Skip to main content
Log in

Economic Preferences or Attitude Expressions?: An Analysis of Dollar Responses to Public Issues

  • Published:
Journal of Risk and Uncertainty Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Participants in contingent valuation surveys and jurors setting punitive damages in civil trials provide answers denominated in dollars. These answers are better understood as expressions of attitudes than as indications of economic preferences. Well-established characteristics of attitudes and of the core process of affective valuation explain several robust features of dollar responses: high correlations with other measures of attractiveness or aversiveness, insensitivity to scope, preference reversals, and the high variability of dollar responses relative to other measures of the same attitude.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, Norman. (1996). A Functional Theory of Cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arrow, Kenneth. (1982). “Risk Perception in Psychology and Economics,” Economic Inquiry 20, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, Jonathan and Joshua Greene. (1996). “Determinants of Insensitivity to Quantity in Valuation of Public Goods: Contribution, Warm Glow, Budget Constraints, Availability, and Prominence,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 2, 107–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, Lawrence. (1992). Cognitive Psychology: An Overview for Cognitive Scientists. NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, Larry M. (1998). “Democracy With Attitudes,” Paper presented at the annual meeting of American Political Science Association, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carson, Richard, Michael Hanemann, Raymond Kopp, John Krosnick, Robert Mitchell, Stanley Presser, Paul Ruud, and V. Kerry Smith. (1994). Prospective Interim Lost Use Value Due to DDT and PCB Contamination in the Southern California Bight. La Jolla, CA: Natural Resource Damage Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carson, Richard and Robert Mitchell. (1993). “The Issue of Scope in Contingent Valuation Studies,” American Journal of Agricultural Economics 75, 1263–1267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carson, Richard and Robert Mitchell. (1995). “Sequencing and Nesting in Contingent Valuation Surveys,” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 28, 155–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, Gretchen, and Brian Bornstein. (1996). “The More You Ask for the More You Get: Anchoring in Personal Injury Verdicts,” Applied Cognitive Psychology 10, 519–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, Ronald, Glenn Harrison, and Elizabeth Rutstrom. (1995). “Homegrown Values and Hypothetical Surveys: Is the Dichotomous Choice Approach Incentive-Compatible?” American Economic Review 85, 260–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeKay, Michael and Gary McClelland. (1996). “Probability and Utility Components of Endangered Species Preservation Programs,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 2, 60–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desvousges, William, F. Reed Johnson, Richard Dunford, Kevin Boyle, Sarah Hudson, and K. Nicole Wilson. (1992). Measuring Non-Use Damages Using Contingent Valuation: An Experimental Evaluation of Accuracy. Research Triangle Institute Monograph 92–1.

  • Diamond, Peter. (1996). “Testing the Internal Consistency of Contingent Valuation Surveys.” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 30, 337–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, Peter, John Hausman, Gregory Leonard, and Michael Denning. (1993). “Does Contingent Valuation Measure Preferences? Experimental Evidence.” In J. A. Hausman ed., Contingent Valuation: A Critical Assessment. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, Peter and John Hausman. (1994). “Contingent Valuation: Is Some Number Better Than No Number?” Journal of Economic Perspectives 8, 45–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, Alice and Shelley Chaiken. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, Alice and Shelley Chaiken. (1996). “Attitude Structure and Function.” In Gilbert, D., S. Fiske, and G. Lindzey eds., The Handbook of Social Psychology, 4th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, Russell, David Sanbonmatsu, Martha Powell, and Frank Kardes. (1986). “On the Automatic Activation of Attitudes.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50, 229–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff, Baruch. (1991). “Value Elicitation: Is There Anything in There?” American Psychologist 46, 835–847.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foster, Vivien, Ian J. Bateman, and David Harley. (1997). “Real and Hypothetical Willingness to Pay for Environmental Preservation: A Non-Experimental Comparison,” Journal of Agricultural Economics 48, 123–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, Shane and Baruch Fischhoff. (1998). “Scope In Sensitivity in Elicited Valuations,” Risk Decision and Policy 3, 109–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, Donald, Karen Jacowitz, Daniel Kahneman, and Daniel McFadden. (1998). “Referendum Contingent Valuation, Anchoring, and Willingness to Pay for Public Goods.” Resource and Energy Economics 20, 85–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, Dale and Amos Tversky. (1992). “The Weighing of Evidence and the Determinants of Confidence,” Cognitive Psychology 24, 411–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanemann, Michael. (1994). “Valuing the Environment Through Contingent Valuation,” Journal of Economic Perspectives 8, 19–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hastie, Reid, David Schkade, and John Payne. (1999). “Juror Judgments in Civil Cases: Effects of Plaintiff's Request and Plaintiff's Identity on Punitive Damage Awards.” Law and Human Behavior 23, 445–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoehn, John and Alan Randall. (1987). “Too Many Proposals Pass the Benefit Cost Test,” American Economic Review 79, 544–551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, Chris. (1996). “The Evaluability Principle: An Explanation for Preference Reversals between Joint and Separate Evaluations of Alternatives,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 67, 247–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacowitz, Karen and Daniel Kahneman. (1995). “Measures of Anchoring in Estimation Tasks,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21, 1161–1166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones-Lee, Michael, Graham Loomes, and P. Philips. (1995). “Valuing the Prevention of Non-Fatal Road Injuries: Contingent Valuation vs. Standard Gambles,” Oxford Economic Papers 47, 676–695.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel. (1986). “Valuing Environmental Goods: An Assessment of the Contingent Valua-tion Method.” In R. Cummings, D. Brookshire, and W. Schulze eds. Valuing Environmental Goods: An Assessment of the Contingent Valuation Method. Totowa, NJ.

  • Kahneman, Daniel. (1995). “Extension Neglect and Violations of Monotonicity in Judgment and Preference: Three Examples,” Bartlett Lecture to the Experimental Psychology Society UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel, Barbara Fredrickson, Charles Schreiber, and Don Redelmeier. (1993). “When More Pain Is Preferred to Less,” Psychological Science 4, 401–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel and Jack Knetsch. (1992). “Valuing Public Goods: The Purchase of Moral Satisfaction,” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 22, 57–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel, Jack Knetsch, and Richard Thaler. (1991). “The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion, and Status Quo Bias,” Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5, 193–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel and Dale Miller. (1986). “Norm Theory: Comparing Reality to Its Alternatives,” Psychological Review 93, 136–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel and Ilana Ritov. (1994). “Determinants of Stated Willingness to Pay for Public Goods: A Study in the Headline Method,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 9, 5–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel, David Schkade, Elana Ritov and Cass Sunstein. (1999). “Reversals of judgement; The effect of cross-category comparisons intendedly absolute scales,” manuscript order review.

  • Kahneman, Daniel, David Schkade, and Cass Sunstein. (1998). “Shared Outrage and Erratic Awards: The Psychology of Punitive Damages,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 16, 49–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel and Amos Tversky. (1972). “Subjective Probability: A Judgment of Representativeness.” Cognitive Psychology 3, 430–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel and Amos Tversky. (1973). “On the Psychology of Prediction.” Psychological Review 80, 237–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel and Amos Tversky. (1979). “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk,” Econometrica 47, 263–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel, Peter Wakker, and Rakesh Sarin. (1997). “Back to Bentham? Explorations of Experienced Utility,” Quarterly Journal of Economics 112, 375–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemp, Michael and Christopher Maxwell. (1993). “Exploring a Budget Context for Contingent Valua-tion.” In Hausman ed., Contingent Valuation: A Critical Assessment. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koehler, Jonathan. (1996). “The Base-Rate Fallacy Reconsidered: Descriptive, Normative, and Methodological Challenges.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19, 1–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levav, Jonathan. (1996). “Questioning Contingent Valuation: Maximality and Violations of Monotonicity in Willingness-to-Pay for Public Goods,” Unpublished undergraduate thesis, Princeton University.

  • Lodge, Milton. (1981). “Magnitude Scaling: Quantitative Measurement of Opinions.” InJ. Sullivan ed., Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, Vol. 25. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loomis, John, Armando Gonzalez-Caban, and Robin Gregory. (1994). “Do Reminders of Substitutes and Budget Constraints Influence Contingent Valuation Estimates?” Land Economics 70, 499–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, Daniel. (1994). “Contingent Valuation and Social Choice,” American Journal of Agricultural Economics 76, 689–708.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, Daniel. (1999). “Rationality for Economists,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 19, 73–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, Daniel and Gregory Leonard. (1993). “Issues in the Contingent Valuation of Environmental Goods: Methodologies for Data Collection and Analysis.” In Hausman ed., Contingent Valuation: A Critical Assessment. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, Robert and Richard Carson. (1989). Using Surveys to Value Public Goods. Washington, DC: Resources for the Future.

  • Neill, Helen. (1995). “The Context for Substitutes in CVM Studies: Some Empirical Observations,” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 29, 393–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, Carol. (1995). “Does Willingness-to-Pay Reflect the Purchase of Moral Satisfaction? A Reconsideration of Kahneman and Knetsch,” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 28, 126–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • NOAA panel report. U.S. Department of Commerce, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administra-tion. (1993). “Natural Resource Damage Assessments under the Oil Pollution Act of 1990,” Federal Register 58, 4601–4614.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novemsky, Nathan and Shirit Kronzon. (1999). “How Are Base-Rates Used, When They Are Used: A Comparison of Bayesian and Additive Models of Base-Rate Use,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 12, 55–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, Charles, George Suci, and Percy Tannenbaum. (1957). The Measurement of Meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John, James Bettman, and Eric Johnson. (1992). “Behavioral Decision Research: A Constructive Processing Perspective.” Annual Review of Psychology 43, 87–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John W., James R. Bettman, and David A. Schkade. (1999). “Measuring Constructed Preferences: Towards a Building Code.” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 19, 243–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John, David Schkade, William Desvousges, and Chris Aultman. (1999). “Valuation of Multiple Environmental Programs: A Psychological Analysis,” Unpublished manuscript, Duke University.

  • Pratto, F. (1994). “Consciousness and Automatic Evaluation.” In Paula M. Niedenthal and Shinobu Kitayama Eds., The Heart's Eye: Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention. Academic Press, Inc, San Diego, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quattrone, George and Amos Tversky. (1984). “Causal Versus Diagnostic Contingencies: On Self-Deception and the Voter's Illusion,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46, 237–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Randall, Alan and John Hoehn. (1996). “Embedding in Market Demand Systems,” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 30, 369–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritov, Ilana, Jonathan Baron, and John Hershey. (1993). “Framing Effects in the Evaluation of Multiple Risk Reduction,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 6, 145–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritov, Ilana. (1996). “Anchoring in a Simulated Competitive Market Negotiation,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 67, 16–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, Eleanor and Barbara Lloyd. (1978). Cognition and Categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rottenstreich, Yuval and Amos Tversky. (1997). “Unpacking, Repacking, and Anchoring: Advances in Support Theory,” Psychological Review, 104, 406–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, Murray, Jack Knetsch, and Thomas Brown. (1998). “Assessing Environmental Losses: Judgments of Importance and Damage Schedules,” Harvard Environmental Law Review, 22, 51–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreiber, Charles and Daniel Kahneman. (2000). “Beyond the Peak and End Hypothesis: Exploring the Relation between Real-Time Pleasure and Retrospective Evaluations,” Journal of Experimental Psychology.

  • Seip, Kalle and Jon Strand. (1992). “Willingness to Pay for Environmental Goods in Norway: A Contingent Valuation Study with Real Payment,” Environmental and Resource Economics 2, 91–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, Paul. (1995). “The Construction of Preference,” American Psychologist, 50, 364–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, V. Kerry. (1992). “Arbitrary Values, Good Causes, and Premature Verdicts,” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 22, 71–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, Fritz and Thomas Mussweiler. (1997). “Explaining the Enigmatic Anchoring Effect: Mechanisms of Selective Accessibility,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73, 437–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, Stanley S. (1975). Psychophysics. Introduction to Its Perceptual, Neural, and Social Prospects. Wiley: NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sunstein, Cass, Daniel Kahneman, and David Schkade. (1998). “Assessing Punitive Damages,” Yale Law Journal 107, 2071–2153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tesser, Abraham and Leonard Martin. (1996). “The Psychology of Evaluation.” In E. T. Higgins, and A. Kruglanski eds., Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, pp. 400–432. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, Richard. (1992). The Winner's Curse: Paradoxes and Anomalies of Economic Life. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman. (1971). “Belief in the Law of Small Numbers,” Psychological Bulletin 76, 105–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman. (1982). “Evidential Impact of Base Rates.” In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, and A. Tversky eds., Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman. (1983). “Extensional vs. Intuitive Reasoning: The Conjunction Fallacy in Probability Judgment,” Psychological Review 90, 293–3l5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman. (1986). “Rational Choice and the Framing of Decisions,” Journal of Business 59, 251–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, Amos and Richard Thaler. (1992). “Preference Reversals.” In R. H. Thaler ed., The Winner's Curse: Paradoxes of Economic Life. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varey, Carol and Daniel Kahneman. (1992). “Experiences Extended Across Time: Evaluation of Moments and Episodes,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 5, 169–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varian, Hal. (1984). Microeconomic Analysis. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, Thomas, Christopher Houston, Kathryn Etling, and Nancy Brekke. (1996). “A New Look at Anchoring Effects: Basic Anchoring and Its Antecedents,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 125, 387–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaller, John. (1992). The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kahneman, D., Ritov, I. & Schkade, D. Economic Preferences or Attitude Expressions?: An Analysis of Dollar Responses to Public Issues. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 19, 203–235 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007835629236

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007835629236

Navigation