novelty responses triggered when these regularities are violated. The mismatch negativity (MMN), for instance, is observed when the repeated presentation of a “standard” sound stimulus is suddenly disrupted by the presentation of a different sound stimulus (Näätänen, 2003; Näätänen, Paavilainen, Rinne, & Alho, 2007). Human sequence knowledge, however, goes way beyond the learning that a single stimulus is repeated with a specific timing, and extends to the encoding and manipulation of temporal sequences with a complex structure (Amalric et al., 2017; Bekinschtein et al., 2009; Planton et al., 2021; Wang, Uhrig, Jarraya, & Dehaene, 2015).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text