Elsevier

Energy Policy

Volume 65, February 2014, Pages 524-540
Energy Policy

Subjective discount rates in the general population and their predictive power for energy saving behavior

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2013.10.049Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Results of a large panel study in Switzerland.

  • Mean subjective discount rates in population are well above market interest rates.

  • Subjective discount rates are moderately stable over four years.

  • Theory suggests impact of subjective discount rates on energy saving behavior.

  • However, subjective discount rates do not contribute to explanation of energy saving behavior.

Abstract

Why do people sometimes refrain from saving energy even if it would pay off in monetary terms? Subjective discount rates present one possible explanation for this lack of foresight, but little is known about their level and reliability in the general population. With regard to behavior, persons with lower discount rates are expected to accept additional costs upfront more readily than those with higher discount rates. Based on a representative nation-wide study, the Swiss Environmental Survey 2007, and a follow-up survey, our analyses reveal that on average subjective discount rates are well above market interest rates and moderately stable over a time interval of four years. Income and education are negatively correlated with discount rates. Contrary to expectations, we did not find convincing support for an impact of discount rates on energy saving behavior.

Introduction

Many decisions relevant to environmental conservation involve a trade-off between short- and long-term benefits. For example, when buying a washing machine, less expensive but less energy-efficient devices have to be compared to options with higher purchase prices and lower operating costs. In some of these situations, investments in energy efficiency would result in lower life-cycle costs. But even in such cases, the corresponding option is not always chosen. Hence, more money than necessary is spent on certain goods and services, and on the aggregate level a large potential for energy saving is lost. This lack of investments corresponds to the so-called “energy efficiency gap” (Howarth, 2004).

This paper focuses on one possible explanation for such a lack of foresight: subjective discount rates.1 They capture the extent to which a person is present- or future-oriented. Daly and Farley (2011, p. 190) describe discounting as follows: “When evaluating present and future values, intertemporal discounting is the process of systematically weighting future costs and benefits as less valuable than present ones. […] The farther off in time that a cost or benefit occurs, the more we discount its present value.” A high discount rate implies that someone is devaluing future rewards rapidly and thus is present-oriented. In contrast, a low discount rate signifies a higher valuation of future utility and therefore a higher degree of future orientation.

Environmentally responsible behavior that pays off financially often only does so in the long run. It therefore requires behaving in a future-oriented manner by delaying utility. For example, the purchase of a fuel-efficient car may initially be more expensive than a less energy-efficient vehicle, but it is profitable in the long term and at the same time causes a lower environmental burden. In such situations, persons with lower discount rates should more readily accept additional costs up front than those with higher discount rates since they put greater value on future utility.

While these theoretical considerations might be compelling, little is known about the level of subjective discount rates and their reliability (and hence temporal stability) in the general population. Most previous studies have been conducted with relatively small (student) samples and report average discount rates that are considerably higher than market interest rates (see Frederick et al., 2002). Furthermore, there are only a few studies analyzing subjective discount rates as predictors of actual behavior. So far the results have been heterogeneous.

The aim of this paper is fourfold: First, it reports subjective discount rates for a representative population sample in Switzerland. Second, a brief analysis of the reliability of discount rates is conducted. This is noteworthy not only because of the sample properties but also since the measurements were conducted four years apart. Third, the effects of different socio-demographic variables on subjective discount rates are analyzed by means of multivariate methods. Fourth, discount rates are used to predict self-reported behavior. This paper thus presents one of the rare examples that analyzes a representative general population sample and links discount rates to energy saving behavior.

So far, only a few studies have reported discount rates for representative population samples. For example, Harrison et al. (2002) have reported a mean of 28% for a Danish sample and Epper et al. (2011) a median of 47% based on an online survey in the German-speaking area of Switzerland.

For student samples, a broad range of average discount rates has been reported. For example, in their classic study Benzion et al. (1989) report mean discount rates of 11–46% (average 21%) depending on framing, amounts and delays involved. There is no clear expectation as to whether the average discount rate of a student or a population sample should be higher. On the one hand, students are more educated than the average population and therefore presumably more adept in handling compound interest computations. On the other hand, they might also be more impulsive and hence more tempted to choose earlier payments rather than more delayed ones (see the next section for a discussion of the effects of education and age on discount rates).

When interpreting subjective discount rates reported by such studies, it should be kept in mind that the way in which discount rates are typically measured already implies there should be positive discounting and hence may bias discount rates upwards (Frederick et al., 2002): Most studies – including the ones cited above – use choice tasks to capture subjective discount rates (Frederick et al., 2002). Usually, respondents are given a choice between a smaller sooner reward (SSR) and a larger later reward (LLR) – for example, a payment of $100 in one year versus a payment of $125 in two years. In the above example, someone preferring the earlier payment is said to have an annual discount rate of at least 25%. As such a measurement simply yields a lower or upper bound on the discount rate, many studies use series of choice tasks varying the delay as well as the amounts of the reward involved. By doing so, the possible range of each person's discount rate can be narrowed down (for example, Kirby et al., 1999). The absolute level of discount rates found in such experiments depends on several factors (for comprehensive reviews see Frederick et al., 2002, Manzini and Mariotti, 2007). For example, lower discount rates are reported when higher amounts of a reward are involved (see Kirby, 1997, Percoco and Nijkamp, 2009). This “magnitude effect” is plausible if the respondents do not only consider the relative but also the absolute height of the amounts involved and behave accordingly (Loewenstein and Prelec, 1991).

A related phenomenon is increasing patience with delay (hyperbolic discounting). For example, a decision between a cookie tomorrow and two cookies the day after tomorrow is perceived differently from a decision between a cookie in 60 days and two cookies in 61 days. In both situations, the same additional waiting period (one day) is required to receive a larger instead of a smaller reward. However, the situations differ with regard to when this additional waiting period begins (in one or in 60 days). Hyperbolic discounting conveys that respondents are more likely to wait for LLRs in the second type of decision. The closer to the present the additional waiting period starts, the higher the discount rates are (Benzion et al., 1989, Thaler, 1981).

So far there is no conclusive evidence on whether discount rates are affected by whether hypothetical rewards, real rewards or rewards depending on a lottery are used (Coller and Williams, 1999, Frederick et al., 2002). It could either be argued that the possibility of actually receiving a reward increases its salience (see for example research on psychological distance; Trope and Liberman, 2010) and hence may lead to more impulsive choices, or it could be assumed that incentivized choices should yield lower discount rates as respondents might be more thoughtful when facing real rewards (see Camerer and Hogarth, 1999).

Apart from the methodological factors discussed so far, there are other possible confounding factors such as transaction costs, risk preferences and trust in the paying institution. To reduce these, studies with incentivized choices often use delayed rewards only (see Frederick et al., 2002, Harrison et al., 2005).

In our surveys, the subjective discount rate was measured twice in each wave. All measures are based on choice tasks, some of which were incentivized while some also included a front-end delay. This allows comparing them and testing for possible influences of the magnitude of the rewards, the delays involved and the presence of a lottery. However, one of the main goals of this paper is to estimate the average discount rate for a general population sample, in this case the Swiss population.

Only a few studies have investigated the reliability or stability of discount rates. Typically, two measurements were conducted with a brief period between them, such as one week or three months, and the findings are based on non-representative and rather small (student) samples. Table 1 gives an overview of correlations between discount rates over time reported by previous studies. Most of them report moderate to high correlations, although the results vary both between and within the studies.

Unfortunately, none of these correlation studies is based on a representative population sample. The present paper will, however, report the test–retest reliability of discount rates over a period of four years as well as the parallel form reliability for a representative Swiss population sample.

Although several socio-demographic variables have been shown to co-vary with subjective discount rates, the evidence regarding the causes of individual differences in discount rates is still very limited (see Kirby et al., 2002). Stable effects have mainly been found for education and income. Most studies report a negative relationship – thus, higher income and higher education both accompany lower discount rates (for example Burks et al., 2011, de Wit et al., 2007, Green et al., 1996, Harrison et al., 2002, Hausman, 1979, Reimers et al., 2009). For gender and age the existing studies report diverging results. Many studies do not find any relationship between discount rates and gender (for example Anderson and Stafford, 2009, Coller and Williams, 1999, Daly et al., 2008, de Wit et al., 2007, Harrison et al., 2002). A few point to females being more present-oriented (Cairns et al., 2000, Read and Read, 2004, Reimers et al., 2009) and others on the delay of gratification generally conclude females were slightly more future-oriented (Silverman, 2003).

Studies on the effect of age are mainly based on cross-sectional data (see Borghans et al., 2008, Frederick et al., 2002, Khwaja et al., 2007). To our knowledge, there are no panel studies over substantial periods. Different theoretical positions either suggest a decline (Rogers, 1994), a steady increase (Trostel and Taylor, 2001) or a curvilinear relationship (u-shaped; Sozou and Seymour, 2003) over adulthood. Each of these positions comes with limited empirical support only (for example Cairns et al., 2000, Harrison et al., 2002, Kirby et al., 2002, Read and Read, 2004, Reimers et al., 2009).

Our analyses focus on the four predictors discussed above – education, income, gender and age – as they are believed to be relevant for many other individual differences, and attempt to shed further light on their role in discounting.

So far, discounting and pro-environmental behavior have not been linked very often. A number of mainly older studies from the US infer (aggregate market) discount rates from actual energy-saving behavior (e.g. Gately, 1980, Hausman, 1979, Liebermann and Ungar, 1983, Meier and Witthier, 1983, Ruderman et al., 1987). Their results vary greatly both between and within product types (partially due to model specifications). For example, annual discount rates of 10–32% were found for thermal insulations, 2–36% for space heating, 3–29% for air conditioning systems and 34–300% for refrigerators (Train, 1985, p. 1246ff). Most of these discount rates are considerably higher than market interest rates. This implies that consumers spend more money on certain goods and services than necessary. But as these studies inferred discount rates from behavior, the latter determines the former and not the other way round. One cannot conclude from these studies that the subjective discount rate has a causal impact on the purchasing behavior.

Many of the more recent studies, in contrast, use discount rates as predictors of behavior and thus measure individual discount rates independently from behavior. Most of these studies focus on financial savings and debts, substance abuse, sexual intercourse and other health-related behaviors such as nutrition and exercise (e.g. Burks et al., 2011, Chabris et al., 2008, Chapman, 1998, Khwaja et al., 2007, Kirby et al., 1999, Nyhus, 2002, Reimers et al., 2009, Sutter et al., 2010). The results so far have been ambiguous.

Only very few studies have linked discount rates to environmental behavior. To our knowledge, these are the studies by Fehr and Leibbrandt (2008) and Liebermann and Ungar, 1997, Liebermann and Ungar, 2002. Fehr and Leibbrandt (2008) analyze the impact of discount rates on fishing in rural Brazil. For the particular behaviors in question, the individual rational solution is to free-ride and thus not to behave environmentally friendly. Nonetheless, one of two measures of discount rates – a choice task involving mineral water – had a significant impact: in comparison to respondents with high discount rates, those with low discount rates do their everyday fishing in a more future-oriented, environmentally conscious manner. An analogous measure using a monetary reward did not predict behavior.

Both studies by Liebermann and Ungar, 1997, Liebermann and Ungar, 2002 first measured discount rates and then presented the respondents with a questionnaire including an item on the purchase of an air-conditioning system. This task required a choice between a more energy-efficient while initially more expensive device and a less energy-efficient system with a lower purchase price. The earlier study implied an annual threshold discount rate of roughly 7%, the later a rate of 10%. In both studies, persons with discount rates below this cut-off value were expected to choose the more energy-efficient device whereas respondents with discount rates above this threshold were expected to do the opposite. Behavior in line with these expectations was labeled as “efficient” (Liebermann and Ungar, 2002, p. 732). Roughly 41% of the participants in 1997 and 71% in 2002 made efficient choices in terms of their discount rates. However, note that this is a weak test for the hypothesis of an impact of subjective discount rates on behavior because both studies rely on hypothetical choices.

Given these inconsistent results, the question of the relevance of discounting for behavioral outcomes needs further investigation. Our study contributes by applying discount rates as a predictor of various energy-saving behaviors. From this, the question arises as to which behaviors are expected to be influenced by discount rates. Generally, discount rates convey a sense of future orientation and self-control; more narrowly, they imply financial optimization. This supplies us with a criterion for when to expect a relationship: where energy-saving behavior is most cost-effective in the long run.

Section snippets

Participants and procedure

All analyses are based on data from the Swiss Environmental Survey 2007, a representative general population study (n=3369), and a follow-up survey conducted around four years later (n=1945). The data collection for the Swiss Environmental Survey 2007 was based on a two-stage random sample taken from the adult population of Switzerland with a registered telephone extension. In a first step, households were selected randomly from regional strata and notified by mail. The study was described as

Socio-demographic determinants of subjective discount rates

To analyze the effects of different socio-demographic variables on subjective discount rates, OLS regression models were run for the serial choice tasks and logistic models were estimated for the singular choice tasks. For these analyses, the different versions of the singular choice tasks were pooled. To control for the effect of the lottery in the 2011 data, a dummy was included. Further information on the predictor variables can be found in Table 10 in the Appendix. Table 7 shows the results

Discussion

Our study pursued four main aims, two of which led to results in accordance with expectations: With regard to our aim to report population values for the subjective discount rate, we were able to demonstrate that in a large, nation-wide survey they are on average very high and well above market interest rates. Based on the serial choice tasks, the mean discount rate of the Swiss population is approximately 27% if extreme values of 100% and higher are excluded (both in 2007 and 2011) and 65%

Acknowledgments

The Swiss Environmental Survey 2007 was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation (project number 100012-107835), the Swiss Federal Office for the Environment, the cantons of Basel City (environment and energy office) and Zurich (waste, water, energy and air office), the central Swiss cantons (environment offices) and the Environment and Health Protection Offices of the city of Zurich. The Swiss Federal Statistical Office provided financial and methodological support but did neither

References (73)

  • A. Meier et al.

    Consumer discount rates implied by purchases of energy-efficient refrigerators

    Energy

    (1983)
  • D. Read et al.

    Time discounting over the lifespan

    Organizational Behav. Hum. Decision Processes

    (2004)
  • S. Reimers et al.

    Associations between a one-shot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior

    Pers. Individ. Differences

    (2009)
  • B.W. Roberts

    Back to the future: personality and assessment and personality development

    J. Res. Pers.

    (2009)
  • R.H. Thaler

    Some empirical evidece on dynamic inconsistency

    Econ. Lett.

    (1981)
  • K. Train

    Discount rates in consumers' energy-related decisions: a review of the literature

    Energy

    (1985)
  • L. Anderson et al.

    Individual decision-making experiments with risk and intertemporal choice

    J. Risk Uncertainty

    (2009)
  • R. Beck et al.

    Test–retest reliability of a group-administered paper-pencil measure of delay discounting

    Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol.

    (2009)
  • U. Benzion et al.

    Discount rates inferred from decisions: an experimental study

    Manage. Sci.

    (1989)
  • L. Borghans et al.

    The economics and psychology of personality traits

    J. Hum. Resour.

    (2008)
  • Bruderer Enzler, H., Consideration of Future Consequences as a Predictor of Environmentally Responsible Behavior....
  • Burks, S., Carpenter, J., Götte, L., Rustichini, A., 2011. Which Measures of Time Preference Best Predict Outcomes?...
  • C.F. Camerer et al.

    The effects of financial incentives in experiments: a review and capital-labor-production framework

    J. Risk Uncertainty

    (1999)
  • C. Chabris et al.

    Individual laboratory-measured discount rates predict field behavior

    J. Risk Uncertainty

    (2008)
  • M. Coller et al.

    Eliciting individual discount rates

    Exp. Econ.

    (1999)
  • P.T. Costa et al.

    Stability and change in personality from adolescence through adulthood

  • H.E. Daly et al.

    Ecological Economics: Principles and Applications

    (2011)
  • Daly, M., Delaney, L., Harmon, C., 2008. Psychological and Biological Foundations of Time Preference: Evidence from a...
  • DEFRA, 2010. Behavioural Economics and Energy Using Products. Scoping Research on Discounting Behaviour and Consumer...
  • A. Diekmann et al.

    Dokumentation Projekt “Zeitpräferenzen und Energiesparen”. Eine Nachbefragung zum Schweizer Umweltsurvey 2007

    (2012)
  • Diekmann, A., Meyer, R., 2008. Schweizer Umweltsurvey 2007. Dokumentation und Codebuch. Zürich, Professur für...
  • Diekmann, A., Preisendörfer, P., 2001. Umweltsoziologie. Reinbek bei Hamburg,...
  • A. Duckworth et al.

    Is it really self-control? Examining the predictive power of the delay of gratification test

    Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull.

    (2013)
  • Epper, T., Fehr-Duda, H., Schubert, R., 2011. Energy-Using Durables: The Role of Time Discounting in Investment...
  • Fehr, E., Leibbrandt, A., 2008. Cooperativeness and Impatience in the Tragedy of the Commons. Working Paper. Institute...
  • S. Frederick et al.

    Time discounting and time preferences: a critical review

    J. Econ. Lit.

    (2002)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text