Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T12:54:12.363Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - A Neurofunctional Theory of Consciousness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 February 2010

Jesse J. Prinz
Affiliation:
Associate Professor of Philosophy, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Andrew Brook
Affiliation:
Carleton University, Ottawa
Kathleen Akins
Affiliation:
Simon Fraser University, British Columbia
Get access

Summary

Introduction: The Problems of Consciousness

Reading the philosophical literature on consciousness, one might get the idea that there is just one problem in consciousness studies, the hard problem. That would be a mistake. There are other problems; some are more tractable, but none is easy, and all interesting. The literature on the hard problem gives the impression that we have made little progress. Consciousness is just an excuse to work and rework familiar positions on the mind–body problem. But progress is being made elsewhere. Researchers are moving toward increasingly specific accounts of the neural basis of conscious experience. These efforts will leave some questions unanswered, but they are no less significant for that.

To move beyond the hard problem, I would like to consider some real problems facing consciousness researchers. First, there is a What Problem. This is the problem of figuring out what we are conscious of. What are the contents of conscious experience? For those looking at the brain, it is closely tied to a Where Problem. Where in the brain does consciousness arise? Locating consciousness may not be enough. We need to address a How Problem. How do certain states come to be conscious? This can be construed as a version of the hard problem, by asking it with right intonation: How could certain physical states possibly be experienced? But there is another reading that is also worth investigating.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cognition and the Brain
The Philosophy and Neuroscience Movement
, pp. 381 - 396
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baars, B. J. (1997). In the Theater of Consciousness. New York: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar, M., and Biederman, I. (1998). Subliminal visual priming. Psychological Science, 9, 464–469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baylis, G. C., and Driver, J. (2001). Shape-coding in IT cells generalizes over contrast and mirror reversal, but not figure-ground reversal. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 937–942CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisiach, E. (1992). Understanding consciousness: Clues from unilateral neglect and related disorders. In Milner, A. D. and Rugg, M. D. (eds.), The Neuropsychology of Consciousness (113–139). London: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Kelley, W. M., and Petersen, S. E. (1999). Frontal cortex contributes to human memory formation. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 311–314CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corchs, S., and Deco, G. (2002). Large-scale neural model for visual attention: Integration of experimental single-cell and fMRI data. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 339–348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, F., and Koch, C. (1995). Are we aware of activity in primary visual cortex? Nature, 37, 121–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell'Acqua, R., and Grainger, J. (1999). Unconscious semantic priming from pictures. Cognition, 73, B1–B15CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diekamp, B., Kalt, T., and Güntürkün, O. (2002). Working memory neurons in pigeons. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, RC 210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Driver, J. (1996). What can visual neglect and extinction reveal about the extent of “preattentive” processing? In Kramer, A. F., Coles, M. G. H., and Logan, G. D. (eds.), Convergent Operations in the Study of Visual Selective Attention (193–224). Washington, DC: APA PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farah, Martha J. (1990). Visual Agnosia: Disorders of Object Recognition and What They Tell Us About Normal Vision. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Ffytche, D. H., Howard, R. J., Brammer, M. J., David, A., Woodruff, P. W., and Williams, S. (1998). The anatomy of conscious vision: An fMRI study of visual hallucinations. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 738–742CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, D. P., Murray, E. A., O'Neil, B., Mishkin, M. (1986). Cortical connections of the somatosensory fields of the lateral sulcus of Macaques: Evidence of a corticolimbic pathway for touch. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 252, 323–347CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, P., Reynolds, J. H., Rorie, A. E., and Desimone, R. (2001). Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science, 291, 1560–1563CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izaki, Y., Maruki, K., Hori, K., and Nomura, M. (2001). Effects of rat medial prefrontal cortex temporal inactivation on a delayed alternation task. Neuroscience Letters, 315, 129–132CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the Computational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Kaas, J. H. (1993). The functional organization of somatosensory cortex in primates. Annals of Anatomy, 175, 509–518CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaas, J. H., and Hackett, T. A. (2000). Subdivisions of auditory cortex and processing streams in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 97, 11793–11799CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leopold, D. A., and Logothetis, N. K. (1996). Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys' percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature, 379, 549–553CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logothetis, N. (1998). Object vision and visual awareness. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 536–544CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S. J., Vogel, E. K., and Shapiro, K. L. (1996). Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink. Nature, 383, 616–618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mack, A., and Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional Blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. New York: W. H. FreemanGoogle Scholar
Marshall, J. C., and Halligan, P. W. (1988). Blindsight and insight in visuospatial neglect. Nature, 336, 766–767CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mather, J. A. (1991). Navigation by spatial memory and use of visual landmarks in octopuses. Journal of Comparative Physiology, A, 168, 491–497CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendola, J. D., Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., Liu, A. K., and Tootell, R. B. H. (1999). The representation of illusory and real contours in human cortical visual areas revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 8560–8572CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menzel, R. (1984). Short-term memory in bees. In Alkon, D. L. and Farley, J. (eds.), Primary Neural Substrates of Learning and Behavior Change (259–274). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A., and Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 5154–5167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakamura, K., and Kubota, K. (1995). Mnemonic firing of neurons in the monkey temporal pole during a visual recognition task. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 162–178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niebur, E., Koch, C., and Rosin, C. (1993). An oscillation-based model for the neuronal basis of attention. Vision Research, 18, 2789–2802CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olshausen, B. A., Anderson, C. H., and Essen, D. C. (1994). A neurobiological model of visual attention and invariant pattern recognition based task. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 6171–6186Google Scholar
Prinz, J. J. (2000). A neurofunctional theory of visual consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 9, 243–259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinz, J. J. (2001). Functionalism, dualism and the neural correlates of consciousness. In Bechtel, W., Mandik, P., Mundale, J., and Stufflebeam, R. (eds.), Philosophy and the Neurosciences: A Reader (278–294). Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Prinz, J. J. (2003). Level-headed mysterianism and artificial experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, 111–132Google Scholar
Prinz, J. J. (2004). Gut Reactions: A Perceptual Theory of Emotion. New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Ramsden, B. M., Chou, P. H., and Roe, A. W. (2001). Real and illusory contour processing in area V1 of the primate: A cortical balancing act. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 648–665CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, G. (2001). Neuroimaging of visual awareness in patients and normal subjects. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 150–156CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, G., Kreiman, G., and Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 261–270CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, G., Wojciulik, E., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Frith, C., and Driver, J. (2000). Unconscious activation of visual cortex in the damaged right hemisphere of a parietal patient with extinction. Brain, 123, 1624–1633CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. T. (1998). The orbitofrontal cortex. In Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., and Weiskrantz, L. (eds.), The Prefrontal Cortex (67–86). Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Sahraie, A., Weiskrantz, L., Barbur, J. L., Simmons, A., Williams, S. C. R., and Brammer, M. J. (1997). Pattern of neuronal activity associated with conscious and unconscious processing of visual signals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 94, 9406–9411CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savic, I., Gulyas, B., Larsson, M., and Roland, P. (2000). Olfactory functions are mediated by parallel and hierarchical processing. Neuron, 26, 735–745CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seghier, M., Dojat, M., Delon-Martin, C., Rubin, C., Warnking, J., Segebarth, C., and Bullier, J. (2000). Moving illusory contours activate primary visual cortex: An fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 663–670CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seguin, E. G. (1886). A contribution to the pathology of hemianopsis of central origin (cortex-hemianopsia). Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 13, 1–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheinberg, D. L., and Logothetis, N. K. (1997). The role of temporal cortical areas in perceptual organizations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 94, 3408–3413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simons, Daniel J., and Chabris, Christopher F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vignolo, L. A. (1982). Auditory agnosia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 298, 49–57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuilleumier, P., Henson, R. N., Driver, J., and Dolan, R. J. (2002). Multiple levels of visual object constancy revealed by event-related fMRI of repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 491–499CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuilleumier, P., Sagiv, N., Hazeltine, E., Poldrack, R. A., Swick, D., Rafal, R. D., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2001). Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: A combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 98, 3495–3500CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamada, A., Sekiguchi, T., Suzuki, H., Mizukami, A. (1992). Behavioral analysis of internal memory states using cooling-induced retrograde amnesia in Limax flavus. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 729–735CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeki, S. (1993). A Vision of the Brain. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×