Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 181, 1 November 2018, Pages 461-470
NeuroImage

Neural mechanisms of the EEG alpha-BOLD anticorrelation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.031Get rights and content

Abstract

An experimentally tested neural field theory of the corticothalamic system is used to model brain activity and resulting experimental EEG data, and to elucidate the neural mechanisms and physiological basis of alpha-BOLD anticorrelation observed in concurrent EEG and fMRI measurements. Several studies have proposed that the anticorrelation originates from a causal link between changes in the alpha power and BOLD signal. However, the results in this study reveal that fluctuations in alpha and BOLD power do not generate one another but instead respectively result from high- and low-frequency components of the same underlying cortical activity, and that they are inversely correlated via variations in the strengths of corticothalamic and intrathalamic feedback, thereby explaining their anticorrelation.

Introduction

Scalp encephalography (EEG) is widely used to noninvasively record cortical activity aggregated at spatial resolutions of few centimeters on a millisecond time scale. Measurements using EEG have shown that spontaneous brain activity includes some resonant oscillations, one of the most dominant in normal adult humans being the 7.5–12 Hz alpha rhythm (Berger, 1929), which is especially prominent during a waking resting state with the eyes closed.

Since its discovery, the alpha rhythm has been used as a marker of the level of arousal, vigilance, and attention (Strijkstra et al., 2003; Babiloni et al., 2004; Niedermeyer and Lopes da Silva, 2005; Thut et al., 2006; Rihs et al., 2007; Olbrich et al., 2009). Several works have also argued that it plays an important role in the perception of external stimuli (Babiloni et al., 2006; Thut et al., 2006; Haegens et al., 2011; Händel et al., 2011) and many cognitive abilities (Klimesch, 1999; Başar et al., 2001; Jensen et al., 2002; Sauseng et al., 2005; Zumer et al., 2014).

Because EEG is spatially coarse-grained, many recent studies have used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), based on the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal, to probe brain dynamics at finer spatial resolution (1 mm) but on a longer time scale (seconds) corresponding to the dynamics of spontaneous BOLD fluctuations. Notably, covariances of BOLD fluctuations are often used to define functional connectivity between discrete brain regions (Fox and Raichle, 2007). Hence, there is a strong interest in exploring the links between EEG and BOLD measurements to better understand structure–function relationships.

Even though EEG and fMRI investigate brain function in different scales, their signals are tightly coupled and originate from the same underlying neural dynamics. In particular, EEG is recorded after volume conduction effects have removed high spatial frequencies of the neural activity (Nunez et al., 1994; Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006). In contrast, fMRI measures BOLD signal resulting from slow hemodynamic responses (typically in frequencies 0.2 Hz) that suppress high temporal frequencies of the neural activity (Zarahn et al., 1997; Robinson et al., 2006; He et al., 2008; Nir et al., 2008; Scheeringa et al., 2011; Aquino et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2014). Thus, they can be combined to complement one another and obtain data with high spatial and temporal resolutions [see for example the reviews of Ritter and Villringer (2006) and Laufs et al. (2008)].

The fusion of EEG and fMRI has led several studies to investigate the relationship of the alpha rhythm and the BOLD signal. These studies have shown that, in various brain structures, fluctuations in the BOLD signal are correlated or anticorrelated with fluctuations in the alpha-rhythm power (Goldman et al., 2002; Laufs et al., 2003; Moosmann et al., 2003; Gonçalves et al., 2006; Danos et al., 2001; Feige et al., 2005). However, these correlations or anticorrelations are not consistently found across different studies, which is possibly due to high inter- and intra-subject variabilities (Gonçalves et al., 2006), or methodological factors such as differences in recording paradigms and the manner in which the alpha-rhythm power was quantified from EEG recordings. Nonetheless, the most robust finding is for the occipital cortex where alpha is strongest and modulations of its power are negatively correlated with those of the BOLD signal. This concurs with several studies using other neuroimaging modalities that show similar relationships between fluctuations in alpha-rhythm power and those in blood flow and oxygenation signals (Jacquy et al., 1980; Sadato et al., 1998; Moosmann et al., 2003).

Even with the abundance of statistical evidence relating the alpha rhythm and the BOLD signal, the underlying physiological mechanisms of the observed alpha-BOLD anticorrelations have not been established. Some studies have conjectured that a decrease in occipital BOLD signal is a result of ‘idling’ of cortex and decreased neural activity (Chawla et al., 1999; Wenzel et al., 2000; Mayhew et al., 2013), or alternatively linked to other functionally coupled processes such as vigilance (Moosmann et al., 2003) and wakefulness (Horovitz et al., 2008); however, mechanistic links remain elusive and concepts such as ‘idling’ remain poorly defined.

Most studies have proposed that there is a direct causal link between fluctuations in the alpha power and the BOLD signal, as shown in Fig. 1a (Goldman et al., 2002; Laufs et al., 2003; Moosmann et al., 2003; Schirner et al., 2018). This was found by employing data processing steps, including high-pass filtering the EEG signal (usually > 0.5 Hz), calculating the power spectrum from the squared amplitude of the filtered signal's Fourier transform, and convolving the time course of the alpha power with an a priori hemodynamic response function (HRF) before comparing with the BOLD signal. These processing steps have the following shortcomings. First, high-pass filtering the EEG signal inevitably throws away the important frequencies that mainly drive the slow activity of BOLD. Second, it is well known that the HRF can take into account the slow processes mediating neural activity and BOLD; hence the neural signal itself, not its power, should be convolved with the HRF to predict the BOLD signal (Friston et al., 2000). This is what convolution of the alpha power with the HRF described above aims to achieve, but we argue that it does not follow the actual neural-HRF-BOLD relationship. Moreover, even though the convolution culminates to a low-frequency signal that appears to be comparable to BOLD, it represents the windowed low-frequency envelope of alpha, not the original low-frequency neural signal that drives BOLD.

In order to overcome the abovementioned shortcomings and to not introduce confounds caused by ad hoc data processing, we instead directly extracted the low-frequency power of the actual EEG signal because it best represents the slow activity of BOLD. In this way, we can relate the powers of the low-frequency and alpha bands, which have identical dimensions. Thus, the type of analysis in this study is certainly different from what has been done previously, but it investigates the mechanisms underlying the alpha-BOLD anticorrelation in a new way that has a more compelling physical basis. In particular, we aim to show that fluctuations in the alpha power do not drive those in the BOLD power, nor vice versa. Instead, they respectively result from high- and low-frequency components of the same underlying cortical activity, which ensue from variations in the strengths of corticothalamic and intrathalamic feedback, as shown in Fig. 1b.

Here, we use an experimentally tested and verified neural field theory of the corticothalamic system (Robinson et al., 2001, 2002, 2004, 2005) to model and analyze experimental EEG data recorded for a cohort of subjects in a relaxed waking eyes-closed state. First, properties of the low-frequency (combination of the < 1 Hz slow-wave and the 1–4 Hz delta-wave frequencies) and alpha bands of the EEG power spectra are analyzed to show that the anticorrelation between alpha and BOLD power (proxied by the low-frequency power) can be reproduced for a large group of subjects. Then, the model is used to fit the data and obtain time series of estimated corticothalamic gains within a single subject and show the validity of the causal relationship in Fig. 1b.

Section snippets

Theory and methods

In this section, we describe the experimental EEG data used in this study and the method to quantify their power spectral properties. We then briefly outline key details of the neural field model used to explain and further analyze the underlying neural dynamics. Finally, the method to fit the model to the data and the process to infer BOLD power are summarized.

Results and discussion

Here, we analyze the relaxed waking eyes-closed dataset discussed in Sec. 2.1 to test whether the anticorrelations between fluctuations in alpha and BOLD power can be obtained by analyses of EEG power spectral properties. Then, we fit the data with the neural field model, as discussed in Sec. 2.4, to obtain time series of the power spectral properties and corticothalamic loop gains of a single subject. Finally, temporal correlations between the mentioned quantities are calculated to

Conclusions

We have analyzed the EEG spectra of a cohort of subjects in a relaxed waking eyes-closed state to explain the existence of the alpha-BOLD anticorrelation.

First, we have used the low-frequency power of EEG as a proxy for BOLD power. This argument rests on the findings of previous studies showing that the BOLD signal corresponds to low frequencies (0.2 Hz) (Zarahn et al., 1997; Robinson et al., 2006; He et al., 2008; Nir et al., 2008; Scheeringa et al., 2011; Aquino et al., 2012; Wang et al.,

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgements

We thank R.G. Abeysuriya for his assistance regarding data processing. We acknowledge the support of the Brain Resource International Database for EEG data acquisition and processing. This work was supported by the Australian Research Council Center of Excellence for Integrative Brain Function (ARC Center of Excellence grant CE140100007), the Australian Research Council Laureate Fellowship Grant FL140100025, and the Australian Research Council Discovery Project Grant DP170101778.

References (70)

  • G. Gratton et al.

    A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1983)
  • R.M. Hutchison et al.

    Electrophysiological signatures of spontaneous BOLD fluctuations in macaque prefrontal cortex

    Neuroimage

    (2015)
  • K. Jann et al.

    BOLD correlates of EEG alpha phase-locking and the fMRI default mode network

    Neuroimage

    (2009)
  • W. Klimesch

    EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis

    Brain Res. Rev.

    (1999)
  • H. Laufs et al.

    Recent advances in recording electrophysiological data simultaneously with magnetic resonance imaging

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • H. Laufs et al.

    EEG-correlated fMRI of human alpha activity

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • F.H. Lopes da Silva et al.

    The cortical source of the alpha rhythm

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (1977)
  • S.D. Mayhew et al.

    Spontaneous EEG alpha oscillation interacts with positive and negative BOLD responses in the visual–auditory cortices and default-mode network

    Neuroimage

    (2013)
  • M. Moosmann et al.

    Correlates of alpha rhythm in functional magnetic resonance imaging and near infrared spectroscopy

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • P.L. Nunez et al.

    A theoretical and experimental study of high resolution EEG based on surface Laplacians and cortical imaging

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1994)
  • S. Olbrich et al.

    EEG-vigilance and BOLD effect during simultaneous EEG/fMRI measurement

    Neuroimage

    (2009)
  • J.C. Pang et al.

    Effects of astrocytic dynamics on spatiotemporal hemodynamics: modeling and enhanced data analysis

    Neuroimage

    (2017)
  • P. Ritter et al.

    Simultaneous EEG–fMRI

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2006)
  • P.A. Robinson et al.

    BOLD responses to stimuli: dependence on frequency, stimulus form, amplitude, and repetition rate

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • R. Scheeringa et al.

    Neuronal dynamics underlying high- and low-frequency EEG oscillations contribute independently to the human BOLD signal

    Neuron

    (2011)
  • A.M. Strijkstra et al.

    Subjective sleepiness correlates negatively with global alpha (8–12 Hz) and positively with central frontal theta (4–8 Hz) frequencies in the human resting awake electroencephalogram

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2003)
  • P.-C. Tu et al.

    Network-specific cortico-thalamic dysconnection in schizophrenia revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity analyses

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2015)
  • K.M. Aquino et al.

    Hemodynamic traveling waves in human visual cortex

    PLoS Comput. Biol.

    (2012)
  • C. Babiloni et al.

    Pre-and poststimulus alpha rhythms are related to conscious visual perception: a high-resolution EEG study

    Cerebr. Cortex

    (2006)
  • H. Berger

    Über das elektrenkephalogramm des menschen

    Eur. Arch. Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci.

    (1929)
  • D. Chawla et al.

    The relationship between synchronization among neuronal populations and their mean activity levels

    Neural Comput.

    (1999)
  • T.T. Dang-Vu et al.

    Spontaneous neural activity during human slow wave sleep

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

    (2008)
  • P. Danos et al.

    EEG alpha rhythm and glucose metabolic rate in the thalamus in schizophrenia

    Neuropsychobiology

    (2001)
  • B. Feige et al.

    Cortical and subcortical correlates of electroencephalographic alpha rhythm modulation

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2005)
  • M.D. Fox et al.

    Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • Cited by (14)

    • Alpha oscillations reflect suppression of distractors with increased perceptual load

      2022, Progress in Neurobiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Importantly, BOLD responses were decreased for irrelevant distractor stimuli. BOLD responses are typically negatively correlated with alpha-band power (Goldman et al., 2002; Pang and Robinson, 2018; Scheeringa et al., 2009). Similarly, using spectroscopy, Bruckmaier et al. (2020) recently showed increased cellular metabolism for attended target and a reduction for unattended stimuli with increased perceptual load.

    • Neural basis of the visual working memory deficit in schizophrenia: Merging evidence from fMRI and EEG

      2021, Schizophrenia Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      One possible explanation for the observed dissociation between HCS and PSZ may be between-group differences in thalamocortical connectivity. Recent modeling work has revealed that the correlation typically observed between alpha power and BOLD represents an emergent property of variation in corticothalamic and intrathalamic functional connectivity (Pang and Robinson, 2018). Together with reports that posterior thalamic nuclei exhibit hyperconnectivity with posterior cortical regions in PSZ (Anticevic et al., 2014; Cheng et al., 2015; Ramsay, 2019), it may be that between-group differences in thalamocortical connectivity can account for the inconsistent pattern of correlation between HCS and PSZ.

    • Accurate assessment of low-function autistic children based on EEG feature fusion

      2021, Journal of Clinical Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Because of its non-invasive, portable and maneuverability, EEG has become the most preferred brain recording method in clinical diagnosis, laboratory research and many other applications [8,9], and it is also a powerful tool for analyzing and evaluating patients with brain diseases [10]. Resting state EEG can effectively evaluate neural activity, and also detect neural oscillation mode without performing tasks or sensory stimulation [11,12]. EEG signals can be generally divided into five frequency bands including delta (1-4hz), theta (4-8hz), alpha (8-13hz), beta (13-35hz) and gamma (above 35hz) [13].

    • EEG spatiospectral patterns and their link to fMRI BOLD signal via variable hemodynamic response functions

      2019, Journal of Neuroscience Methods
      Citation Excerpt :

      Supra-threshold voxels are not present within the basal ganglia for the low β-band frequencies, where we instead observe a strong correspondence with voxels within left sensory-motor areas likely associated with the VOT right hand button press (Fig. 6, Table 2). The observed antagonistic relationship between non-α-band and α-band patterns and corresponding BOLD responses (Figs. 4a and 5c) is supported by (Pang and Robinson, 2018)⁠. EEG α-band responses often demonstrate negative relationships with fMRI, since increase in the α-band reflect neural disengagement (e.g. inhibition-timings hypothesis (Klimesch et al., 2007)⁠) and a reduced metabolic state.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text