Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T16:06:37.062Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

“That's really clever!” Ironic hyperbole understanding in children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 May 2017

MARC AGUERT
Affiliation:
Université de Caen Normandie, EA 7452, France
CORALIE LE VALLOIS
Affiliation:
Center for Early Medical and Social Action, Saint-Lô, France
KARINE MARTEL
Affiliation:
Université de Caen Normandie, EA 7452, France
VIRGINIE LAVAL
Affiliation:
Université de Poitiers, UMR 6234, France

Abstract

Hyperbole supports irony comprehension in adults by heightening the contrast between what is said and the actual situation. Because young children do not perceive the communication situation as a whole, but rather give precedence to either the utterance or the context, we predicted that hyperbole would reduce irony comprehension in six-year-olds (n = 40) by overemphasizing what was said. By contrast, ten-year-olds (n = 40) would benefit from hyperbole in the way that adults do, as they would perceive the utterance and context as a whole, highlighted by the speaker's ironic intent. Short animated cartoons featuring ironic criticisms were shown to participants. We assessed comprehension of the speaker's belief and speaker's intent. Results supported our predictions. The development of mentalization during school years and its impact on the development of irony comprehension is discussed.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Achim, A. M., Guitton, M., Jackson, P. L., Boutin, A., & Monetta, L. (2013). On what ground do we mentalize? Characteristics of current tasks and sources of information that contribute to mentalizing judgments. Psychological Assessment, 25(1), 117126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ackerman, B. P. (1983). Form and function in children's understanding of ironic utterances. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 487508.Google Scholar
Aguert, M., & Laval, V. (2013). Request complexity is no more a problem when the requests are ironic. Pragmatics & Cognition, 21(2), 329339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S. (2000). Irony as relevant inappropriateness. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 793826.Google Scholar
Attardo, S., Eisterhold, J., Hay, J., & Poggi, I. (2003). Multimodal markers of irony and sarcasm. Humor, 16, 243260.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390412.Google Scholar
Bernicot, J., Laval, V., & Chaminaud, S. (2007). Nonliteral language forms in children: In what order are they acquired in pragmatics and metapragmatics? Journal of Pragmatics, 39, 21152132.Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2004). PRAAT, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Retrieved from <www.praat.org>..>Google Scholar
Burgers, C., Brugman, B. C., Renardel de Lavalette, K. Y., & Steen, G. J. (2016). HIP: a method for linguistic hyperbole identification in discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 31(3), 163178.Google Scholar
Burgers, C., van Mulken, M., & Schellens, P. J. (2012). Type of evaluation and marking of irony: the role of perceived complexity and comprehension. Journal of Pragmatics, 44(3), 231242.Google Scholar
Carston, R., & Wearing, C. (2015). Hyperbolic language and its relation to metaphor and irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 79, 7992.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2000). On necessary conditions for verbal irony comprehension. Pragmatics & Cognition, 8, 277324.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2002). Contrast and assimilation in verbal irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 111142.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & O'Brien, J. (2000). Contrast of kind versus contrast of magnitude: the pragmatic accomplishments of irony and hyperbole. Discourse Processes, 30(2), 179199.Google Scholar
Demorest, A., Meyer, C., Phelps, E., Gardner, H., & Winner, E. (1984). Words speak louder than actions: understanding deliberately false remarks. Child Development, 55, 15271534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demorest, A., Silberstein, L., Gardner, H., & Winner, E. (1983). Telling it as it isn't: children's understanding of figurative language. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 121134.Google Scholar
Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79, 126138.Google Scholar
Filippova, E., & Astington, J. (2010). Children's understanding of social-cognitive and social-communicative aspects of discourse irony. Child Development, 81(3), 913928.Google Scholar
Friend, M. (2000). Developmental changes in sensitivity to vocal paralanguage. Developmental Science, 3, 148162.Google Scholar
Friend, M., & Bryant, J. B. (2000). A developmental lexical bias in the interpretation of discrepant messages. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 342369.Google ScholarPubMed
Gerrig, R. J., & Goldvarg, Y. (2000). Additive effects in the perception of sarcasm: situational disparity and echoic mention. Metaphor & Symbol, 15, 197208.Google Scholar
Hancock, J. T., Dunham, P. J., & Purdy, K. (2000). Children's comprehension of critical and complimentary forms of verbal irony. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 227248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, M., & Pexman, P. M. (2003). Children's perceptions of the social functions of verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 36, 147165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivanko, S. L., & Pexman, P. M. (2003). Context incongruity and irony processing. Discourse Processes, 35, 241279.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434446.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Roberts, R. M. (1995). Two cues for verbal irony: hyperbole and the ironic tone of voice. Metaphor & Symbolic Activity, 10, 2131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laval, V., & Bert-Erboul, A. (2005). French-speaking children's understanding of sarcasm: the role of intonation and context. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 610620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lightfoot, C., & Bullock, M. (1990). Interpreting contradictory communications: age and context effects. Developmental Psychology, 26, 830836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., Harris, L., & Patriquin, M. (1993). Lexical and prosodic cues in the comprehension of relative certainty. Journal of Child Language, 20, 153167.Google Scholar
Morton, J. B., & Trehub, S. E. (2001). Children's understanding of emotion in speech. Child Development, 72, 834843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Reilly, K., Peterson, C. C., & Wellman, H. M. (2014). Sarcasm and advanced theory of mind understanding in children and adults with prelingual deafness. Developmental Psychology, 50(7), 18621877.Google Scholar
Partington, A. (2007). Irony and reversal of evaluation. Journal of Pragmatics, 39(9), 15471569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: advancing theory-of-mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger syndrome. Child Development, 83(2), 469485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pexman, P. M., & Glenwright, M. (2007). How do typically developing children grasp the meaning of verbal irony? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 178196.Google Scholar
Pexman, P. M., Glenwright, M., Krol, A., & James, T. (2005). An acquired taste: children's perceptions of humor and teasing in verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 40, 259288.Google Scholar
Recchia, H. E., Howe, N., Ross, H. S., & Alexander, S. (2010). Children's understanding and production of verbal irony in family conversations. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(2), 255274.Google Scholar
Utsumi, A. (2000). Verbal irony as implicit display of ironic environment: distinguishing ironic utterances from nonirony. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 17771806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varga, D. (2000). Hyperbole and humor in children's language play. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 14(2), 142151.Google Scholar
Waxer, M., & Morton, J. B. (2011). Children's judgments of emotion from conflicting cues in speech: why 6-year-olds are so inflexible. Child Development, 82(5), 16481660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, D. (2013). Irony comprehension: a developmental perspective. Journal of Pragmatics, 59, Part A , 4056.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winner, E., Windmueller, G., Rosenblatt, E., Bosco, L., Best, E., & Gardner, H. (1987). Making sense of literal and nonliteral falsehood. Metaphor & Symbolic Activity, 2, 1332.Google Scholar