Social norms and energy conservation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.03.003Get rights and content

Abstract

This paper evaluates a series of programs run by a company called OPOWER to send Home Energy Report letters to residential utility customers comparing their electricity use to that of their neighbors. Using data from randomized natural field experiments at 600,000 treatment and control households across the United States, I estimate that the average program reduces energy consumption by 2.0%. The program provides additional evidence that non-price interventions can substantially and cost effectively change consumer behavior: the effect is equivalent to that of a short-run electricity price increase of 11 to 20%, and the cost effectiveness compares favorably to that of traditional energy conservation programs. Perhaps because the treatment included descriptive social norms, effects are heterogeneous: households in the highest decile of pre-treatment consumption decrease usage by 6.3%, while consumption by the lowest decile decreases by only 0.3%. A regression discontinuity design shows that different categories of “injunctive norms” played an insignificant role in encouraging relatively low users not to increase usage.

Research Highlights

► This paper evaluates effects of mailing social comparisons to residential energy users. ► The average program reduces energy consumption by 2.0 percent. ► Cost effectiveness compares favorably to traditional energy conservation programs. ► Heterogeneous treatment effects, and highest users decrease usage by 6.3 percent. ► RD shows that injunctive norm categorizations had insignificantly different effects.

Introduction

Climate change has emerged as one of the most important economic policy issues of the early 21st century, and many view energy efficiency as an appealing approach to reducing greenhouse gas emissions. Traditionally, economists and policymakers have focused on relative prices as the primary force driving energy demand. As a result, carbon cap-and-trade programs are the centerpiece of proposed climate change policies, and subsidies for energy efficient durable goods draw the vast majority of public energy efficiency funding in the U.S. (Gillingham et al., 2006).

There are three problems with price-based approaches to energy conservation. First, it has not been politically feasible to implement Pigouvian carbon taxes or a carbon emissions trading program in the U.S., suggesting that average wholesale energy prices are below social cost. Second, measuring the effects of an energy efficiency subsidy on energy use requires knowledge of the elasticities of demand for energy efficient durable goods and for energy conditional on capital stock. Lacking context-specific values of these parameters, subsidy-based programs are typically evaluated using a controversial approach called “deemed savings”; randomized controlled impact evaluations are exceedingly rare. A third problem is that while subsidies are in theory innocuous because they are transfers, they are in practice a large drain on increasingly-limited public funds.

Spurred by these problems, interest has dramatically increased in non-price energy conservation programs that are informed by insights from behavioral science and evaluated via randomized trials. Non-price interventions are typically inexpensive relative to subsidies, and as demonstrated by Bertrand et al. (2010) in the context of consumer finance, carefully-crafted psychological cues can have effects on demand that are comparable to large changes in relative prices. A critical challenge, however, is to craft interventions that are powerful and cost-effective when implemented at large scale.

This paper examines one of the most notable non-price energy conservation programs, which is run by a company called OPOWER. OPOWER mails Home Energy Report letters (HERs) that compare a household's energy use to that of similar neighbors and provide energy conservation tips. The neighbor comparisons were directly influenced by academic work showing that providing social norm information induces people to conserve energy (Schultz et al., 2007, Nolan et al., 2008). More broadly, the program was motivated by similar evidence on the power of social norms in a variety of domains, including voting (Gerber and Rogers, 2009), retirement savings (Beshears et al., 2009), and charitable giving (Frey and Meier, 2004). As of the end of 2010, OPOWER had contracts to run programs at 47 utilities in 21 states, including six of the largest ten utilities in the U.S.

The first parts of this paper are an impact evaluation of all of the OPOWER programs begun before the end of 2009. With nearly 600,000 households in treatment and control groups, this is one of the largest randomized field experiments in history. I show that the point estimates of the Average Treatment Effects (ATEs) of OPOWER's first 17 experiments range from 1.4 to 3.3%, with an unweighted mean of 2%.1 While there is often concern over the durability of treatment effects in similar non-price interventions (Ferraro and Price, 2010), the Home Energy Reports appear to have constant or increasing effects as they are repeatedly delivered over the first two years of treatment.

These effect sizes have several different economic interpretations. First, different energy conservation programs are typically compared on a basis of program implementation cost per kilowatt-hour of electricity saved. OPOWER's initial set of programs have cost effectiveness ranging from 1.3 to 5.4 cents per kilowatt-hour, with an unweighted mean of 3.3. These results compare favorably to estimates for traditional energy efficiency programs, and because they are estimated using randomized trials, they are much more certain. The welfare effects, however, are ambiguous: the costs that households incur to reduce energy use are unobserved, as is the change in welfare from learning that one compares favorably or poorly to neighbors.

A second way of interpreting effect sizes is to calculate the energy price changes that would induce the same changes in demand. Calibrating with estimated price elasticities, I show that the effects of sending Home Energy Reports are equivalent to a 11 to 20% short-run price increase or a 5% long run price increase. Taken as a whole, these effects are remarkable: simply sending letters can significantly and cost-effectively affect energy use behaviors.

The remainder of the paper builds on theoretical predictions of heterogeneous treatment effects. In particular, many models predict that the “descriptive norm” element of the Home Energy Report treatment, in which a household's energy use is compared to that of its neighbors, would cause households that previously used more than the norm to decrease usage, but would cause households that used less than the norm to use more. Social psychologists call these unintended consequences “boomerang effects” (Clee and Wicklund, 1980), and they are certainly undesirable if the objective is to induce energy conservation. Combining data across all of OPOWER's experiments, I show that Conditional Average Treatment Effects are larger than 6% in the highest decile of pre-treatment usage and close to zero in the lowest decile, but even these households that compare most favorably to their neighbors do not increase energy use. In this sense, the OPOWER intervention does not cause a “descriptive norm boomerang effect.”

The Schultz et al. (2007) experiment that motivated OPOWER's work had found a boomerang effect for relatively low users. To combat this, they employed what social psychologists call “injunctive norms,” which convey that energy conservation is pro-social (Cialdini et al., 1990). Specifically, they added a treatment condition that included hand-drawn “smiley faces” on the descriptive norm feedback reports given to these relatively low users. Although the group of low users that received this injunctive norm did not use statistically significantly less energy than the group that that did not, this group's increase in energy use was also not statistically distinguishable from zero. Based on this result, it was believed that injunctive norms could eliminate the boomerang effect.

OPOWER's Home Energy Reports therefore include injunctive norms, which are defined based on sharp cutoffs. Households are labeled as “Great” if they use less than the 20th percentile of their neighbor comparison group, “Below Average” if they use more than the mean, and “Good” if they are in between. The “Great” group receives two “smiley face” emoticons, the “Good” group receives one, and the “Below Average” group initially received “frownie faces” until customer complaints ended this practice. The treatment effects are substantially different across the three groups, although this could be caused either by the categorizations or by other factors correlated with baseline energy use that could affect how households respond to the treatment: for example, high users may have lower-cost opportunities to conserve. Notice, however, that households that had used just more energy than the 20th percentile of their comparison group are in the limit identical to households that use just less energy, but the former are labeled “Good,” while the latter are labeled “Great.” Similarly, households using just more than the mean of their comparison group were labeled “Below Average,” while households using just less were labeled “Good.”

I use a regression discontinuity (RD) design to test for whether these normative categorizations cause differential effects on energy use. I show that while the treatment effects differ substantially for households in the three different categories, the causal effects of the categorizations themselves are “tightly estimated zeros.” Being labeled “Good” instead of “Great” has a differential treatment effect of less than 0.20 percentage points, or about one-tenth of the ATE. Being labeled “Below Average” instead of “Good” has a differential ATE of less than 0.16 percentage points. Therefore, the fact that we do not observe a descriptive norm boomerang effect is likely due not to the different categorizations. Instead, the potential effect is likely mitigated by the energy conservation tips or other aspects of the injuntive norms that affect all categories equally.

The paper proceeds by first giving more detail on the treatment and potential pathways of effects. The rest of Section 2 then provides background and descriptive statistics on OPOWER's experiments. Section 3 details the average treatment effects, from the econometric strategy to the parameter estimates and resulting cost effectiveness. In the spirit of Lalonde (1986), this section also documents the poor performance of non-experimental estimators. Section 4 discusses heterogeneous treatment effects and the RD design. Section 5 concludes.

Section snippets

The treatment and mechanisms of effects

The Home Energy Reports are several-page letters with two key components. The first is the Social Comparison Module, which appears at the top of the letter's first page. As illustrated in Fig. 1, the graph on the left side of the Social Comparison presents the “descriptive norm” by comparing the household to the mean and 20th percentile of its comparison group. A household's comparison group comprises approximately 100 geographically-proximate houses with similar characteristics, including

Estimation

The initial estimand of interest is the Average Treatment Effect τ = E[Yit(1)  Yit(0)] in the population of experimental households, where Yit(1) and Yit(0) denote the “potential outcomes” for household i's electricity use at time t if the household were treated and were not treated, respectively (Rubin, 1974). As some households opted out, the “Treatment” here is defined as “being mailed the Home Energy Reports or actively opting out.”5

Heterogeneous treatment effects

While the empirical focus so far has been on Average Treatment Effects, theory predicts that treatment effects could vary over time and across households. Although there is some heterogeneity on other observed characteristics, the primary observable source of heterogeneity is as a function of pre-treatment usage (Allcott, 2009). This could be high-usage households can reduce consumption at lower cost, or alternatively because social norm information has differential effects for households in

Conclusion

This paper evaluates the effects of the OPOWER Home Energy Reports, which give households feedback on past energy consumption, compare them to their neighbors, and provide energy conservation tips. The program is a remarkable departure from traditional energy efficiency programs in that it is a non-price intervention designed with direct insight from behavioral science that is evaluated using randomized controlled trials. The perceived success or failure of these pilot programs will directly

Acknowledgements

I thank, without implicating, Ian Ayres, Bob Cialdini, Tyler Curtis, Rajeev Dehejia, Kenneth Gillingham, Larry Goulder, Michael Greenstone, Matt Harding, Kosuke Imai, Seema Jayachandran, Karthik Kalyanaraman, Alex Kaufman, Ogi Kavazovic, Alex Laskey, Aprajit Mahajan, Justin Marion, Sendhil Mullainathan, Dave Rapson, Todd Rogers, Eldar Shafir, Joe Shapiro, Lan Shi, Marc Solomon, Dmitry Taubinsky, two anonymous referees, and seminar participants at the Congressional Budget Office, the

References (56)

  • Gary Becker

    A Theory on the Allocation of Time

    Economic Journal

    (1965)
  • Mark Berger et al.

    Evaluating Profiling as a Means of Allocating Government Services

  • Marianne Bertrand et al.

    How Much Should We Trust Difference-in-Differences Estimates?

    Quarterly Journal of Economics

    (2004)
  • Bertrand, Marianne, Karlan, Dean, Mullainathan, Sendhil, Shafir, Eldar, and Zinman, Jonathan, 2010. What's Advertising...
  • John Beshears et al.

    The Effect of Providing Peer Information on Retirement Savings Decisions

  • Hongbin Cai et al.

    Observational Learning: Evidence from a Randomized Natural Field Experiment

    American Economic Review

    (2009)
  • Robert Cialdini et al.

    A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct: Recycling the Concept of Norms to Reduce Littering in Public Places

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1990)
  • Mona Clee et al.

    Consumer Behavior and Psychological Reactance

    Journal of Consumer Research

    (1980)
  • Conley, Timothy and Udry, Christopher, 2010. Learning About a New Technology: Pineapple in Ghana. American Economic...
  • Dora Costa et al.

    Energy Conservation Nudges and Environmentalist Ideology: Evidence from a Randomized Residential Electricity Field Experiment

  • Sarah Darby

    The Effectiveness of Feedback on Energy Consumption

  • Lucas Davis

    Durable Goods and Residential Demand for Energy and Water: Evidence from a Field Trial

    RAND Journal of Economics

    (2008)
  • Stefano DellaVigna et al.

    Testing for Altruism and Social Pressure in Charitable Giving

  • Jeffrey Dubin et al.

    An Econometric Analysis of Residential Electric Appliance Holdings and Consumption

    Econometrica

    (1984)
  • Paul Ferraro et al.

    Using Non-Pecuniary Strategies to Influence Behavior: Evidence from a Large-Scale Field Experiment

  • Sergio Firpo

    Efficient Semiparametric Estimation of Quantile Treatment Effects

    Econometrica

    (2007)
  • Andrew Foster et al.

    Learning by Doing and Learning from Others: Human Capital and Technical Change in Agriculture

    Journal of Political Economy

    (1995)
  • Bruno Frey et al.

    Social Comparisons and Pro-Social Behavior: Testing ‘Conditional Cooperation’ in a Field Experiment

    American Economic Review

    (2004)
  • Cited by (1781)

    • On the perils of environmentally friendly alternatives

      2024, Journal of Environmental Economics and Management
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text